Rachel Bishop Seminar Preparation # 8 Reading Educational Research October 24, 2006 “This is one of the few places in the book where Bracey cannot resist making a political point. He suggests that making NAEP the definition of proficiency for the nation could be used to make public schools look bad so our education system could be privatized by right-wing zealots.” Pg ix This quote is from the foreword to the book. It is not written by the author but comes from a review of the book in the Washington Post. In the section of the book that we have read, Bracey has not yet made this point. But it does not surprise me that it is coming. It seems like it is in fact often that the author “cannot resist making a political point.” ““Most of the time, most people simply accept statistics without question.” One of the reasons we do so is that we are often not in the position to do otherwise. We lack the information to challenge the figures.” Pg xiii Certainly having the knowledge to be critical of the information that one is inundated with is important in contemporary society. As teachers one of our roles is to help give our students the tools to be critical questioners. “Statistics, the language of data, are human constructions and must be interpreted by other humans for the numbers to have meaning.” Pg 2 Because statistics are interpreted they can be interpreted in different ways. As Bracey suggests they can be twisted or they can be presented in more accurate ways. But still they must be interpreted. “Yes, personal experiences have two qualities. They are wholly compelling to the person who has them, and they are often wrong. They make a terrible base from which to generalize. That is why we invented something that doesn’t depend solely on personal experiences. It’s called science.” Pg 8 Personal experience and anecdotal evidence are very persuasive. Research shows that they tend to be more persuasive then data driven arguments. This can lead to inaccurate conclusions. But perhaps with the tools to understand and be critical of statistical data, this will be less of a problem. “A set of test scores won’t tell you why groups differ, but it can point to group differences that might need exploring. NCLB, for example, forces schools to look at differences among groups. That might be the only good feature, in my opinion: schools cannot obscure the failure of some groups to achieve as high as others by pointing to high schoolwide averages or ignoring subgroups.” Pg 10 Here Bracey starts to get political. This is the first of many instances. This type of comment seems very out of place in a book that’s premise is to teach the skills to be critical consumers of statistical information. On one hand the author is trying to convince us to be critical and use our understanding and judgment. On the other hand he is clearly trying to convince the reader of his viewpoint on educational research and testing. “... the fact that that error made it all the way to publication reveals something related to a principle of data interpretation, something I call the Neurotic Need to Believe the Worst. If the numbers, any numbers, show public education in a bad light, the find much easier acceptance than numbers indicating something good or something improved.” Pg 22 Is it true that we have a “neurotic need to believe the worst,” or is it that the media has a “neurotic need to portray the worst.” Because really bad news is more compelling. “An analysis by the Washington Post revealed that teachers would get about enough to buy a new TV, while someone making $1,000,000 a year would get a tax break about twice as large as the average teacher’s annual salary.” Pg 43 Isn’t this just the type of twisting of statistics to be salacious and persuasive that Bracey is so critical of? “Of course, the stated goal of NCLB is to wipe out the impact of those family and community variables—to close the achievement gap between rich and poor, majority and minority. It won’t happen, if only because children, from birth to age eighteen, spend only 9 percent of their lives in school.” Pg 51 This is an important thing to understand. School is only one factor that contributes to a students achievement. As pointed out earlier by Spring in the American School schools have long been expected to solve social and economic problems. But it is not necessarily true that they can do so. In sum, passing rates tell you nothing about what is happening to any achievement gap. Bet it remains quite likely that those who fail the exit exam, or fail to contribute to the school’s making adequate yearly progress under NCLB, will spend more time learning the tasks that are on the test while those who pass go on to learn new material. The achievement gap has increased. Pg 57 This is a perspective on the achievement gap that I have not heard before. If students are given extra help then the achievement gap widens because they are studying the same things in order to pass tests while the high achieving students move on to harder subjects. Is the achievement gap inevitable then? Perhaps schools can not solve the problem by themselves and more systematic changes will be necessary so that students can enter school in a more equal place. In some despairing moments, I am tempted to found the National Association for the Abolition of Correlation Coefficients. Although the correlation coefficient can be a useful statistic, it causes a great deal of mischief. It creates havoc because people so often misinterpret it. I think that we tend to misinterpret the correlation coefficient because our brains are hardwired to do so. Pg 74 Something about a correlations makes it so easy to just to a casual relationship. Its as if this is the only way we can understand what a correlation is. The idea that our brains are hardwired to see patterns and jump to these type of conclusions is one that is consistent with what I have learned about the brain previously. I would have liked to see a reference for this comment though.