Categorization of Alternative Conceptions in Electricity and Magnetism Categorization of Alternative Conceptions in Electricity and Magnetism: the case of Ethiopian Undergraduate Students Abstract The purpose of this study was to categorize of 35 Ethiopian undergraduate physics students’ alternative conceptions in electric potential and energy concepts. A descriptive qualitative research design was used. To categorize the students’ alternative conceptions, four independently homogeneous ability focus groups were formed. A five stage thematic (categorical) framework analysis – familiarization, identifying a thematic framework, coding, charting and interpretation – was made to analyze data of the focus group discussions. The categories of alternative conceptions were based on the students’ epistemological and ontological descriptions of the concepts investigated. Consequently, the following categories were diagnosed: naive physics, lateral alternative conceptions, ontological alternative conceptions, Ohm’s p-primes, mixed conceptions and loose ideas. The extensivenesses of the alternative conceptions from the epistemological and ontological perspectives were comparable and considerable. In the categories, the alternative conceptions were less frequently depicted. Within and across the categories, the alternative conceptions were inconsistently revealed. Category wise, the naïve physics and lateral alternative conceptions were more extensive than the others. In general, it was concluded that the categories have commonly characteristics of diversified distributions of alternative conceptions and multiple alternative conceptions of specific concepts within and across the categories. Finally, instructional and theoretical implications were forwarded. Keywords Alternative conception. Categories of alternative conceptions. Conceptual change. Electricity and Magnetism. Ethiopia. Framework thematic analysis. 1 Introduction Alternative conceptions1 are known as pre-instructional conceptions that are often inconsistent with scientific concepts to be taught. In literature, they are also described as theorylike student ideas (Ioannides & Vosniadou 2002), misclassification of concepts within or across ontological categories (Chi 2008; Chi & Roscoe 2002; Chi & Slotta 1993) or fragmented “pieces” of knowledge (diSessa 1993; diSessa et al. 2004). It is noticed that these distinctions between alternative conceptions are resulted from different perspectives of conceptual change2 researches. In the cognitive domain of conceptual change researches, these perspectives are the epistemological and ontological views of students’ descriptions of concepts (Duit & Treagust 2003). In other words, the researches in epistemological perspective of conceptual change (e.g. Duschi & Gitomer 1991, Posner et al. 1982) view alternative conceptions as consistent theorylike and/or inconsistent piece-like ideas. However, researches from the ontological perspective of conceptual change view alternative conceptions as incorrect ontological categorization of concepts (Chi Slotta & Deleeuw 1994; Chi 2008). Nevertheless, so far alternative conceptions researches in the epistemological perspectives of conceptual change are dominated (Duit & Treagust 2003) and studies on students’ alternative conceptions from inclusive epistemological 1 The term alternative conception is used as it is preferred by many constructivist perspective researchers in science and physics education. The term conceptual change is viewed as a learning process in which students’ alternative conceptions transform into the intended scientific conceptions (Vosniadou 2007). 2 1 Categorization of Alternative Conceptions in Electricity and Magnetism and ontological perspectives of conceptual change are limited. The highlights of alternative conceptions types in epistemological and ontological views are described in the paragraphs of section 1.2. Researchers in cognitive science and science/physics education from the two perspectives of conceptual change agree on the influence of prior knowledge in the way new science concepts are learned. However, these researchers disagree on how alternative conceptions are organized and developed in the learning process (diSessa et al. 2004; Vosniadou 2002). Their dispute is based on whether the students’ alternative conceptions form a relatively coherent model or naïve theory (Ioannides & Vosniadou 2002; Linder 1993; Vosniadou, 2002; Vosniadou & Brewer 1992) that is persistent against change or whether they are simply unstructured “pieces of knowledge” (diSessa 1993; diSessa et al. 2004) that need to be corrected. The former one is called the coherence (McCloskey 1983) of students’ alternative conceptions; while the later is said to be the fragmentation (diSessa 1993) of students’ alternative conceptions. These disputations of the researchers’ ideas on coherence versus fragmentation of students’ alternative conceptions have influences on the way a syllabus is designed and a conceptual learning is supported. The different perspectives on students’ alternative conceptions put science teachers in general and physics teachers in particular in indecisive situations with regards to change of alternative conceptions of their students. The only identification of students’ alternative conceptions in a given domain of science, as have been studied for the last 30 years, is believed to be inadequate for the design of learning supportive approaches for conceptual change. The inadequacy of the latter process is because of the different disputable perspectives on students’ alternative conceptions and conceptual change processes. In addition, earlier researches (e.g. Chi & Roscoe 2002) have raised blurred ideas about students’ conceptions. As they showed, alternative conceptions have different meanings due to the different ways they are formed. This complexity of alternative conceptions have also posed problem to students’ conceptual change because they imply different ways of conceptual change. Since the last three decades, different types of conceptual changes have been distinguished. These distinctions are based on different perspectives of researchers in the area. Accordingly, from the epistemological perspective, conceptual change is viewed as assimilation and accommodation of concepts. “Assimilation” (Posner et al. 1984) or “conceptual capture” (Hewson 1981) of concepts refers to the use of existing conceptions to deal with new (scientific) conceptions while “accommodation” (Posner et al. 1984) or “conceptual exchange” (Hewson Hewson 1981) involves replacing the learner’s persistent conceptions. These two forms of conceptual change, assimilation and accommodation, respectively, are also named as “weak restructuring” and “radical restructuring” (Carey 1985), because the former involves modification or incorporation of the existing conceptions while the latter involves the rejection of the existing conception and the acceptance of a new conception. Analogously, from ontological perspective, conceptual change is distinguished as “tree jumping” and “branch switching” (Chi 2008; Thagard 1990) to distinguish the conceptual restructuring from incorrect to correct ontological categories. In this perspective, branch switching is meant for rearrangement of concepts within an ontological category, while tree jumping is meant for restructuring of concepts in different categories. In this paper, a categorization of alternative conceptions is made by viewing students’ descriptions of concepts from epistemological and ontological perspectives of conceptual change. This categorization of alternative conceptions is believed to reduce the complexity of 2 Categorization of Alternative Conceptions in Electricity and Magnetism conceptual change process and be more useful than the only identification of the alternative conceptions themselves, because the type of alternative conception categorized would indicate the type of conceptual change needed. 1.1 Consistent/ Inconsistent Students’ Alternative Conceptions This section is not to dispute between the consistent or inconsistent perspectives regarding students’ alternative conceptions, but to review on some earlier empirical evidence. Thus, it is assumed that the consistency or inconsistency of students’ alternative conceptions occur on the basis of the empirical evidence shown in each perspective. From the consistent perspective of alternative conceptions, for example, the empirical studies done in the science areas like mental models of the earth (Vosniadou 1992), five alternative mental models were identified. These students’ naïve models about the shape of the earth were: the rectangular earth, the disc earth, the dual earth, the hallow earth and the flattened sphere. Another example was the study of motion of macroscopic objects in introductory mechanics, like motion implies force (Celment 1982) consistent students’ model. Likewise, in DC circuits, there are documented students’ consistent alternative conceptions, such as the unipolar or sink model (Fredette & Lochhead 1980; Osborne 1981), the clashing or twocomponent model (Osborne 1983) and the current consumption or sequence (attenuation) model (Osborne 1983; Shipstone 1984). In general, the common attribute regarding studies of alternative conceptions in mechanics and DC circuits is that they are highly related to students’ daily experiences. As a result, students are confident in their alternative conceptions (Planinic et al. 2006) because the concepts in these two areas of physics are closest to their immediate everyday experience and are easily observable. From the inconsistent perspective of alternative conceptions, students are inexperienced in the topic under study or they are just beginning to learn new concepts and their responses are strongly context dependent (Bao & Redish 2006). In other words, as diSessa (1993) argued, students are assumed to have their knowledge “in pieces” and different bits of knowledge tend to be weakly connected. In addition, studies (Tao & Gunstone 1999; Li et al. 2006) showed that students’ alternative conceptions in science are idiosyncratic and they have characteristics vacillating from one context to another. That is, different contexts of a concept can easily indicate different responses. Accordingly, the concepts in electricity and magnetism (EM) are complex with abstract formal relations that are significantly more difficult to understand than classical mechanics (Chabay & Sherwood 2006). For example, concepts like electric potential, potential difference and fields are unfamiliar to students’ everyday experiences. In particular, these mentioned concepts are unfamiliar to several students in developing countries like Ethiopia. Such students begin to learn most of the EM concepts in formal classroom learning situations. As a result, unlike in Newtonian mechanics and DC circuits, it is supposed that students have inconsistency on their alternative conceptions of EM concepts. Therefore, in general, it is believed that the nature of the concepts under investigation is a decisive factor for the consistent or incontinent perspectives regarding the structures of students’ conceptions. In addition, studies (Finkelstein 2005; Planinic 2006) have reported low scores of several students in conceptual diagnostic tests on EM concepts. For example, studies on the assessment of the difficulties in the concepts of EM have shown that the difficulties have similar trends across countries and universities (Maloney et al. 2001; Planinic 2006; Saglam & Millar 2006). This shows that students have problems with the concepts of EM globally and may cause 3 Categorization of Alternative Conceptions in Electricity and Magnetism problems in physics learning. However, so far there are no studies conducted on categorizing students’ alternative conceptions in EM concepts. 1.2 Categories of Students’ Alternative Conceptions Categories of alternative conceptions are meant for classification of the students’ alternative conceptions that are depicted on the basis of the nature and structure of the students’ descriptions of a given concept. Thus four categories of students’ alternative conceptions were reviewed from different related literature of cognitive science and science education based on the distinctions noticed in the students’ descriptions and the conceptual change perspectives followed by the researchers. These categories are naïve physics (diSessa 1982; McCloskey 1983), ontological alternative conception (Chi 2008; Chi & Slotta 1993), lateral alternative conception (Chi 2008; Chi & Slotta 1993) and the Ohm’s p-prim (diSessa 1993). The distinctions among these categories are highlighted in the subsequent paragraphs. Naïve physics: In this context, naïve physics is an intuitive physics (diSessa 1982; McCloskey 1983) defined as a simplified and less organized students’ theoretical view of a concept or knowledge experientially acquired. For example, Aristotelian ‘force implies velocity’ is naïve physics. Scientifically, Newtonian mechanics states that force implies acceleration but no force implies a zero velocity or a constant none zero velocity. Accordingly, naïve physics have some valid element of a concept or a principle of the physical world. In other words, it represents insufficient ideas or particular cases of a concept or a principle. In the area of electricity, for example, students’ conception of a charge moves in the direction of an electric field is naive physics. This is so because the conception is valid at a special situation when a point positive charge is released in an electric field but it does not apply for a negative charge or a charged particle projected with some angle to the direction of an electric field. Phenomenological primitives (p-primes): According to diSessa (1993), Phenomenological primitives (p-prims) are intuitive or spontaneous physics which are made up of smaller and more fragmented cognitive structures. The p-primes can have different forms depending on how students respond to the concepts to be investigated. These forms are actuating agency, dying away, resistance and interference, and Ohm's p-prim (diSessa 1993; Hammer 1996). In general, the first three forms of the p-primes can be related to students’ conceptions of relating proximity to intensity, i.e. closer means stronger (Hammer 1996). For example, to a question of why it is hotter in the summer than in the winter; students’ response could be that the earth is closer to the sun in the summer. Among the different forms of the p-primes described by diSessa (1993), the Ohm’s p-prim, which means more effort creates more result, is relevant to this paper. The Ohm’s p-prim is one of the phenomenological primitives (diSessa 1993) that may be reflected by physics students who consider Ohm’s law as a fundamental law of EM. In Ohm’s law, the electric potential is the agent that creates an electric current against a resistance in an electric circuit. The higher the effect (electric potential) is the higher the result (current) and the higher the resistance the less the current will be in a circuit. Thus, students’ conceptions in the other parts of EM concepts are supposed to be influenced by the Ohm’s law of electric circuits. For example, in the Columbic interaction of two different charges, students may consider an exertion of a larger force on a larger charge (Maloney et al. 2001; Leppävirta 2012). Here, the students simply related larger charge to the larger force without understanding of action and reaction force in a system of the two charges. Ontological alternative conception: According to Chi (2008), categorization is a process of identifying or assigning a concept to a category to which it belongs and is therefore assumed 4 Categorization of Alternative Conceptions in Electricity and Magnetism as an important process in physics learning. Most of the concepts in physics have attributes of being classified into categories based on their nature, properties and descriptions. In an ontological categorization of science concepts, matter and process are two main ontological categories or representations (Chi 2008; Chi & Slotta 1993). For example, several teachers use tap water analogy in teaching their students about the electric current concept. However, at the end of the lesson some of the students may consider electric current as a fluid. It is known that electric current is a process in relation to charge flow; while water is a substance. This means, in such instruction, some students may conceive current as a fluid. It is such students’ incorrect categorization of concepts which in this paper is called ontological alternative conception. Lateral alternative conception: Students may misclassify concepts within an ontological category (Chi 2008). It means that a concept in a hierarchy of concepts could be incorrectly categorized into different branches. For example, students may consider electric potential as a force, i.e. they incorrectly label electric potential under some properties of force. Though electric potential and Columbic force can share some properties and they are found in the same ontological category; laterally, they belong to different sub categories. It is such a conception which in this paper is called lateral alternative conception. In general, earlier studies on alternative conceptions of science concepts have used either epistemological or ontological perspectives of conceptual change. As a result, the naive physics and p-primes were based on epistemological perspective, while ontological and lateral conceptions were viewed from ontological perspective. However, this study used inclusive epistemological and ontological perspectives to identify and then categorize students’ descriptions of electric potential and energy (EPE) concepts. Therefore, it is supposed that the categories of alternative conceptions that are mentioned above, and some others, can exist in the EPE concepts. Along this, the extensivenesses of the alternative conceptions in the categories as well as in the perspectives are studied. In addition, in this categorization of alternative conceptions it is supposed that the coherence or fragmentation of the students’ alternative conceptions is observed in and across the categories to be arisen. This coherence versus fragmentation involves important theoretical and practical implications (Clark et al. 2011). Theoretically, it contributes to the current emerging literature of the disputable two paradigms (coherence versus fragmentation) (Clark et al. 2011; diSessa et al. 2004; Elby 2010) of the students’ alternative conceptions. Empirically, in line with Clark et al. (2011) and Leppävirta (2012) teachers’ and curriculum experts’ awareness about coherence or fragmentation of students’ alternative conceptions is believed to facilitate the design of conceptual change learning activities and curriculum to support students’ conceptual learning processes. Research is, therefore, needed in the conceptual change process in which students correct their alternative conceptions. Hence, a study is done on the categorization of students’ alternative conceptions based on inclusive perspectives of conceptual change on students’ descriptions of EM concepts. 1.3 Theoretical Framework Conceptual change is viewed as a learning process in which students’ alternative conceptions transform into the intended scientific conceptions (Vosniadou 2007). This paper used the theory as a framework because it requires primarily the identification of existing students’ conceptions (Vosniadou & Brewer 1987). The theory is based on the constructivist theory of learning (Von Glaserfeld & Steffe 1991; Driver et al. 1994) that describes about knowledge and learning. In this view, knowledge is not the facts which are memorized and repeated temporarily, but it is developmental, internally constructed and socially mediated 5 Categorization of Alternative Conceptions in Electricity and Magnetism (Fosnot 1996). Along this, the constructivist theory of learning focuses on the process of knowledge construction by individuals and its mediation by groups. In the constructivist theory of learning, there is a claim of epistemological and ontological perspectives that refers knowledge to individual experience rather than to the world and it is constituted by individual conceptual structures (Vosniadou & Brewer 1987). In this case, conceptual structures constitute knowledge when individuals regard them as viable in relationship to their experience. In this view, alternative conceptions refer to students’ personal understanding of the concepts from their epistemological and ontological perspectives (Treagust & Duit 2008). In other words, from an epistemological perspective, students’ alternative conceptions can be viewed based on how they describe the conception being investigated; while from an ontological perspective students’ alternative conceptions are viewed based on how they view the nature of the conception being investigated. Therefore, this paper follows a pragmatic inclusive conceptual change perspective that involves students’ epistemological and ontological forms of their alternative conceptions. 1.4 Context Currently, in Ethiopia, students enroll in higher institutions after successful completion of two years of preparatory school education (grades 11 & 12). The enrollment criteria to all the universities of the country, Ambo University being one of them, are the same and done nationally by the Ministry of Education based on the students’ choices, their academic performances and the programs offered at the universities. Electricity and magnetism is a domain of physics selected for this study. In Ethiopia, the concepts of EM are taught from junior secondary school to university. In school, the concepts are incorporated in the subject general physics and are taught in grades 8, 10 & 12. At university level, independent courses are usually offered. Consequently, the EM as a course is offered to the first year of physics and other sciences and engineering undergraduate students. The course is outlined into 11 chapters according to the Harmonized National Curriculum for BSc degree program in Physics (MOE 2009). Its outline is comparable with the conceptual areas of EM that Maloney et al. (2001) surveyed with their conceptual survey test, but with the exclusion of the extensively researched part of DC circuits. Along this, Planinic (2006) classified the concepts of EM into six broad units (conceptual areas) by maintaining the conceptual coherence of the concepts. These conceptual areas are electric charge and force, electric field and force, electric potential and energy, magnetic field ad force, electromagnetic induction and Newton’s laws in an electromagnetic context. However, this paper is limited to only one of the above mentioned conceptual areas, namely, the electric potential and energy (EPE), for the following three reasons: first, the conceptual area is one of the most difficult parts of the EM and challenge students across different countries (Planinic 2006; Saglam & Millar 2006). Second, students’ alternative conceptions in this area are not yet well addressed using appropriate supportive approaches that could develop conceptual change. This could be because the alternative conceptions in this area are not well studied compared to that of Mechanics and DC electric circuits. Third, there are recommendations (Chabay & Sherwood 2006; Ding et al. 2006; Maloney et al. 2001; Planinic 2006) for the need to undertake in-depth studies by taking a few conceptual areas of EM using qualitative methods. However, research has not been undertaken on categorization of students' alternative conceptions in one or some parts of the EM concepts. 6 Categorization of Alternative Conceptions in Electricity and Magnetism 1.5 The Problem Alternative conceptions in physics concepts have been studied extensively in mechanics (e.g., McDermott & Redish 1999; Minstrell 1982; Ramadas et al. 1996) and in DC circuits (Baser 2006; Baser & Durmus 2010; Bilal & Erol 2009; McDermott & Redish 1999; Rosenthal & Henderson 2006). However, alternative conceptions in the other concepts of EM have not been investigated to such an extent as in Mechanics (e.g. Duit 2009; McDermott & Redish 1999; Planinic 2006; Saglam & Millar 2006). The investigations on EM concepts were mainly focused on the development and evaluation of diagnostic tests (Ding et al. 2006; Engelhadt & Beichner 2004; Maloney et al. 2001; Marx 1998; Saglam & Millar 2004). The diagnostic tests are meant to identify alternative conceptions in terms of only the options that are provided in them. Usually these options have one correct answer (the scientific conception) and three or four alternative conceptions. These alternative conceptions are mostly the naïve physics ideas and they do not constitute the ontological and the lateral alternative conceptions. In other words, they rely only on students’ epistemological views (Elby 2010; Smith & Wenk 2006). In this paper, it is estimated that students’ alternative conceptions are dependent on their own personal epistemological and ontological views. As a result, it is believed to have inclusive perspectives on the students’ conceptions in order to have holistic categories of their descriptions of the investigated concepts. At present, little is known about categories of students' alternative conceptions in physics concepts in relation to inclusive conceptual change perspective. Previous studies (Clark et al. 2011; Elby 2010) have been done in the context of introductory mechanics, especially in relation to force and motion. These earlier studies have mainly focused to the disputable ‘coherence versus fragmentation’ structure of the concept of force based on their respective perspectives (Clark et al. 2011; diSessa et al. 2004; Elby 2010), that is, students’ conceptions as theories or as “pieces” perspectives. So far no study has been undertaken to categorize students’ alternative conceptions in the concepts of EM. In short, knowledge about categories of alternative conceptions in the concepts of EM is lacking. Therefore, at present, there is a need to investigate and fill in the existing gap with regards to the categories of students’ alternative conceptions in the concepts of electric potential and energy. Consequently, this study addresses the following research questions in the context of Ambo University, Ethiopia. 1. What are the categories of students’ alternative conceptions in the concepts of electric potential and energy? 2. How extensive the students’ alternative conceptions are in the existing categories? 3. How consistent/inconsistent the students’ alternative conceptions are in the existing categories? 2 Methodology 2.1 Design A descriptive case study design that involved qualitative research approach was used to increase the validity of the investigation reported in this paper. Qualitative method was chosen because of the nature of alternative conceptions research. That is, as studies (Stavy 1998, Vosniadou 2007) have shown, the nature of the study of students’ conceptions is a complex process. This is mainly because of the fact that students’ conceptions in science are vacillated from one context to another and features found in students’ conceptions are idiosyncratic and diversified (Li et al. 2006). Besides, the investigation was a descriptive case study that used the 7 Categorization of Alternative Conceptions in Electricity and Magnetism presupposed categories of alternative conceptions based on the earlier literature review. Therefore, in general, the method was used to have an in-depth investigation on the categories of the students’ alternative conceptions in the conceptual area of electric potential and energy by using focus group discussion as a means of data collection. In doing so, the students’ different ways of depictions or descriptions of concepts in their discussion were considered based on the nature and structure of their alternative conceptions. 2.2 Method 2.2.1 Sample The study involved 35 first year undergraduate physics students (age ranged from 18 to 23 years) who were registered for the course Electricity and Magnetism at Ambo University, Ethiopia in 2011. Data were collected from these students in the beginning of the year 2011. All the students knew each other as they were taught together in the first semester (October to January 2010) of the Ethiopian academic year. Concerning ethical issues, individuals in the research were treated as autonomous whose decisions on whether or not to participate in research was respected and not dominated by the researcher. So, participants’ permission was requested along with the department’s support before data collection. The students were then informed about the aim of the research before the collection of data. They were informed that the research test scores and any other means of data collection done are confidential and have no impact on the classroom assessment. In addition, written consents with the participants were made. Moreover, ethical clearance was secured from the research committee of UNISA (University of South Africa). 2.2.2 Instrument: Focus Group Discussion (FGD) In this study, focus group discussion was used as a tool for data collection to elicit participants’ conceptual perceptions in the concepts of EPE. Focus group is a research technique that enables to collect data through group interaction on a topic determined by a researcher (Morgan 1996). In recent years, using focus group discussion as a research tool has grown in educational research to encourage discussion among participants in order to generate qualitative data (Valerie 1997; Parker & Tritter 2006). Four focus groups were formed out of the volunteering students. In each focus group, eight to nine students participated in the discussions. In the formation of the focus groups, maximum effort was made to form nearly homogeneous ability focus groups and minimize influences of potentially dominant students. To this end, the students’ conceptual knowledge in EM was checked with the Conceptual Survey of Electricity and Magnetism (CSEM) (Maloney et al. 2001). The CSEM is a survey multiple-choice diagnostic test to assess students’ conceptual knowledge in conceptual areas of EM. It has been widely implemented in different contexts of physics education researches related to EM (Maloney et al. 2001; Planinic 2006; Pollock 2008; Zavala & Alarcon, 2008). Therefore, the test was used to notice the students’ conceptual knowledge level and then form approximately homogenous focus groups for discussions. The students’ average score on the test was 20.8%. This score was low and lay in the random response state3 (Bao & Reddish 2001). From the participants, 98% of them scored below 31% and only one student (2% of them) scored 40.6%, the maximum score for the class. This means the students’ scores lay nearly in the low random response state. Therefore, it was The random response state is a low-level response state in which the students’ responses are somewhat evenly distributed among more than three choices. 3 8 Categorization of Alternative Conceptions in Electricity and Magnetism noticed that the students’ conceptual knowledge was in the same low-level that their dominance on each other during discussions was assumed insignificant. The discussions were held before the semester’s classroom learning commenced and helped to record their alternative conceptions. The discussions were audio-taped and observation notes (for capturing of non-verbal communication) were taken. The moderator was attentive to the focus groups interactions that allowed him to observe and understand the agreement or disagreement among participants concerning of the EPE concepts investigated. The discussions were exhaustively lasted until no additional and new students’ ideas/conceptions of EPE were added. For validity of focus groups discussion, the factual fitness of the participants’ discussion was checked (not to omit and/or incorrectly transcribe). With regards to its reliability a focus group formation and discussion guide was developed for implementation and replication (if a need arises) of the discussions. 2.2.2.1 Focus Groups Formation and Discussion Guide This guide was developed to keep consistency of the discussions among the focus groups and help other researchers who need to undertake similar investigations or who need to replicate the study in a similar or other context. It incorporates the number and size of the groups, the type and number of discussion questions, participants’ selection methods, the time schedule and the moderator’s experience in relation to the concepts of the study. The number and size of the groups: Initially, 45 students who were registered for the course EM agreed to participate in this study. Accordingly, four focus groups, each with 11 to 12 students, were formed on the basis of their score in the CSEM test. However, 2 to 3 students from each group were absent from the discussions. This reduced the total number of participants to 35 with group size of 8 to 9 students each. However, it was believed that the absence of 2 to 3 students from each group did not affect the result as their scores lay in the same level. The moderator: In this study, the moderator was the first author, who has more than 20 years experience of teaching physics in both high school and university and also has the necessary subject matter knowledge, which is essential in relation to alternative conceptions and conceptual change research. The type and number of discussion questions: Four open-ended conceptual questions from the EPE concepts (see Appendix) were prepared for discussion. Every member of the group was motivated and expected to participate actively in the discussion. Selection Method of Participants: Students were voluntarily asked to participate in the focus group discussions. Each focus group was homogeneous with regard to their score in the CSEM test. This was done to minimize influences of some dominant students during the discussions. The time schedule: On average, the maximum time for the discussion of each group was 75 minutes. However, each group’s discussion on the four discussion questions was finished within 60 minutes. 2.2.3 Data Analysis The purpose of this paper is to report on the categorization students’ alternative conceptions in the concepts of EPE. Subsequently, the analysis of the data collected from the focus group discussions was driven by the purpose of the study. To this end, ‘framework analysis’ method (Rabiee 2004; Ritchie & Spencer 1994) for focus groups data analysis was adapted. The reason for adapting this method was, specifically, its characteristic feature that it allows themes to develop from both the presupposed and newly emerged (Rabiee 2004). In this 9 Categorization of Alternative Conceptions in Electricity and Magnetism study, the main topic (alternative conception) and the four predetermined themes (naïve physics, ontological alternative conception, lateral alternative conception and Ohms p-prim) were developed from the literature. Besides, there was an open-mindedness to include additional emerging themes from the discussions of the students. Thus, five stages of the framework analysis described by Ritchie & Spencer (1994) were used as the basis for the focus group data analysis. These were familiarization, identifying a thematic framework, coding, charting and interpretation. Familiarization: In this first stage, the audio-taped interviews were transcribed verbatim and complemented with observation notes. Repeated listening to tapes and reading the observational notes were done to understand a sense of the entire interviews. In short, a systematic review of observational notes and transcripts were conducted at this level. Identifying a thematic framework: In this stage, the presupposed themes were recognized and extra emerged themes were developed in accordance with the conceptions expressed by the students. Simultaneously, the conceptions of the students were filtered and classified (categorized) into themes. Filtering, in this case, was meant to keep aside the irrelevant participants’ statements, which were irrelevant to the research question. Also, it was maintained open-mindedness not to force the data to fit with the predetermined themes. Consequently, in addition to the predetermined themes, extra themes were categorized. The extra themes were meant for those students’ ideas that did not fit into the preset themes. These extra themes were labeled as mixed conceptions and loose ideas. The mixed conceptions were the students’ incorrect descriptions of concepts that possibly be categorized into more than one predetermined categories, while the loose ideas were incorrect descriptions of the students that impossibly be categorized into one or more categories. Coding: This third stage applied to all the transcribed data that correspond to particular themes (categories). At this stage, segments of transcripts were coded in terms of the categories they belonged to with their respective focus groups. For example, a segment of the transcripts of data that belonged to naïve physics and was discussed by the first focus group was coded as NPFG1. Likewise, a segment of transcript that belonged to lateral alternative conception and was discussed by the second focus group was coded as LACFG2 and so on. The codes were annotated in the margins beside the texts. Charting: In this fourth stage of the analysis, the specific pieces of data that were coded in the previous stage were arranged in tables of the themes (categories of alternative conceptions). Firstly, the coded segments of the transcribed data were organized into categories in relation to the concepts in the discussion questions. In other words, coded descriptive statements of the students’ conceptions in EPE concepts were developed into categories. These categories were both the presupposed and the emerged themes based on the students’ own knowledge structures and natures of their conceptual explanations given in each conceptual question, according to the information required by the research question. Then the data were shifted from their original textual context and placed in tables that consist of headings and subheadings that were drawn during the thematic framework. Interpretation: This fifth stage involved the analysis of the key characteristics as presented in tables. This analysis was able to provide tables of the data set. In short, interpretations were done on the developed categories or the framework in terms of frequency, extensiveness, consistency/inconsistency (Rabiee 2004) and distribution of the students’ alternative conceptions within and across the categories. These key characteristics were analyzed as follows. 10 Categorization of Alternative Conceptions in Electricity and Magnetism Frequency and Extensiveness: The term frequency relates to the consideration of how often a conception or an idea was made across the four focus groups discussions, while the term extensiveness refers to the total frequency of the alternative conceptions in a category. The frequencies of students’ conceptions discussed across the four focus groups were analyzed. In other words, this frequency was the same as the number of focus groups independently discussed a conception. The maximum of this frequency of a conception was four, which is equal to the number of the focus groups, while its minimum was one, i.e., when only one focus group discussed a conception. Correspondingly, the total frequencies (extensivenesses) were analyzed in each category of students’ conceptions. The extensivenesses were analyzed to study how often the students’ conceptions were found in the categories. The frequency and extensiveness of the conceptions in each category were provided by tables in the results section to guide the interpretation (see Tables 1 to 7). Consistency or Inconsistency: Consistency/inconsistency was considered as the changes or the variations in conceptions of a concept by the students within and across the categories. The consistency/inconsistency of the students’ alternative conceptions was provided by a table in the results section (see Table 8). Distribution: Distribution referred to the total number of alternative conceptions in a category of alternative conceptions. Correspondingly, percentage distribution referred to the percentage of the ratio of the number of alternative conceptions in a category to the total number of alternative conceptions and ideas in all the categories (see Table 7). 2.3 Results The results to the first research question of ‘what are the categories of students’ alternative conceptions in the conceptual area of electric potential and energy’ were analyzed and presented. In this paper, there were six diagnosed categories of alternative conceptions out of which four were the presupposed categories while the other two emerged. These were the naive physics, the lateral alternative conceptions, the ontological alternative conceptions, the Ohm’s p-primes, the mixed conceptions and the loose ideas. The presentation of the categories of the students’ alternative conceptions followed their order of distributions with respect to the frequencies and extensivenesses of the alternative conceptions. These categories were analyzed and presented as follows. 2.3.1 Theme1: Naïve Physics The naive physics was one of the themes developed from the students’ descriptions of concepts in the conceptual area. In the concepts of EPE, 17 alternative conceptions were categorized into the naïve physics (see Table 1). From these alternative conceptions, seven were in electric potential, four in electric field and six in the energy concepts. For example, several students described that ‘there is an electric potential only when a charge is at rest’ and they considered the potential difference as the difference between only positive and negative potentials. Also, they perceived that potential of two opposite charges separated by some distance is zero. This statement is true if the two opposite charges are equal and the potential is calculated at their midpoint. Another example was that of ‘uniform electric field implies a uniform velocity’. The students considered that a charge released in a uniform electric field moves at a constant velocity. This was a consequence of “force implies velocity” from Aristotelian physics. Students had some intuitive reasons for their explanations. However, they were unable to visualize that the electric field can change the velocity of a moving body or consequently, it can accelerate a charge released in it. 11 Categorization of Alternative Conceptions in Electricity and Magnetism Table1 shows the students’ alternative conceptions categorized into the naïve physics in relation to the frequency and extensiveness of their alternative conceptions. Table 1: Naïve physics in EPE concepts with their frequency & extensiveness Concepts Electric potential Electric field Energy 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. 7. 8. 9. 10. 11. 12. 13. 14. 15. 16. 17. Naïve physics in EPE concepts frequency Charge at rest implies electric potential. 2 Electric potential of two opposite charges system is zero. 2 Potential difference is the difference between positive and negative potentials. 2 The potential difference of charges separated by large distance is zero. 1 An electric potential is confined only in an atom. 1 Equipotential lines have only circular structure. 1 A changed body has only one type of charge 1 Positive charge is the only source of electric field. 2 Uniform electric field implies a uniform velocity of a charge. 2 In an electric field, Positive charge accelerates but the negative charge 1 decelerates. Uniform electric field implies a projectile motion of a charge 1 Kinetic energy of a charge is constant in a uniform electric field. 1 Kinetic and potential energies are constant in an electric field (naïve energy 2 idea). Positive charges have positive energy; negative charges have negative energy. 1 Work is done on an equipotential line. 1 Moving charges have no electric potential energy. 1 The kinetic energy of a charge changes on an equipotential line. 1 Extensiveness of Naïve physics in EPE concepts 23 2.3.2 Theme 2: Lateral Alternative Conceptions In the EPE concepts, there were 12 lateral alternative conceptions (see Table 2). For example, several students agreed on consideration of electric potential as a force. It means that they incorrectly label electric potential under some properties of force. Although electric potential and force can share some properties and they are found in the same ontological category; laterally, they belong to different sub categories. In this conceptual area, eight alternative conceptions were documented in the concept of electric potential, four in the concept of electric potential energy and one in the electric field concept. Table 2 Lateral alternative conceptions in EPE concepts with their frequency & extensiveness Concepts Electric potential 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. 7. Lateral Alternative Conceptions in EPE concepts frequency Electric potential as a force: Electric potential is the attraction or repulsion of two 3 charges. Electric Potential as an attractive force: Electrostatic potential is a potential of 2 positive and negative charged particles. Electric potential as potential energy: Electric potential is a potential energy that 2 electric charges possess. Electric potential as kinetic energy: Electric Potential is created by an electric field 1 because of moving charges. Electric potential as a vector: Electric potential is a vector which is based on the 2 signs (directions) of charges. Electric potential as charge flow/current: Electric potential is made up of flow of 1 charges from negative to positive charged materials. Potential as position & Potential difference as a distance: Potential difference is 1 the distance between two equipotential lines. 12 Categorization of Alternative Conceptions in Electricity and Magnetism 8. Electric field Electric potential energy 9. Equipotential lines as electric field lines: Equipotential lines are electric field lines that are equal and parallel to each other. Electric field as force: Electric field is the force on any test charge. 1 1 10. Electric energy as a force: Electric potential energy is a conservative force. Electric potential energy exists between two unlike charges (electric potential energy implies attraction force). 11. Electric potential energy as a potential difference: Electric potential energy is the difference between electric potentials. 12. Electric potential energy as kinetic energy: Electric potential energy is when charges are in motion- (motion implies energy). 13. Electric potential energy as a vector: Electric potential energy is a vector that depends on the signs (+ or -) of charges. Extensiveness of lateral alternative conceptions in EPE concepts 2 1 3 1 21 2.3.3 Theme 3: Ontological Alternative Conceptions The alternative conceptions, in this case, were based on whether students had placed the concepts under investigation in a correct or an incorrect ontological category. In this theme, nine alternative conceptions were documented. Among these alternative conceptions, four were in the concept of electric potential, three were in the electric field concept and two were in the concept of electric potential energy. For example, the students considered electric potential, electric field and energy as substances under the category of matter (see Table 3). It means that they considered the concepts as an ontological nature of substance or matter. Table 3 Ontological alternative conceptions in EPE concepts with their frequency & extensiveness Concepts Electric potential 1. 2. 3. Electric field 4. 5. 6. 7. Electric potential energy 8. Ontological Alternative conceptions in EPE concepts Electric potential as material object: Positive potential is connected to positive charge; negative potential is connected to a negative charge. When electric potential is released charge is also released. Electric potential as charges: Electric potential is divided into two; negative charges & positive charges. Electric potential as a charge at rest: Electric potential is an electric charge which does not possess motion. Equipotential lines as real lines: Equipotential lines move in a parallel direction. Electric field as a substance: Electric field flows from positive to negative. Electric field exists due to flow of charges through a given surface. Electric lines as a flow of charges: Electric field lines pass through a surface with charges. Field lines as a flow of potential: A negative charge is used for storage of electric potential. Students considered electric field lines as electric potential. Potential energy as a material object: Potential energy moves with equal speed and parallel to each other 9. Kinetic energy as a substance: When electric potential and charges are released, kinetic energy is also released. Extensiveness of ontological alternative conceptions in EPE concepts frequency 2 1 1 1 2 1 1 1 1 11 2.3.4 Theme 4: Ohm’s P-Primes The Ohm’s Law p-prim category of alternative conceptions was reflected by some of the students involved in this study. For example, students discussed that “when distance from a 13 Categorization of Alternative Conceptions in Electricity and Magnetism charge increases electric potential also increases” (see Table 4). In this case, the students considered electric potential as it increases proportional to the position. Concepts Electric potential Electric field & energy Table 4 Ohm’s p-primes in EPE concepts with their frequency & extensiveness Ohm’s p-primes in EPE concepts frequency 1. When distance from a charge increases electric potential also increases. 1 2. An electric potential is directly proportional to kinetic energy. 1 3. In an electric field, electric potential energy and kinetic energy of a charge 1 are directly proportional. 4. Electric potential energy is directly proportional to the distance. 1 4 Extensiveness of Ohm’s primes in EPE concepts 2.3.5 Theme 5: Mixed Conceptions A mixed conception was meant for an alternative conception that was believed to have at least two characteristic features of the predetermined four categories ((see paragraph 3 in section 2.2.3). Accordingly, two alternative conceptions were identified and categorized in this theme (see Table 5). Table 5 Mixed conceptions in EPE concepts with their frequency & extensiveness Concepts Electric field Energy Mixed type Mixed category alternative conceptions in EPE Motion of changes in a field as capacitor’s charging process: In a uniform electric field, a positive charge moves to the positive terminal and a negative charge moves to the negative terminal. 2. Total energy increases and they are proportional to each other: When a charged particle is released in a uniform electric field, its energy will increase and become positive. i.e., when potential energy increases the kinetic energy also increases. Here, the students conceived as if both are directly proportional to each other. Extensiveness of mixed conceptions in EPE concepts Naïve physics & Lateral alternative conception Naïve physics & Ohm’s p-primes 1. frequency 1 2 3 2.3.6 Theme 6: Loose ideas Loose ideas are meant for the incorrect students' descriptions that were thought not to be attached to any of the four predetermined categories (see paragraph 3 in section 2.2.3). The conceptions in this theme have characteristics of fluidity that pose a problem not to definitely categorize them into either the predetermined or the mixed category. For example, ‘electric potential is a potential that depends on given two charges’ was one of the conceptions categorized into the loose ideas theme (see Table 6). In this case, it was impossible to be certain whether the students conceived the electric potential as a force, as an electric energy or as a summation of the potentials of the two charges because all these concepts are dependent on a system of two charges. Table 6 Loose ideas in EPE concepts with their frequency & extensiveness Concept Electric potential 1. Loose ideas frequency Electric potential is a potential that depends on two given charges. In this case, no evidence was shown by the students whether they described the potential of two charges or they considered potential as potential energy. 3 14 Categorization of Alternative Conceptions in Electricity and Magnetism 2. Energy 3. Electric Potential is one type of potential that could be related to kinetic energy Equipotential energy is the energy stored in the charge at equal distances. The students did not mention whether they described equipotential lines or electric potential energy. Extensiveness of loose ideas in EPE concepts 1 1 5 2.3.7 Extensiveness of Alternative Conceptions in the Categories In response to the second research question regarding extensivenesses of the alternative conceptions categories, Table 7 presents a summary of the students’ alternative conceptions in the categories in terms of their frequencies and extensivenesses. In general, as illustrated in the table, the naive physics and lateral alternative conceptions were more extensive; while the Ohm’s p-primes and mixed conceptions were less extensive in the categories. However, perspective wise, the total number of alternative conceptions in the naïve physics and Ohm’s p-primes were 21 (43.6%); while the total number of lateral and ontological alternative conceptions were 22 (45.8%). This shows that the distribution of alternative conceptions in the two cognitive views of conceptual change (epistemological and ontological) was comparable and considerable. Correspondingly, perspective wise extensivenesses were compared. As a result, the extensiveness of the naive physics and Ohm’s p-primes was totally 40.9%; while the extensiveness of categorical alternative conceptions (ontological and lateral) was totally 48.5%. These two extensivenesses in the two perspectives were also considerable though they had insignificant difference in favor of categorical conceptions (see Table 7). Table 7 Distribution of alternative conceptions, frequencies and extensivenesses in the categories Category Naïve physics Lateral Ontological Ohm’s p-primes Mixed Loose ideas Total Distribution of alternative conceptions 17 (35.4%) 13 (27.0%) 9 (18.8%) 4 (8.3%) 2 (4.2%) 3 (6.3%) 48 4 - Frequency 2 6 4 2 1 3 13 (6%) (27%) 3 2 1 Extensiveness 1 11 7 7 4 1 2 32 (67%) 23 (34.8%) 21 (31.8%) 11 (16.7%) 4 (6.1%) 3(4.5%) 4 (6.1%) 66 2.3.8 Inconsistency of the Alternative Conceptions In response to the third research question, Tables 1 to 6 were combined in Table 8 to analyze and present the consistency or inconsistency of alternative conceptions of the concepts in EPE within and across the categories. For example, the alternative conceptions of electric potential emerged across all the categories. These alternative conceptions were electric potential as a force (lateral alternative conception), rest implies electric potential (naive physics), electric potential as a material object (ontological alternative conception), an electric potential is proportional to distance (Ohm’s p-prim) and electric potential could be related to kinetic energy (loose idea). Similarly, within the category of lateral alternative conceptions, the electric potential was considered as a force, energy, a vector and a current. This showed the inconsistency of the students’ alternative conceptions across and within the categories. 15 Categorization of Alternative Conceptions in Electricity and Magnetism Table 8 Inconsistency in students’ alternative conceptions of EPE concepts within and across the categories Category Electric Potential Naïve physics Ontological Alternative conceptions Lateral Alternative conceptions Loose ideas An electric potential is proportional to distance Electric potential proportional to kinetic energy Positive change is the only source of electric field Uniform electric field implies a uniform velocity In a uniform electric field, positive charge accelerates; while the negative charge decelerates Uniform electric field implies a projectile Electric field as a force Energy Electric field as a substance Electric lines as a flow of charges Field lines as a flow of potential 3 Electric potential as a force Electric Potential as an attractive force Electric potential as potential energy Electric potential as kinetic energy Electric potential as a vector Electric Potential as charge flow/current Electric potential as position & Potential difference as a distance Equipotential lines as electric lines Electric potential as material object Electric potential as charges Electric potential as a charge at rest Equipotential lines as real lines Mixed concepti ons Ohm’s pprimes Rest implies electric potential Potential of two opposite charges system is zero The potential difference is the difference between positive and negative potentials The potential difference of charges separated by large distance is zero An electric potential is confined only in an atom Equipotential lines have only circular structure A changed body has only one type of charges Concepts Electric Field Motion of changes in a field as capacitor’s charging process Electric potential depends on a given two charges. Electric potential could be related to kinetic energy. Kinetic energy of a charge is constant in a uniform electric field Kinetic and potential energies are constant in an electric field Positive charges have positive energy; negative charges have negative energy. Work is done on an equipotential line Moving charges have no potential energy Kinetic energy of a charge changes on an equipotential line Energy as a force Electric potential energy as potential difference Electric potential energy as kinetic energy Electric potential energy as a vector Potential energy as a material object Kinetic energy as substance In an electric field, electric potential energy and kinetic energy of a charge are directly proportional to each other Electric potential energy is directly proportional to the distance The total energy of a charge increases and they are proportional to each other Equipotential energy is the energy stored in the charge at equal distances Discussion This case study was conducted mainly to categorize undergraduate first year physics students’ alternative conceptions in the concepts of electric potential and energy in Ethiopia. Besides, the extensivenesses of the alternative conceptions in the categories and the inconsistencies of the alternative conceptions within and across the categories were studied. The first reason was the existence of a research gap with regards to the categories of students’ 16 Categorization of Alternative Conceptions in Electricity and Magnetism alternative conceptions in the concepts of electricity and magnetism. The second was its practical implication for both instruction and curriculum. The third was its theoretical implication to contribute to the existing disputable perspectives of the structure of students’ alternative conceptions (Clark et al. 2011; diSessa et al. 2004; Elby 2010). And the fourth was to undertake an in-depth qualitative study by focusing on a few concepts of EM as recommended in earlier studies (Planinic 2006). To categorize the existing students’ alternative conceptions of the EPE concepts in the situation, open-ended questions on the concepts were discussed by the students in the focus groups. During the analysis six categories of alternative conceptions were diagnosed. These categories were naïve physics (35.4%), lateral alternative conceptions (27.0%), ontological alternative conceptions (18.8%), Ohm’s p-primes (8.3%), loose ideas (6.3%) and mixed conceptions (4.2%) in order of their percentage distributions, which shows that the categories have different distributions of the alternative conceptions (see Table 7). The overall percentage distribution of loose ideas and mixed conceptions was 10.5%, which was less than the overall distribution of the other four predetermined categories. Therefore, in this conceptual area of EM, the students’ alternative conceptions identified from their focus groups discussions were dominantly (about 90%) categorized into the presupposed four categories. Among these, the naïve physics and the lateral alternative conceptions have 30 (62.5%) of the depicted alternative conceptions (see Table 7). These two categories were more dominant than the ontological alternative conceptions and the Ohm’s p-primes. Although most of the alternative conceptions revealed were categorized into naïve physics and lateral alternative conceptions, the alternative conceptions were inconsistent and diversified within the categories (see Table 8). The students’ alternative conceptions not only lay in multiple categories but also in multiple conceptions of a concept within a category. For example, the concept of electric potential were considered as a force, as potential energy, as a kinetic energy as a current and as a vector. All these conceptions were categorized as lateral alternative conceptions. A similar situation was investigated in the ontological alternative conceptions. As seen in the data (see Table 8), several students considered independently electric potential as a material body and as a charge. In this case, although the dependence of electric potential on charges is known, the students were unaware of the cause and effect relation; instead, they simply consider potential and charge as the same concept that share similar properties. Furthermore, in the Ohm’s p-primes, it was independently considered that ‘electric potential is proportional to distance and also proportional to the kinetic energy.’ In this study, the students have multiple conceptions of a concept in different categories (see Table 8). As an example, the students’ alternative conceptions in the concept of electric potential lay in almost all the existed categories. These were electric potential as charges (ontological alternative conception), as electric potential energy (lateral alternative conception), a zero electric potential for two opposite charges system (naïve physics) and electric potential proportional to kinetic energy (Ohm’s p-prim). This shows the inconsistency of the students’ alternative conception across the categories. In the EPE concepts, a total of 45 alternative conceptions and three loose (uncertain) ideas were discussed among the participants in the four focus groups. From all these conceptions and ideas no alternative conception was discussed by all the four focus groups; 6% of the alternative conceptions were discussed by any three of the focus groups; 27% of the alternative conceptions were discussed by any two of the focus groups and 67% of the alternative conceptions were discussed by any one of the focus groups (see Table 7). This implies that most of the students’ 17 Categorization of Alternative Conceptions in Electricity and Magnetism alternative conceptions were inconsistent. In other words, the students’ conceptions were vacillated highly among and within the focus groups. This result agrees with the inclusive idea of vacillation of students’ conceptions in science concepts (Li et al. 2006; Tao & Gunstone 1999). However, only two most frequently revealed lateral alternative conceptions were found in this study. These lateral alternative conceptions were the students’ conceptions of electric potential as a force and electric energy as a force (see Table 2). In the existing categories, the distributions of alterative conceptions and their extensiveness across the focus groups showed similar patterns. This means both the distributions were found to have approximately comparable values (see Table 7). In other words, a category with a largest number of alternative conceptions was found to have a largest distribution of extensiveness across the FCD and vice versa. This showed again the diversification of alternative conceptions in all the categories. In this study, the epistemological and ontological descriptions of students’ conceptions were considered to categorize their conceptions in EPE concepts. However, if one of the perspectives, either epistemological or ontological, was preferred as a view of this research, then such a holistic categorization would not be investigated. For example, on the one hand, if the epistemological perspective was traced then the dominant categories would be the naïve physics and the Ohm’s p-primes that could overlook the ontological and lateral alternative conceptions of the students. On the other hand, if the ontological perspective was traced then the reverse would be the case. Based on this view and the result of this study, it was concluded that one of the bases to the disputable perspectives on the structure of alternative conceptions is the different theoretical framework used by different research of alternative conceptions. Therefore, a pragmatic inclusive perspective was helpful to study these complex and holistic classifications of alternative conceptions. Among the alternative conceptions identified in this study, there are some similar alternative conceptions identified in earlier studies. For example, studies (Galili 1995; Planinic 2006) have identified ‘uniform field implies uniform velocity’ naïve conception. Also, Baser & Geban (2007) identified ‘a charged body contains only one type of charge’ naïve conception. However, most of the ontological and lateral alternative conceptions are newly identified. From the category of naïve physics, alternative conceptions, like potential of two opposite point charges is zero; rest implies electric potential; a potential difference of charges separated by large distance is zero; and moving charges have no electric potential energy are newly identified alternative conceptions. In this study, the categories of students’ alternative conceptions were studied using data from focus groups discussions that mostly based on the participants’ consensuses of responses on the discussion questions. As a result, some individuals’ conceptions were overlooked that might limit the descriptive inference of study. This means that the distributions of the alternative conceptions in the categories might be affected. Therefore, for more usefulness of the results in this study, a similar study that would consider individuals' perceptions was designed to be undertaken using individual concept maps as a tool of investigation in the same or different concepts. 4 Conclusion This paper may be the first report of its kind on a categorization of undergraduate physics students’ alternative conceptions in the concepts of electric potential and energy in Ethiopia. In general the categories of alternative conceptions studied in this study are complex and holistic in nature. Five categories of alternative conceptions and one category of loose ideas were 18 Categorization of Alternative Conceptions in Electricity and Magnetism diagnosed (see paragraph 1 in section 2.3). The first four categories are presupposed or prescribed categories based on earlier studies in science education and cognitive science; while the last two categories emerged from the study. In general, the categorized alternative conceptions have the following characteristics. The distributions of the alternative conceptions are different across the categories; The students’ alternative conceptions of a specific concept are categorized into different thematic categories; Within a category, the students have multiple conceptions of specific concepts; In the existing categories, most of the students’ conceptions are described less frequently; Category wise, the naïve physics and lateral alternative conceptions were more extensive than the others; Perspectives wise, both epistemological and ontological alterative conceptions are comparable and considerable; The students’ conceptions are mostly inconsistent across and within the categories; and The students’ alternative conceptions in the categories are diversified. Earlier studies in physics concepts were mainly focused on identifying alternative conceptions. However, this study is the first of its kind to diagnose the existing categories of students’ alternative conceptions in physics, particularly, in the electric potential and energy concepts. Therefore, the results of this study are helpful in both practical and theoretical problems related to science and technology education in general and physics education in particular. The practical and theoretical implications of this study are discussed in the following section. 5 5.1 Implications Instructional Implication A number of studies (Dykstra et al. 1992; Grayson 1994; Hake 1998) have shown that conceptual change in physics cannot be achieved solely by traditional teaching. Because students’ alternative conceptions are highly resistant to change by traditional teaching strategies; and they are firmly established in everyday experiences. In addition, studies (Chabay & Sherwood 2006; Saglam & Millar 2006; Planinic 2006) that have reported on students’ concepts in EM at university level showed that the sequential structure of instruction that spend most of the course on problem solving was ineffective to reduce students’ alternative conceptions. Moreover, the cognitive conflict (Hewson & Hewson 1984) of the classical conceptual change approach, which are based on conceptual change model (Posner et al. 1982), have their own limitations in the context of this study, though they appear to be more efficient than traditional teaching. The limitations, also investigated in this study, are due to the inconsistent nature of students’ alternative conceptions (Duit & Treagust 2003). Further constraints of cognitive conflict are attributed to the diversity found in students’ alternative conceptions as investigated in this study. The cognitive conflict strategy (Hewson & Hewson 1984) often facilitates conflict between students’ coherent alternative conceptions and scientific conceptions intended to be taught. In addition, students’ responses to scientific conceptions are influenced by their prior knowledge (Chinn & Brewer 1993). That is, if students have inconsistent alternative conceptions about a topic, like in the concepts of electric potential and energy investigated in this study (see 19 Categorization of Alternative Conceptions in Electricity and Magnetism paragraph 3 in section 2.2.2), then cognitive conflict cannot be meaningful at all. Consequently, it may be difficult to expect students’ conceptual change. In the physics concepts studied in this situation, students’ alternative conceptions are mostly inconsistent; different in distributions within and across the categories and less frequently described. Besides, the alternative conceptions in from the two perspectives were considerable and comparable. Furthermore, multiplicities of alternative conceptions of concepts within a category were found. This means students' conceptions diversify from one context of a concept to another. In short, the students are not confident in their alternative conceptions. Therefore, in order to address and change alternative conceptions of such students, an appropriate supportive approach needs to be proposed and implemented. Based on the results of this study, the supportive approach proposed for conceptual change of such students in the domain of this study would incorporate the following. First, the supportive approach should be evolutionary but not revolutionary theory change paradigm, like the cognitive conflict strategy. In other words, the process for conceptual change must be gradually done, because there was no consistent alternative conception found not to apply the revolutionary cognitive conflict strategy and change it into the scientific conception. Then, the approach should comparably take both epistemological and ontological components of the students’ conceptual change. This means the mechanism must encompass categorical (lateral and ontological) restructuring of concepts as well as exchange of epistemological concepts. On the basis of the aforementioned, it is supposed that the cognitive perturbation strategy (Li et al. 2006) would be a better strategy to bring conceptual change among students. The basic idea of this strategy is based on the proposition that the paths of conceptual change for different students/groups of students are idiosyncratic, diverse and context sensitive (Li et al. 2006). Based on the contexts students would immerse, the types (paths) of perturbations necessary for initiating conceptual change can be varied. That is, the strategy is based on constructivist theory of learning (Driver et al. 1994), like the cognitive conflict strategy (Hewson & Hewson 1984) of classical conceptual change approach. However, the cognitive perturbation strategy provides appropriate perturbations to initiate students’ conceptual change towards more scientifically viable intermediate conceptions than their preconceptions (evolutionary process), before suddenly reaching scientific conceptions (Li et al. 2006). 5.2 Theoretical Implication This study was not intended to dispute between the coherence and fragmentation of alternative conceptions (Clark et al. 2011; diSessa et al. 2004; Elby 2010); however, the methods of this study and the results may facilitate arguments. Therefore, the diagnosed alternative conceptions are in favor to fragmentation over coherence perspective of students’ alternative conceptions, because the diagnosed students’ alternative conceptions are diversified, inconsistent within and across the categories and less frequent and less extensive in the categories. 6 References Bao, L., & Redish, E. F., (2001). Concentration Analysis: A Quantitative Assessment of Student States. Phys. Edu. Res., American Journal of Physics. 69 (7), S45-53. Bao, L., & Redish, E. F. (2006). Model analysis: representing and assessing the dynamics of student learning. Physical Review Special Topics – Physics Education Research, 2, 010103. Baser, M. (2006). Effects of Conceptual Change and Traditional Confirmatory Simulations on PreService Teachers’ Understanding of Direct Current Circuits. Journal of Science Education and Technology, 15(5), 367-381. 20 Categorization of Alternative Conceptions in Electricity and Magnetism Baser, M., & Durmus, S. (2010).The Effectiveness of Computer Supported Versus Real Laboratory Inquiry Learning Environments on the Understanding of Direct Current Electricity among PreService Elementary School Teachers. Eurasia Journal of Mathematics, Science & Technology Education, 6(1), 47-61. Baser, M., & Geban, O. (2007).Effect of instruction based on conceptual change activities on students’ understandings of static electricity concepts. Research in Science & Technological Education, 25(2), 243- 267. Bilal, E., & Erol, M. (2009). Investigating students' conceptions of some electricity concepts. Latin American Journal of Physics Education, 3(2), 193-201. Carey, S, (1985). Conceptual change in childhood. Cambridge, MA: MIT press Chabay, R & Sherwood, B. (2006). Restructuring the introductory Electricity & Magnetism course. American Journal of Physics, 74(4), 329-336. Chi, M.T.H. (2008). Three types of conceptual change: Belief revision, mental model transformation and categorical shift. In S. Vosniadou (Ed), Handbook of research on conceptual change, 61-82. Hillsdale, NJ:Erlbaum. Chi, M.T.H., & Roscoe, R.D. (2002). The processes and challenges of conceptual change. In M. Limon & L. Mason (Eds.), Reconsidering Conceptual Change: Issues in Theory and Practice (pp.3–27). Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers. Chi, M.T.H., & Slotta, J. D. (1993). The Ontological Coherence of Intuitive Physics. Cognition & Instruction, 10(2&3), 249-260. Chinn, C. A., & Brewer, W. F. (1993). The role of anomalous data in knowledge acquisition: a theoretical framework and implications for science education. Review of Educational Research, 63 (1), 1–49. Clement, J. (1982). Students’ Preconceptions in Introductory Mechanics, American Journal of Physics, 50, 66–71. Clark, D. B., D'Angelo, C. M., & Schleigh S. P. (2011). Comparison of Students' Knowledge Structure Coherence and Understanding of Force in the Philippines, Turkey, China, Mexico, and the United States, Journal of the Learning Sciences, 20(2), 207-261. Ding, L., Chabay, R., Sheewood, B., & Beichner, R. (2006). Evaluating an electricity and magnetism assessment tool: Brief electricity & magnetism assessment. Physical Review Special Topics-Physics Education Research. 2, 1-7. diSessa, A. A. (1982). Unlearning Aristotelian physics: A study of knowledge-based learning. Cognitive Science, 6(1), 37-75. diSessa, A. A. (1993). Towards an epistemology of physics. Cognition and Instruction, 10, (2&3), 105– 225. diSessa, A.A., Gillespie, N., & Esterly, J. (2004). Coherence versus fragmentation in the development of the concept of force. Cognitive Science, 28(6), 843-900. Driver, R., Asoko, H., Leach, J., Mortimer, E., & Scott, P. (1994). Constructing scientific knowledge in the classroom. Educational Researcher. 23(7), 5-12. Duit, R. (2009). Students’ and teachers’ conceptions and science education. Kiel: Institute for Science Education (IPN) (Distributed electronically). Duit, R., & Treagust, D. (2003). Conceptual change: a powerful framework for improving science teaching and learning. International Journal of Science Education. 25(6), 671-688. Duschl, R. A., & Gitomer, D. H. (1991). Epistemological Perspectives on Conceptual Change: Implications for Educational Practice. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 28 (9), 839-858. Dykstra, D.I., Boyle,C.F., & Monach, I. A. (1992). Studying conceptual change in learning physics. Science Education, 76 (6), 615-652. Elby, A., (2010). Coherence vs. Fragmentation in student epistemologies: A reply to Smith & Wenk. Electronic Journal of Science Education, 14(1), 1-22. Engelhadt, P.V., & Beichner, R.J. (2004). Students’ understanding of current resistive electrical circuits. American Journal of physics, 72(1), 98-115. 21 Categorization of Alternative Conceptions in Electricity and Magnetism Finkelstein, N. (2005). Learning Physics in Context: A study of student learning about electricity and magnetism. International Journal of Science Education. 27(10), 1187-109. Fosnot, C. T. 1996 (Ed), constructivism: theory, perspectives and practice, Teachers college, Columbia University, New York and London Fredette, N., & Lochhead, J. (1980). Student conceptions of simple circuits. Physics Teacher 18(3), 194198. Galili, I. (1995). Mechanics Background Influences Students’ Conceptions in Electromagnetism. International Journal of Science Education, 17, 3, 371-387. Grayson, D.J. (1994). Concept Substitution: An instructional Strategy for Promoting Conceptual Change. Research in Science Education, 24, 102-111. Hake, R.R. (1998). Interactive-engagement versus traditional methods: A six-thousand student survey of mechanics test data for introductory physics courses. American Journal of Physics, 66(1), 64–74. Hammer, D. (1996). Misconceptions or P-prims: how may alternative perspectives of cognitive structure influence instructional perceptions and intentions? The Journal of the Learning Sciences, 5(2), 97– 127. Hewson, P.W. (1981). A conceptual change approach to learning science. European Journal of Science Education, 3, 383–396. Hewson, P. W., & Hewson, M. G. (1984). The role of conceptual conflict in conceptual change and the Design of Science Instruction. Instructional Science, 13, 1-13. Ioannides, C., & Vosniadou, S. (2002). The changing meaning of force. Cognitive Science Quarterly, 2(1), 5-61. Leppävirta, J. (2012). The Effect of Naïve Ideas on Students’ Reasoning about Electricity and Magnetism. Research in Science Education, 42(4), 753-767. Li S. C., Law, N. & Lui, K. F. A., (2006). Cognitive perturbation through dynamic modeling: a pedagogical approach to conceptual change in science, Journal of Computer Assisted Learning. 22(6), 405–422. Linder, C.J. (1993). A challenge to conceptual change. Science Education, 77, 293-300. Maloney, D. P., O’Kuma, T. L., Hieggelke C. J., & Heuvelen A.V., (2001). Surveying students’ conceptual knowledge of electricity & magnetism. American Journal of Physics, S69 (7), 12-23. Marx, J. D. (1998). Creation of a Diagnostic Exam for Introductory, Undergraduate Electricity and Magnetism (Doctoral dissertation, Rensselaer Polytechnic Institute). Retrieved December 5, 2010, from http://www.compadre.org/Repository/document/ServeFile.cfm?ID=3786&DocID=114 McCloskey, M. (1983). Intuitive physics, Scientific American, 248(4), 122–130. McDermott, L. C., & Redish, E. F. (1999). Resource Letter: PER-1: Physics Education Research. American Journal of Physics, 67 (9), 755-767. Minstrell, J. (1982). Explaining ‘at rest’ condition of an object, The Physics Teacher, 20(1), 10–14. MOE (2009). Harmonized curriculum for BSc Degree program in Physics, Ethiopia. Addis Ababa. Morgan, D. L. (1996). Focus groups. Annual Review of Sociology, 22, 129–152. Osborne, R. (1981). Children’s ideas about electric current. New Zealand Science Teacher, 29, 12-19. Osborne, R. (1983). Towards Modifying Children’s Ideas about Electric Current. Research in Science and Technological Education, 1 (1), 73-83. Parker, A., & Tritter J., (2006).Focus group method and methodology: current practice and recent debate, International Journal of Research & Method in Education, 29 (1) 23–37. Planinic, M. (2006). Assessment of difficulties of some conceptual areas from Electricity & magnetism using the Conceptual survey of Electricity and magnetism. American Journal of Physics. 73(12), 1143-1148. Planinic, M., Boone, W. J., Krsnik, R., & Beilfuss, M. L. (2006). Exploring alternative conceptions from Newtonian dynamics and simple DC circuits: links between item difficulty and item confidence. Journal of research in Science Teaching, 43(2), 150-171. Pollock, S. (2008, July 23-24). Comparing Student Learning with Multiple Research-Based Conceptual Surveys: CSEM and BEMA. Paper presented at Physics Education Research Conference 2008, 22 Categorization of Alternative Conceptions in Electricity and Magnetism Edmonton, Canada. Retrieved January 5, 2011, from http://www.compadre.org/Repository/document/ServeFile.cfm?ID=8109&DocID=742 Posner, G. J., Strike, K. A., Hewson, P. W. & Gertzog, W. A. (1982). Accommodation of a scientific conception: Toward a theory of conceptual change. Science Education. 66, 211–227. Rabiee, F., (2004). Focus-group interview and data analysis. Proceedings of the Nutrition Society, 63, 655–660. Ramadas, J., Barve, S., & Kumar, A., (1996). Alternative conceptions in Galilean relativity: inertial and non‐inertial observers. International Journal of Science Education, 18(5), 615-629. Ritchie, J. & Spencer, L., (1994). Qualitative data analysis for applied policy research. In Analyzing Qualitative Data, pp. 173–194 [A Bryman and RG Burgess, editors]. London: Routledge. Rosenthal, A. S., & Henderson, C. (2006).Teaching about circuits at the introductory level: An emphasis on potential difference. American Journal of Physics. 74 (4), 324-328. Saglam, M., & Millar, R. (2004). Diagnostic test of students’ ideas in electromagnetism. Retrieved October 29, 2010, from http://www.york.ac.uk/depts/educ/research/ResearchPaperSeries/index.htm Saglam, M., & Millar, R. (2006). Upper High School Students’ Understanding of Electromagnetism. International Journal of Science Education. 28 (5), 543-566. Shipstone, D. M. (1984). A Study of Children’s Understanding of Electricity in Simple D.C. Circuits. European Journal of Science Education, 6, 185-198. Slotta, J. D., Chi, M.T.H., & Joram, E. (1995). Assessing students’ misclassifications of physics concepts: An ontological basis for conceptual change. Cognition and Instruction, 13, 373– 400. Smith, C. L., & Wenk, L. (2006). Relations among Three Aspects of First-Year College Students’ Epistemologies of Science. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 43(8), 747-785. Stavy, R, (1998). Special Issue: Conceptual Development in Science Education. International Journal of Science Education. 20(10), 1151-1154. Tao, P. K., & Gunstone R.F. (1999). The process of conceptual change in force and motion during computer-supported physics instruction. Journal of Research in Science Teaching 36(7), 859–882. Thagard, P. (1990). Concepts and conceptual change. Syntheses, 82, 255–274. Treagust, D., & Duit, R. (2008). Conceptual Change: A Discussion of Theoretical, Methodological and Practical Challenges for Science Education. Cultural Studies of Science Education, 3(2), 297-328. Valerie, V. (1997) 'Focus Groups: a useful qualitative method for educational research?’ British Educational Research Journal, 23(2), 209 – 224. Von Glasersfeld, E., & Steffe, L. P. (1991). Conceptual models in Educational research and practice. Journal of Educational Thought, 25(2), 91-103. Vosniadou, S. (2002). On the nature of naïve physics. In M. Limon & L. Mason (Eds.), Reconsidering conceptual change. Issues in theory and practice (pp. 61–76). Dordrecht, Netherlands: Kluwer Academic Publishers. Vosniadou, S, (2007). Conceptual Change and Education. Human Development, 50, 47-54. Vosniadou, S., & Brewer, W. F. (1987). Theories of knowledge restructuring in development. Review of Educational Research, 57(1), 51–67. Vosniadou, S., & Brewer, W.F. (1992). Mental models of the earth. A study of conceptual change in childhood. Cognitive Psychology, 24, 535-585. Zavala, G., & Alarcon, H. (2008, July 23-24). Evaluation of Instruction Using the Conceptual Survey of Electricity and Magnetism in Mexico. Paper presented at Physics Education Research Conference 2008, Edmonton, Canada. Retrieved December 1, 2010, from http://www.compadre.org/Repository/document/ServeFile.cfm?ID=8126&DocID=759 23 Categorization of Alternative Conceptions in Electricity and Magnetism Appendix: Focus Group Discussion Questions in Electric Potential & Energy Concepts 1. 2. 3. 4. What is electric potential? What is electric potential energy? What relationships between electric potential and electric field could exist? How would electric potential and potential energy be related? 24