Senechal, M., & Cornell, E. H. (1993). < active participation 01> Vocabulary acquisition through shared reading experiences. Reading Research Quarterly, 28 (4), 361-374. 001 Abstract The present study aims to investigate the effects of different reading conditions on children’s receptive and expressive vocabulary. Four adult reading conditions were utilized in the experiment. The four reading measures were the use of what-and where- questions, the use of recasts, the word repetition condition, and the verbatim reading condition. Eighty 4-and eighty 5- years-old children were recruited in the study. Results indicated that requesting active participation in the book-reading interactions did not boost children’s vocabulary learning; reading the book verbatim was just as effective as asking questions or recasting new vocabulary introduced in the book. However, the researchers found the effect of a single reading of a storybook on children’s receptive vocabulary. (L1) Design: 1 storybook Reading conditions:(a) the use of questions (b) the use of recasts (c) repetition (d) read the book verbatim Findings: The reading episode was sufficient to boost young learners’ receptive vocabulary and that the immediate test may have primed older children to acquire target words in extraexperimental contexts. Children’s acquisition was superior to the reasonable estimate of guessing. Also, children remembered more words than they forgot words. (after investigating the influence of remembering, forgetting, and reminiscence on their receptive vocabulary development) On the other hand, although the children failed to produce the target words, after the reading, they were able to produce appropriate synonyms for the target words. Moreover, they produced few inappropriate labels and produced few failures to respond. Multiple exposure of book reading is a must! Senechal, M. (1997). < active participation 02> The differential effect of storybook reading on preschoolers’ acquisition of expressive and receptive vocabulary. Cambridge University Press, 24, 123- 002 138. Abstract The present study was conducted to assess the effect of didactic techniques used during storybook reading on young children’s acquisition of new vocabulary introduced in storybooks. 30 three-year-old and 30 four-year-old children are included in the experiment. One storybook was utilized and three reading conditions were set up for the participants: single reading, repeated reading and questioning. In both the repeated reading and questioning conditions, the storybook was read three times. Results indicated that listening to multiple readings of a storybook facilitated children’s acquisition of expressive and receptive vocabulary, whereas answering questions during the multiple readings was more helpful to the acquisition of expressive than receptive vocabulary. (L1) Design: 1 storybook (the same with one that used in Senechal & Cornell, 1993) Testing: Pretest of receptive vocabulary, Posttests of receptive & expressive vocabulary Reading conditions: (a) single-reading (b) repeated-reading (c) questioning condition. Discussion Increased exposure of book reading enhanced children’s receptive and expressive vocabulary similarly whereas active responding during repeated book reading events enhanced children’ expressive vocabulary more than their receptive vocabulary. Also, adults’ reading behaviors may have different effects on children’s receptive and expressive vocabulary. Ewers. C. A., & Brownson, S. M. (1999). < active participation 03> Kindergarteners’ vocabulary acquisition as a function of active vs. passive storybook reading, prior vocabulary, and working memory. Journal of Reading Psychology, 20, 11-20. 003 Abstract The purpose of the current study is to investigate the effect of active versus passive storybook reading conditions on the vocabulary acquisition of kindergarteners who differed in level of prior vocabulary and phonological working memory. Results revealed that children with higher vocabulary knowledge acquired significantly more words than lower vocabulary peers; active participants acquired significantly more words than passive participants; and children with high versus low working memory did not differ in word acquisition. (L1) Design: 1 storybook Testing:(a) the Senechal Vocabulary Test-Adapted (b) Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test (PPVTR), and (c)the Children’s Test of Nonword Repetition (CNRep) to get the current proficiency (d) pretest (e) posttest of target words Reading conditions: (a) active storybook reading (b) passive storybook reading. Discussion Kindergarteners could acquire a significant number of new vocabulary words as a result of listening to a single storybook reading. Active participation would facilitates the learning of voca. Justice, L. M. (2002). Word exposure conditions and preschoolers’ novel word learning during shared storybook reading. Reading Psychology, 23, 87-106. < active participation 04> 004 Abstract The researcher designed an experiment in which 23 preschoolers participated in 2 adult-child shared storybook reading sessions over 1-week period. The purposes of the present study are, firstly, to characterize the influence of various conditions. Secondly, to investigate the effects of the two contrasting conditions. Results indicated that adults’ labeling of novel words facilitated children’s receptive word learning but not for expressive word learning. Also, results suggested that no differences in receptive or expressive word learning in terms of conceptual versus perceptual questions. ( L1) Design: Testing: (a) Novel Receptive Vocabulary, and (b) Novel Expressive Vocabulary Reading conditions: (a) questioning versus labeling of novel words, and (b) conceptual versus perceptual questions about novel words Discussion Adults’ labeling of novel words resulted in significantly greater gains in preschool children’s receptive learning of novel words. Also, both questioning and labeling did not exert positive effects regarding to the young children’s expressive learning of novel words. Last, nor did various types of adult questions exert differential effects on novel word learning by preschool children Senechal, M., Thomas, E., & Monker, J. A. (1997) <active participation 05> Individual differences in 4-year-old children’s acquisition of vocabulary during storybook reading. Journal of Educational Psychology, 87 (2), 218-229. Abstract 005 Two experiments were conducted to assess how children who differ in vocabulary knowledge learn new vocabulary from listening to storybooks. Participants were pretested for their prior word knowledge and classified as high or low word knowledge based on their Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-Revised (PPVT-R) standard scores. In experiment 1, children either listened passively or labeled pictures using novel words. Results shown that children with larger vocabularies produced more novel words than did children with smaller vocabularies, and children who answered questions during the book readings comprehended and produced more words than did children who passively listened to the story. In Experiment 2, participants either listened to readings of a book, pointed to pictures during the readings, or labeled pictures during the readings. Results indicated that children with larger vocabularies comprehended more novel words than did children with smaller vocabularies. Also, children who actively participated by labeling or pointing learned more words than did children who listened passively to book readings. Therefore, the results highlighted the importance of active participation during the readings, whether verbal or nonverbal responding, namely, labeling or pointing. (L1) Design: 2 storybooks, 2 exposures of book reading, delayed/ post-test of comprehension & production, 32 & 48 sub. Low & high word knowledge groups Discussion: an example of the Matthew Effects; active participation facilitates word learning (pointing or labeling); home reading experience is a key role in children’s vocabulary base Reading storybooks to kindergarteners helps them learn new vocabulary words. Journal of Educational Psychology, 86 (1), 54-64. Ehri, L. C, & Robbins, C. (1994) . 006 Abstract 33 five-year-old kindergarteners comprised the body of the subjects in this study. Two storybooks were used and were read twice to the participants. After the reading, the participants completed a posttest measuring their knowledge of the meanings if 22 unfamiliar words. Results indicated that children recognized the meanings of significantly more words from the story than words not in the story. Namely, storybook reading facilitated children’s learning of vocabulary words. The study results were another example of the Matthew Effect, which supported the phenomenon that the rich get richer and the poor get poorer. Moreover, four exposures to words appeared to be necessary but not sufficient for higher rates of word learning. (L1) .Design: Testing: 2 storybooks; multiple-choice posttest (22 words, 11 heard, 11 not-heard) 3 groups: low, middle, & high word knowledge Discussion: 1. an example of the Matthew Effects 2. multiple exposures of book reading is a must (in this study, 4 exposures are necessary but still not sufficient for learning all new words in a story, at least 2 exposures for enhancing word learning) Senechal, M., LeFevre, J. A., Hudson, E., & Lawson, E. P. (1996). Knowledge of storybooks as a predictor of young children’s vocabulary. Journal of Educational Psychology, 88 (3), 520-536. <Knowledge as a Predictor of Vocabulary 01> 007 Abstract In the present study, the researchers examined variables accounted for the children’s vocabulary acquisition such as frequency of storybook reading at home, frequency of reading requests during a typical week and so on. Additionally, parents’ education background, socioeconomic status, occupation and other potential influencing factors were also being taken into consideration in the research. In Experiment 1, parents’ knowledge of storybooks explained unique variance in children’s receptive vocabulary scores. In Experiment 2, children’s knowledge of storybook explained unique variance in their receptive and expressive vocabulary scores. The findings obtained in the present study indicate that storybook experiences during the preschool years may play a key role in children’s vocabulary development. Furthermore, a book-exposure checklist would be an alternative to self-reports of reading frequency. (L1) .Design: Ex.1: 119 children & parents Ex.2: 47 children & 50 parents Checklists: CTC (Children’s Title Checklist), CAC (Children’s Author Checklist), & AAC (Adults’ Author Ckl.) Discussion: 1. Book exposure plays a key role in enhancing young children’s vocabulary development. 2. Reading exposure checklists are relatively more reliable than parents’ self-report of children’s reading experiences. (without social desirability) 3. After controlling for other influencing factors (environmental & cognitive), storybook exposure (environmental variables) explains unique variance in children’s vocabulary knowledge Snow, C. A., & Goldfield, B. A. (1983) Turn the page please: situationspecific language acquisition. J. Child Lang., 10, 551-569. <mother-infant dyads 01> 008 Abstract The study investigated the effects of storybook reading on children’s language development via motherinfant dyads. The research results showed that specific lexical items and constructions used to talk about a picture frequently recurred in subsequent discussions, and that the child learned many of these same items and constructions. Also, the child was most likely to acquire what he had heard his mother say about a picture if he had repeated it in an earlier discussion. (L1) syntactic structures! .Design: one mother-child pair over a period of 11 months Age: 2.5 years old (to 3.4 years old) Discussion: 1. The recurrences of storybook reading and discussion did facilitate the child's language development. 2. In 37.7% of the discussions, the child initiated the conversation, indicating that he acquired the lexical items he needed through the reading experiences. (the capacity and willingness to initiate the discussion) 3. In this particular interactive context, the child utilized the strategy of saying what he had heard others say in precisely the same situation. * Book reading is a frequent and powerful source of language learning. * Book reading is an ideal and highly routinized activity. * Routinization of situations and predictability of adult utterances from situation crucial variables determining optimal usability of the linguistic environment. Ninio, A. (1983) Joint book reading as a multiple vocabulary acquisition device. Developmental Psychology, 19, 445-451. <mother-infant dyads 02> 009-1 Abstract The study investigated the effects of joint picture-book reading by 20 mother-infant dyads. Several labeling formats have been found from the content analysis, among them simple labeling by the mother or the infant, elicitation of labeling by “what-questions, “ elicitation of pointing by “where-questions,” and elicited and spontaneous imitation by the infant. The results imply that imitation, comprehension, and productive responses to words by vocabulary- learning infants do no represent different levels of word knowledge, and also that the respective vocabularies are overlapping at a given point in time. (EFL) Design: The study investigates vocabulary-teaching formats occurring in the context of joint picture-book reading by mother-infant dyads. It focuses on repeated discussions of the same words occurring within a single book-reading session of 15-min duration. 3 picture books are used in the study. Discussion: 1. At the stage, correct responses appear intermixed with errors, implying that labeling games concentrate on words in the process of being acquired rather than with fully mastered vocabulary items. 2. Comprehension and production of labels represent the same level of word knowledge—about 70% correct. Imitation occurs at a slightly lower level of knowledge—about 43% success rate. 3. Words that are imitated are slightly less well-known by the child than others. That is, imitation occurs literally on the threshold of acquisition. Following imitation, the success rate in producing and comprehending the same items approaches the 70% level. 4. Production, comprehension, and imitation represent alternative forms of rehearsal, with imitation more likely Ninio, A. (1983) Joint book reading as a multiple vocabulary acquisition device. Developmental Psychology, 19, 445-451. <mother-infant dyads 02> to occur for less well-learned, but already comprehended, items. 009-2 5. Labeling statements were preceded by a significantly lower proportion of correct responses than either whatquestions or where-questions. (20.8% for labeling statements, 67.4% for what-questions, 51% for wherequestions) 6. Mother exhibited a high degree of sensitivity to signals of word knowledge or lack of it in their children and chose their subsequent move accordingly. Mothers seemed to be motivated to impart labeling information only if the children appeared not to know the word; otherwise, they attempted to elicit production or recognition of the word from the children. Snow, C.A., & Ninio, A. (1986) The contracts of literacy: What children learn from listening to read books. In W. H. Teale, & E. Sulzby (Eds.), Emergent literacy: Writing and Reading (pp.116-138). Norwood, NJ: Ablex. <mother-infants dyads 3> 010 Abstract With complementary lines of research, Snow & Ninio turned their attention completely to the contributions of book reading to the child’s literacy development and discuss how children learn the “contracts of literacy,” the basic rules related to the use of books and the meaning of texts. (L1) Design: mother-child dyads infant at their initial stage of language learning (one: at his early language acquisition and the other was 8 months old) Discussion: *The 7 contracts: 1) Books are for reading, not for manipulating; (2) In book reading, the book is in control and the readeros led; (3) Pictures are not things but representatives of things; (4) Pictures are for naming; (5) Pictures can represent events; (6) Book events occur outside real time; (7) Books constitute an autonomous fictional world Whitehurst, G. J. et al. (1988). Accelerating Language Development 011 Through Picture Book Reading. Developmental Psychology, 24, 552-559. <mother-infants dyads 4> Abstract : The study assessed the effects of joint book reading between mothers and infants. Mothers of the experimental group, after receiving a less than 1 hour reading training, followed the instructions in their reading yet mothers in the control read in their customary fashion. Results showed that the experimental outperformed their counterparts on standardized posttests of expressive language ability. Also, the experimental had a higher MLU (mean length of utterance), frequency of phrases, and a lower frequency of single words. Follow-up 9 months showed differences between the two groups. (L1) Design: mother-child dyads / 1 month duration The average age: 1.75~2.91 years old Discussion: Variations in reading to young children can have appreciable effects on language development. Results from posttests and follow-up on PPVT, EOWPVT, and ITPA indicated that children in the experimental group had a more positive results than ones in the control after being read to by their mothers under the requirements of using open-ended questions, function/attribute questions, and expansions; to respond appropriately to children’s attempts to answer these questions; and to decrease their frequency of straight reading and questions that could be answered by pointing. Moreover, the experimental showed higher MLU, frequency of phrases, and a lower frequency of single words. PPVT: Peaboby Picture Vocabulary Test EOWPT: Expressive One Word Picture Vocabulary Test ITPA: Illinois Test of Psycholinguistic Abilities Arnold, D. H. et al. (1994). Accelerating Language Development Through Picture 012 Book Reading: Replication and Extension to a Videotape Training Format. Journal of Educational Psychology, 86, 235-243. <mother-infants dyads 5> Abstract: As a replication of Whitehurst et al (1988)’s study, the present study assessed the effects of joint book reading between mothers and infants using a videotape training program. The intervention program, called dialogic reading, produced substantial effects on preschool children’s language development. Concerning the costs of the reading training program, the authors developed and evaluated an inexpensive videotape training package for teaching dialogic reading techniques. Results supported the conclusions of Whitehurst et al.: Dialogic reading had powerful effects on children’s language skills and indicated that videotape training provide a cost-effective, standardized means of implementing the program. (L1) Design: mother-child dyads / 5 weeks duration/ 64 children, 27 in the control, 23 the direct, 14 in the video The average age: 2~2. 83 years old Discussion: As a replication of Whitehurst, G. J. et al. (1988), the results also found that variations in reading to young children can have appreciable effects on language development. Besides, with the cost-effective concern of the application, the researchers utilized a videotape training program to provide parents with principles of reading storybook aloud. As expectation, the research results confirmed the effects of reading stories to young children did facilitate their vocabulary acquisition. Moreover, with the emphasis of eliciting production from children, the storytelling evokes children’s productive vocabulary than their receptive vocabulary. In other word, the series of dialogic reading studies demonstrates that large effects on children’s language can obtained by a storytime intervention that encourages children to talk about picture books and provides appropriate language feedback and models. Akhtar, N., Jipson, J., & Callanen, M. A. (2001). Learning words from 013 overhearing. Child Development, 72, 416-430. Abstract : Three studies examined 2 year-olds’ ability to learn novel words when overhearing these words used by others. The results found that children aged 2.6 were equally good at learning novel words, both object labels and action verbs when they were overhearers as when they were directly addressed. But for younger 2 year-olds, this was true for object labels than for action verbs. The research highlighted the active role played by toddlers in vocabulary acquisition. (L1) Design: 3 studies (1A/1B2.6 year-olds, 2A/2B young 2 year-olds, 3 younger 2 year-olds with a modified procedure (provided chances of knowing the action prop first, experiences with the action verbs) 2 groups: 1overhearing, 1 addressed The average age: 2.4~2.8 years old Discussion: The research results indicated that by age 2.6, children are capable of learning novel words through overhearing. (both nouns and verbs) Moreover, younger 2 year-olds show the ability of acquiring novel labels but not action verbs through overhearing. For obvious reasons, younger 2 year-olds seemed not to learn action verbs through overhearing. Reflection: Context plays a critical role in children’s language acquisition. Only in meaningful social interaction could children absorb language naturally and substantially. Also, the research findings reveal the fact that toddlers can acquire words through meaningful literacy event even as an overhearer and this, to me, may provide an explanation why children would absorb new words through storytelling. Smith, H, & Higgins, S. (2006). Opening classroom interaction: the importance of feedback. Cambridge Journal of Education, 36, 485502. 014 Abstract : This article uses evidence gathered from a large-scale research project examining classroom interactions during literacy and numeracy lessons, and the researchers’ critical reflections upon this process, to examine conceptions of interactive pedagogy. In order to “open” classroom interaction, emphasis should be on the manner with which teachers’ react to pupils’ responses to questions. Episodes of classroom interaction from video recorded literacy and numeracy lessons taken as part of the study are used to support this argument. Findings: It is not whether a question is factual, closed or open or even the degree of openness and authenticity with which it is phrased, the act of asking questions itself, or the types of questions teachers ask, which determines classroom interactivity. Rather it is how a teacher reacts to pupils’ responses in terms of their feedback and the historical precedence of the perception of teacher intent this engenders. Types of feedback moves which appear to facilitate pupils’ subsequent use of talk for thinking and learning. (1) quality feedback moves: to encourage a more conversational and symmetrical interaction, (2) encouraging peer-peer feedback: by asking pupils to review one another’s work, rephrasing the pupils’ ideas etc., which may enhance selfevaluation, (3) reciprocal engagement: to signal an authentic interest in what the pupils were saying and an implicit cue prompting their continuance, (4) following pupils’ ideas: by taking pupils seriously, acknowledging and building on what they said, which encouraged pupils to have ownership not only of the solution of the problems, but also the flow of the lesson. Notes: (1) the feedback move: I-R-F initiation-response-feedback (2) reason for using a video camera:: paralinguistic and non-linguistic features of talk and the use of the board as a pedagogical tool were captured. The recordings were used to identify not just the function of teacher and pupil discourse, and “broad patterns in the distribution of talk,” but also the content of what was actually said and the manner in which it was spoken.--> the whole picture! Cornell, E. H., Senechal, M., & Broda, L. S. (1988). Recall of picture books by 3 year-old children: Testing and repetition in joint reading activities. Journal of Educational Psychology, 80, 537-542. 015 Abstract : The study probed the effects of rereading and testing for recall on children’s knowledge of content of the given texts. An additional between-subject variable was whether the researcher or the child pointed to items to be remembered; the different books served as a within-subject variable. The dependent variable measure was memories of book items when the child was asked to anticipate events from page to page. There was little evidence that these memories were strengthened by the child’s pointing during reading, but testing for recall and rereading the book were found to be effective for teaching the content. (L1) (72 three-year-old children, duration: N/A) Findings: The results indicated that rereading and testing boosted children’s knowledge of content of picture-books. More importantly, once the book items were recalled and confirmed, children rarely forgot them. However, pointing was not as effective for boosting recall of book items as hypothesized in this current study. Namely, recall of pic. books was boosted by a single rereading of the books, by testing for memories of book items, and by probing with different reminders. Discussions: (1) testing effects seen in the study: testing serves to (1) retrieve new items and (2) correct items that are recalled in the wrong context. (2) (2) lack of an effect of pointing: investigations involving different ages, book formats, and dependent measures should be considered. Moreover, pointing may be instrumental for other aspects of language learning such as vocabulary acquisition.(Ninio, 1983) (3) certain pic. books facilitate certain aspects of book learning. The researchers urged that the literature seldom acknowledge that the child’s competence and the parents’ techniques may be shaped by the book in hand.(Effie: the importance of book selection) (4) children’s intrusion reflect knowledge of content. The book that yielded more recall yielded less incorrect guessing, less failure to respond to initial probes but more intrusions. Children are willing to name items that they knew appeared somewhere in the book. the importance to identify the memorable characteristics of favorite books (Robinson & Sulzby, 1984). Whitehurst, G. J., Arnold, D. S., Epstein, J. N., Angell, A. L., Smith, M., & Fischel, J. E. (1994) A picture book reading intervention in day care and home for 016-1 children from low-income families. Developmental Psychology, 30, 679-689. Abstract : The study investigated the effect of an interactive book reading program with children from lowincome families who attended subsidized day-care center in New York. Children were pretested and assigned randomly within classrooms to 1 of conditions: (a) a school plus home condition in which the children were read to by their teachers and their parents, (b) a school condition in which children were read to only by teachers, and (c) a control condition in which children engaged in play activities under the supervision of their teachers. The intervention lasted for 6 weeks, at which point children were posttested on several standardized measures of language ability that had been used as pretests. These assessments were repeated at a 6-month follow-up. Statistically and educationally significant effects of the reading intervention were obtained at posttests and follow-up on measures of expressive vocabulary. (n= 73 three year-old L1 children) Findings: Children in the reading conditions gained approximately double the number of words between pretests and posttests than the counterparts in the control. Interestingly, from the correlational findings, number of books in the home and the child’s enjoyment of shared reading contributed to children’ s performance on the language assessment. The research results showed that book reading can be a practical intervention for preschoolers from low-income backgrounds.(with readers are day care center teachers or parents other than highly educated adults) Discussion: (1) The principles underlying dialogic reading suggest that children benefit from active responding to pic.books in a setting in which an adult gently pushes the child, through questions, expansions, and sensitivity to the child’s interests and abilities. Whitehurst, G. J., Arnold, D. S., Epstein, J. N., Angell, A. L., Smith, M., & Fischel, J. E. (1994) A picture book reading intervention in day care and home for children from low-income families. Developmental Psychology, 30, 679-689. 016-2 Discussion: (2) The model that underlies the research is that dialogic reading and related activities during the preschool period enhance language and preliteracy skills, which in turn help children in learning to read and other academic tasks when they begin school. Note: In day care, teachers read to a small group of nor more than 5 children at a time. Oeizman, Z. O., & Snow, C. (2001). Lexical Input as Related to Children’s Vocabulary Acquisition: Effects of Sophisticated Exposure and Support for 017 Meaning, 37, 265-279. Abstract : The study aims to investigate the relationship between early lexical input and vocabulary acquisition among in low-income children. With 53 children recruited in the study, totally 263 mother-child conversations in 5 settings were studied. Children’s vocabulary performance in kindergarten and later in 2nd grade related more to the occurrence of sophisticated lexical items than to quantity of lexical input overall. Density of sophisticated words heard and the density with which such words were embedded in helpful or instructive interactions, at age 5 at home, independently predicted over a third of the variance in children’s voca. performance in both kindergarten and 2nd grade. Design: 5 settings: toy-play, mealtime, magnet play, information book reading, storybook reading The average age: 53 low-income mother & child, 5 years old, L1 (mother-child dyads) Findings: Study results were two folded. Firstly, there’re profound quantitative and qualitative differences in early voca. exposure among low-income preschoolers. Secondly, a powerful linkage b/w early exposure to sophisticated words during mealtime and playtime conversations and later voca. performance was found. Study results indicated that age 5 PPVT-R score absorbs most of the variance in the age 7 PPVT-R score. This suggests that the relationships carried over into the early school years. Discussion: The present research indicated the importance of the occurrence of sophisticated low-frequency words at a child’s early literacy development. More importantly, it is particularly imperative for children in lowincome family. Also, the importance of providing interactive contexts that support the utility of sophisticated language input was highlighted. Accordingly, the researchers suggest that descriptions of vocabulary input must involve 3 fundamental elements: lexical quantity, lexical sophistication, and conversational support, in which instructive or helpful interactions would be seen. Lomax, C. L. (1977) Interest in books and stories at nursery school. Educational Research, 19, 100-112. 018-1 Abstract : The study aims to investigate children’s interest in books and stories at nursery school. 28 preschool children were compared by means of observations, tests, interviews with parent about activities at home, and questionnaires for staff about activities at school. Design: The subjects were divided into 2 groups: high-interest group & low-interest group. At the first stage, the children were observed for two non-consecutive days to decide who has high interests in books and vice versa. At the second stage, both Story Group (averagely spent 22.5 min in the books areas) and Control Group (spent only 4 mins) was observed continuously for a period of 100 minutes. The average age: 3 and 4 years old, L1 Findings: Significances were found in the study. Firstly, results from the first stage showed that story telling and looking at books, taken together, were among the most commonly observed activities in the nursery school. (there’s a dearth of research studies in giving information about children’s activities in nursery schools.) Secondly, the girls tended to be using the book corner for longer periods than the boys. Thirdly, there’s big difference in time spent between the two groups. CG spent more time on ‘nonspecific activity’ and SG stayed focus on ‘any’ activity for a much longer time than their counterpart. Moreover, SG spent contrastively long time on listening to a story (12.72 min) from an adult and CG only a very short period of time (4.43 mins). Next, parents of SG reported that their children had a story from an adult at home significantly more often than parents of CG. Besides, parents of SG reported that their children painted more often than peers of CG, while children of CG were reported to pretend to be an ‘action figure’ more often than the Story children. Furthermore, children of SG listened to a story from an adult were more frequent their peers at the nursery school. Also, the frequency of using construction toys of SG was higher than CG at the nursery school. Lomax, C. L. (1977) Interest in books and stories at nursery school. Educational Research, 19, 100-112. 018-2 Findings: On the other hand, all children in the SG were reported as having a story form an adult at home at least once a week, and in most cases every day. Eight of the 12 CG children also had stories from an adult for at least once a week, though one mother commented that her child received reading aloud no more than once a week. The other four Comparison children had stories from adults at home less often than this, in two cases less often than once a month. When it comes to the liking or disliking of playing alone, CG children disliked to play alone compared the Story children most of whom were seen as liking to this or even liking it very much. By contrast the CG children were mostly reported as liking to play with other young children very much compared with the Story children who merely liked this situation of in many cases disliked it. Interestingly enough, the association between age and apparent interests in books and stories for boys was also found in a longitudinal study undertaken at another nursery school as part of the same project (Lomax, unpublished research). Nursery nurses kept records of children’s interests in various play activities over a two-year period. Most of the boys in the study were reported as showing interests in books corner at age five but not at age three; most of the girls, on the other hand, were reported as already showing interests by the age of three. Last, CG children engaged in most nursery school activities more often than the SG but generally attended to these activities for shorted periods and spent a significantly greater proportion of their time doing nothing in particular. Lomax, C. L. (1977) Interest in books and stories at nursery school. Educational Research, 19, 100-112. 018-3 Discussion: The findings of the research were very critical and highly influencing. As in expectation, the researcher indicated that many instances in the investigation showed that the children has a strong liking for repetition, which was again supported the common belief that young children love repetition of hearing the same books again and again once they love that given story. Moreover, the introduction of the ‘jukebox’ system which the children could operate themselves would not only be interesting form a research point of view but might help to satisfy the children’s evident desire for stories to be repeated several times. Also, the small number of children present at any one time for a story suggests that there would be plenty of opportunity for individual attention and child-adult interaction in this situation. Speaker, K. M., Taylor, D., & Kamen, R. (2004). Storytelling: Enhancing language acquisition in young children. Education, 125, 3-14. 019 Abstract: The research aims to investigate the effects of listening to stories on preschoolers’ language acquisition. In details, it is to assess the qualitative changes in verbal fluency in four aspects: vocabulary, grammatics, MLU, and sentence formation. 5 subjects were enrolled in the study. The subjects received pretests and posttests on Brown’s Stages of Language Development to probe the acquisition of syntax (MLU) and Templin’s Type of Token Ratio to study the word knowledge (semantic content). Data collection was also from the language samples to analyze the four aspects of language. The questions used were open-ended, thought provoking and intentionally written at eh higher stages of Bloom’s taxonomy. Each child’s language development was analyzed through unstructured (free play) and structured activities (narratives) using sequential picture cards and themed pictures before and after the storytelling program. Design: a qualitative research, 4 weeks & 10 books The average age: 3, 4, and 5years old, L1 Findings: The study results showed that each of the five preschool children displayed improvement of language skills after engaging in the rigorous storytelling program. While the level of overall progress varied in vocabulary, grammatics and sentence formation, significant gains in MLU can be noted. The increased use of elaboration and the use of complex sentences can also be seen in the post intervention stage in the participants. These findings reiterated that increased exposure to storytelling would foster emergence of more advanced stages of different aspects, here, with grammatics, vocabulary, length of utterance, and sentence formation, of language development in preschoolers. Discussion: The MLU corresponds roughly to the child’s chronological age, in which case an MLU of 3.94 would roughly correspond to a 4 year-old child. The flaws of the study: the way of storytelling was deficient. (N/A) and the study duration seemed too short (with only 4 weeks). Wood, E., Pressley, M., Turnure, J., & Walton, R. (1987). Enriching children’s recall of picture-dictionary definitions with interrogation and elaborated pictures. ECTJ, 35, 43-52. 020 Abstract: The hypothesis of this study was that preschoolers’ learning of vocabulary-meaning associations could be improved by interventions that increased the likelihood of extensive, elaborated processing during the study of vocabulary words and their definitions. Subjects were randomly put into 4 conditions: questioned & elaborated/ nonquestioned & elaborated/ questioned& nonelaborated, and nonquestioned & nonelaborated. Design: 4 conditions (questioned(1) showed the pictures (2) read by the experimenter the short definitional paragraph (3) ask questions & nonquestioned(1) showed the pictures (2) read by the experimenter the short definitional paragraph (3) declarative sentences) The average age: eighty 41 months ~ 69 months preschoolers Findings: The study results showed that firstly questioned subjects performed better in total recall that was paraphrased than nonquestioned counterparts but were less likely to recall the exact word wording of the text, consists with the hypothesis that questioning would produce more extensive processing of the definitions. Secondly, the presentation of elaborated pictures resulted in more congruent recall than the presentation of the less complex pictures. That is, the elaborated pictures might also produce deeper processing than did the nonelaborated illustrations, for subjects who viewed elaborated pictures were more likely to recall congruent information at testing. Finally, this present study made it clear that preschool children’s memory of a definition can be made richer, with pictorial and interrogative variables making unique contributions to recall. The study emphasized the importance of pictures and questions in reading (and reading aloud) to children in terms of their language development. Linse, C. (2006). Predictable books in the children’s EFL classroom. ELT Journal, 61, 46-54. 021 Abstract: The article explores the use of predictable books in EFL to help develop both their oral language and literacy development. Furthermore, it explores the range of language found in predictable books and discusses the suitability of this material for young children learning in EFL. Discussion: (1) repetition is important of the learning. Many folktales are embedded with this feature such as Goldilocks, Little Red Hen, and Three Little Pigs. Also, folktales were originally told orally in a social context. The social environment of the story can assist in providing scaffolding as is the case when adults engage in telling stories to children that use repetitive and predictable language. Last, repetition would provide linguistic support as a young child developed concepts of language and narration. (2) One of the most precious moments that teachers share with their very young students is story time when they crowd around to see the pictures and listen to a tale read or told for the first or twentieth time. (3) Predictable story is a popular type of storybooks that often used by native English teachers. Predictable books contain illustrations that help to clarify or support the word, sentence, or pattern that is repeated in the text. Also, predictable books are considered to be an educationally sound way of easing emergent readers into reading authentic literature. (“These predictable features give emergent readers the sensation of reading and help them make associations between spoken and written words (Popp, 1996)). Predictable book like Brown Bear, Brown Bear, what do you see? (4) the advantages of reading predictable books: illustrate specific aspects of grammar, provide content (using controlled patterns). For example, Polar Bear, Polar Bear, what do you hear? (repetitive structures and features factual content). Next, it can teach children pronunciation, help them develop writing skills (it is important to use books that children are familiar with and are at or slightly above children’s level of language acquisition.) Linse, C. (2006). Predictable books in the children’s EFL classroom. ELT Journal, 61, 46-54. 021-1 Discussion: When search for a predictable book for reading aloud to young EFL learners, it is important to find tales that are of interests and are linguistically accessible to beginning language learners. To decide whether a book is suitable, some rules might help. For example, the structures in a specific book may be familiar to the learners, although the vocabulary may be new. The repetition of text, combined with illustrations, is ideal for use in the young EFL learners. Note: the definition of predictable books: a core structure is repeated over and over again. Lonigan, C. J.,& Whitehurst, G. J. (1998). Relative efficacy of parent and teacher involvement in a shared-reading intervention for preschool children from lowincome backgrounds. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 13, 263-290. 022-1 Abstract: The effects of shared-reading intervention were evaluated with 3 to 4 years old children from low-income families. The children were pretested and assigned to 1 of 4 conditions: no treatment, school condition, home condition, school plus home condition. The intervention was conducted for 6 weeks, after the experiment, the children were posttested on standardized measures of oral language and language samples were obtained during a shared-reading assessment. Also, the children received follow-up 6 months. (L1, 91 children) Design:4 (conditions)x 2 (centers: high & low compliance) ANCOVA/ shared-reading sessions were scheduled to occur daily for approximately 10 minutes per reading group/ 6 books/ Assessments: PPVT/EOWPVT(names for pic.)/ ITPA-VE (verbal fluency)/ verbal production(answer open-ended questions to see children’s spontaneous speech growth) Discussion: The results obtained under a more naturalistic measure of language. Findings: The ANCOVA on the EOWPVT at posttest revealed a significant effect of intervention group and the group by center compliance interaction. In the high compliance centers, the combined intervention groups scored significantly higher than the control group, and none of the intervention groups differ from each other. The school plus home group scored higher than the control group, and there was a trend for the school group to score higher than the control group. Lonigan, C. J.,& Whitehurst, G. J. (1998). Relative efficacy of parent and teacher involvement in a shared-reading intervention for preschool children from lowincome backgrounds. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 13, 263-290. 022-2 Findings: The ANCOVA on the ITPA-VE at posttest revealed a significant effect of intervention group. The combination of all three intervention groups scored higher than the control group. Scores in the home group were higher than scores in either the school group or the school plus home group. School for the home group were higher than scores in the control group. The ANCOVA on the PPVTR at posttest revealed no significant effects. Only 66 of the 91 children who were available for the semi-structured reading intervention assessment to gain verbal production data. Concerning the high and low compliance centers, significant effects of group were found for children’s MLU, the total number of words produced, the number of different words produced, the number of different nouns produced, and the number of different adjective/ modifiers produced. Planned comparisons revealed that the combined intervention groups produced longer utterances, produced more words overall, produced a higher diversity of words, and produced more adjectives/ modifiers than the control group. And there were minimum differences between the intervention groups. In the low compliance centers, there was a significant effect of group for the number of different words produced; however, neither the combined intervention groups nor any individual intervention group differed from the control group. Children’s verbal production assessment showed that in the high compliance centers, significant effects of group were found for the total number of words produced, the number of different words produced, and the number of different verbs produced. Planned comparisons indicated that the combined intervention groups produced more words overall, produced a higher diversity of words, and produced Lonigan, C. J.,& Whitehurst, G. J. (1998). Relative efficacy of parent and teacher involvement in a shared-reading intervention for preschool children from lowincome backgrounds. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 13, 263-290. 022-3 more verbs than the control group. Children in high compliance centers scored higher on all of these variables, and the effects were due to differences between centers in the school plus home and home groups. (Reading frequency!) Discussion: The results obtained under a more naturalistic measure of language. Both the teachers and parents can produce significant positive changes in the development of low-income children using a relatively brief dialogic reading intervention. The effects were apparent on two standardized tests of expressive language (EOWPVT & ITPA-VE). These results add important data to the theoretical and empirical controversy concerning the role of shared-reading activities in the development of oral language and other emergent literacy skills. Also, the effects on structural aspects of spontaneous speech were relatively large. Within high compliance centers, children who were exposed to dialogic reading at both home and school appeared to be benefit more than those exposed just at home or just at school. The two school groups resulted in the largest gains in expressive vocabulary suggests that teachers may focus on teaching specific age appropriate vocabulary. In contrast to the results of vocabulary development, parents appear to be more influential in increasing their children’s descriptive use of language. Results for the ITPA-VE were stronger in the home group than in either the school or the school plus home group. (reading frequency! The nature of school versus home reading one on one reading & small group reading) The relationship between dialogic reading and Vygotsky’s (1978) ZPD was discussed. Impressively, the researchers found that after the study, several parents reported that because of the experience, both they & their children enjoyed shared-reading and may ask whether they could get more books. Note: high compliance center: conduct the intervention with a high frequency and vice versa. Horner, S. L. (2004). Observational learning during shared book reading: The effects on preschoolers’ attention to print and letter knowledge. Reading Psychology, 25, 167-188. 023-1 Abstract: The study aimed to investigate the effects of observational learning one preschoolers’ use of a questioning technique, attention to print, and knowledge of the alphabet. The hypotheses were, children who observe a model will ask questions ask more questions. Also, children who observe a model ask print-related questions will make more print-related response. Last, children who make print-related responses will learn more target letter names. Design: 62 preschoolers, 28 three-year-old; 28 four-year-old; 6 five-year-old/ 4 treatments: print questions videotape, picture questions videotapes, no questions videotape, no videotape control/ pretests (to decide who could participate in the study) and posttest/ 2 book readings (alphabet book) *12% of the children enrolled in the program do not speak English while 88% speak English Findings: Firstly, both the print questions and picture questions groups asked significantly more questions than did the no questions and no videotape control groups. Specifically, children who observed a model asked 15 times more questions during BR1 and 30 times more questions during BR2 than children who did nor observe a model. Also, it has found that the print questions videotape group focused on the individual letter more often than the children in any other group. That is, children in the print questions condition commented on the letters approximately 8 times more than the children in the other conditions. However, results failed to support the hypothesis that preschoolers who made letter-focused responses would learn more target letters names than children who did not make letter-focused responses within shared book-reading sessions. The number of letterfocused responses was positively correlated with pretest letter-name knowledge for all 26 letters. Horner, S. L. (2004). Observational learning during shared book reading: The effects on preschoolers’ attention to print and letter knowledge. Reading Psychology, 25, 167-188. 023-2 Namely, those children who knew more letters at the beginning of this study imitated the model’s focus on the individual letter more than children who knew fewer letters. Twice as many children who knew most letters commented on the print than children who knew few letters. Also, the two children who knew most letters (25 &26) read most of the pages before the researcher did. Discussion: The possible reasons that observational learning did not support children’ letter-name knowledge might be “that letters are graphically abstract and highly confusable, learning the names of letters is not an easy task.” (Adams, 1990; Ehri, 1983) For further discussion, see the paper. The influence of home reading or preschool reading plays a key role in young children’s letter-name knowledge. Children who are frequently read to at home or at preschool may learn behavioral patterns more readily within a book-reading episode than children who are read to less frequently. Whitehurst, G. J., Epstein, J. N., Angell, A. L., Payne, A. C., Crone, D. A., & Fischel, J. E. (1994). Outcomes of an emergent literacy intervention in Head Start. Journal of Educational Psychology, 86, 542-555. 024-1 Abstract: The research aims to investigate the effects of an emergent literacy intervention to further explore the relations between the intervention and its outcomes. The participants were pretested and posttested on standardized tests of language, writing, linguistic awareness, and print concepts and were randomly assigned to 2 conditions: intervention, involving an add-on emergent literacy curriculum, or a control, involving the regular Head Start curriculum. (Intervention: interactive book reading at home & school plus a classroom-based sound and letter awareness program) The hypothesis was that the combination of dialogic reading and sound foundations would enhance language, linguistic awareness, and print knowledge when introduced into intervention classrooms and compared with the performance of children in control classroom. Design: 167 four-year-old children, 2 conditions, pretests: the PPVT-M, the Expressive One Word Picture Vocabulary Test, ITPA (a test of verbal fluency), and DSC (the Developing Skills Checklist but it’s not a checklist); posttests: the same measurements in alternative forms if it’s available/ duration: 30 weeks with 30 books; 4 emergent literacy skills have been scrutinized: language, writing, linguistic awareness, print concept. Findings: The MANCOVA analysis resulted in significant main effects for intervention versus control. That is, children in the intervention group performed a significantly higher level than did children in the control condition on the Writing factor and the Language factor. The effect sizes of .516 for the Writing factor and .624 for the Print Concepts factor fall into Cohen’s medium effect size category, indicating educationally meaningful effects (e.g. Lee, Brooks-Gunn, Schnur, & Liaw, 1990). In particular, there’s a significant effect of the intervention on the Identify Sounds and Letters subtest although there’s no overall effect on the Linguistic Awareness factor on which this subtest loads most highly. Whitehurst, G. J., Epstein, J. N., Angell, A. L., Payne, A. C., Crone, D. A., & Fischel, J. E. (1994). Outcomes of an emergent literacy intervention in Head Start. Journal of Educational Psychology, 86, 542-555. 024-2 Also, the only significant effect for sex was that girls performed at a higher level than did boys on the Writing factor. And boys performed better than girls in Language factor. More importantly, the researchers indicated that effects on language were limited to children whose parents were actively involved in the at-home reading program, which stressed the critical influence of home reading experience in children regarding to their language growth. Other research also drew such conclusions (e.g. Lonigan & Whitehurst, 1998). Accordingly, the Whitehurst et al. maintained that children from low-income families need frequent one-on-one language interactions with an adult to enhance their language skills. Group-based interactions may not be sufficient in the late preschool years, even if the groups are small and the forms of interaction are optimized. Still, frequency of exposure may be the most straightforward explanation of why the home is a more powerful venue fro language growth than school. (home environment is critical! On the other hand, teachers, to achieve the goal of increasing children’s language development, have to increase the frequency of reading.) Note: CROWD: Completion prompts, Recall prompts, Open-ended prompts, Wh-prompts, Distancing promts (e.g. Did you ever play in the snow like Peter did?) It was found that open-ended prompts and distancing prompts were the least question type teachers in both experiment & control groups utilized. (adult readers should use the five types of questions in the reading) PEER: prompt the child to respond to the book, evaluate the child’s response, expand the child’s response by repeating or adding information to it, and encourage the child to repeat the expanded utterance (remind the adult reader to use CROWD in the reading) Thoreson, C. C., & Dale, P. S. (1992). Do early talkers become early readers? Linguistic precocity, preschool language, and emergent literacy. Developmental Psychology, 28, 421-429. 025-1 Abstract: The aim of the longitudinal study is to examine whether verbal precocious children are also precocious in reading. 25 children at 20 months of age were recruited in the study. The study was to relate linguistically precocious children’s early verbal skills, parent-child interaction patterns, and instructional experiences to their later language and literacy skills. Design: 3 classes of predictor variables: measures of cognitive and language ability administered at age 24 months; a measure of child engagement during story reading at age 24 months; and parental reports of child exposure to literacy collected when children were 24 months and 4 1/2 years. Two classes of outcome variables: language and cognitive measures at ages 2 1/2 and 4 1/2 and literacy measures at age 4 1/2. Findings: Conclusion that verbally precocious children are not likely to be early readers is correlational evidence that language and literacy are separable abilities at 4 1/2 years in this group of children. Moreover, children engagement in the story-reading episode showed greater predictive validity than either proportion or frequency of specific parental utterance functions (e.g. questions, responses, simplifications) or that of child utterance functions. No effects of type of questions were found in the study. (e.g. Justice (2002) also reached the same conclusion) Story-reading engagement is moderately related to frequency of story reading at home. Exposure to instruction in letter names and sounds was a significant predictor of children’s knowledge of print conventions, invented spelling, and a nearly significant predictor of decoding skill but not of phonological awareness, which is predicted by MLU at 24 months. Three of the four literacy outcomes were predicted significantly or nearly significantly by at least one of the two measures of story reading. The two story-reading variables predicted different aspects of literacy, suggesting that these predictors capture different aspects of Thoreson, C. C., & Dale, P. S. (1992). Do early talkers become early readers? Linguistic precocity, preschool language, and emergent literacy. Developmental Psychology, 28, 421-429. 025-2 Exposure to literate activity. Engagement in the story-reading explained a significant portion of the variance in the Concepts of Print outcome measure. Frequency of story reading in the home explained significant portions of the variance in both PIAT (a measure of reading ability) and the phonological awareness outcome measure. Story-reading frequency or story-reading engagement (but not both simultaneously) significantly predicts syntactic comprehension at 2 1/2 years (PPVT-R). When the children grew older, it was found that either, but not both, of the variables significantly predicts performance on the test of real-world knowledge, and the story engagement is also a significant predictor of the test of analytic ability. Besides, the results indicated that there’s stability of individual differences within the sample: 24-month PPVTR scores are correlated with 4 1/2-year PPVT-R scores. Moreover, as the results showed, the verbal precocious children did not show substantial evidence in reading precocity, which was against the hypothesis. Also, the researcher indicated that, in terms of individual differences, early language measures were not significant predictors of literacy outcomes. More importantly, story reading with parents was found to be a role in literacy and language development beyond that played by reading instruction and early language ability. Namely, story reading is an important way in which parents prepare their children to become readers (e.g. Wells, 1985b). It is striking to find such a storing relationship between story-reading variables and language outcomes even within a group selected for linguistic precocity. This relationship is borne out with measures of vocabulary development, syntactic development, and the development of real-world knowledge. These results also provided further evidence for the hypothesis that what is learned from books changes with development. At 2 1/2 years, the child’s focus is vocabulary and syntax acquisition; therefore, story-reading exposure predicts vocabulary and Thoreson, C. C., & Dale, P. S. (1992). Do early talkers become early readers? Linguistic precocity, preschool language, and emergent literacy. Developmental Psychology, 28, 421-429. 025-3 Syntactic knowledge 6months later. At age 4 1/2, these highly capable children were no longer learning labels from books; rather, they were focusing on more complex linguistic and cognitive constructs. (the developmental framework, p. 428) The story reading with parents is fueling the growth of knowledge at the “leading edge” of the child’s development. Still, the researchers claimed that measures of child engagement proved superior at capturing the quality of the interaction over measures of parental behavior. Last, the story-engagement variable was nor correlated with concurrent child ability measures but was predictive of language, cognitive, and literacy outcomes. The data suggest that children who have more opportunities to read stories with parents and who take advantage of such opportunities through engagement learn something from story reading about both language and literacy. This conclusion is consistent with experimental work showing that story reading can directly influence the growth of language skills (Whitehurst et al., 1988). Furthermore, frequency of story reading in the home and child engagement in a story reading episode at age 24 months were significant predictors of children’s language ability at 2 1/2 and 4 1/2 and knowledge of print conventions at age 4 1/2. It is concluded that story reading with parents as well as literacy instruction contributes to the development of emergent literacy in verbally precocious children. Nagy & Herman (1987) Learning word meanings from context during normal reading. American Educational Research Journal, 24, 237-270. 026-1 Abstract: The study investigated incidental learning of vocabulary from context during normal reading. The researchers hypothesized that learning form context typically takes place in small increments, so that any one encounter with a word usually results in only a small gain in knowledge of that word. Design: Three hundred and fifty-two students in third, fifth, and seventh grades. Narrative or expository passages. Totally 418 children; with 157 in third grade, 100 in fifth grade, and 161 in seventh grade. Checklist reading multiple choice test (1 week later) Findings: Children at all three grade levels gained substantial knowledge about an unknown word form a single exposure. Most of the target words occurred only once in a text and the target words were not highlighted in any way. The students ranged widely in age and reading ability. Results indicated that significant effect of learning from context has been found. Multiple-choice test scores were higher for easy texts than difficult ones, higher for narratives than expositions, and higher for words children mentioned that they knew on the Checklist already. More importantly, there’s a very high correlation between children’s prior knowledge of that target words as measured on the checklist task and their performance on the multiple-choice test. Furthermore, there was no learning from context for words at the highest level of conceptual difficulty. As for text properties, results indicted that the more difficult the text, the fewer unknown words were learned; the longer the sentences and words, the less was learned form context; and fewer words were learned form context in texts that had a higher proportion of conceptually difficult words. Also, the longer the average length of a target word in a text, the less likely any target word is to be learned from context. In this regard, texts with more conceptually difficult words tend to have more long words, and vice versa. Nagy & Herman (1987) Learning word meanings from context during normal reading. American Educational Research Journal, 24, 237-270. 026-2 Note: (1)Nagy at el. (1985) argued that even a small probability of learning a word from context can result in large-scale vocabulary growth, if there is a sufficient volume of wide reading. (2) How much vocabulary a child actually gains from written context depends on three factors: the volume of a child’s exposure to written language, the quality of the text, and the child’s ability to infer and remember the meanings of new words encountered during reading. In sum, Nagy and Herman claim the importance of prior knowledge in young children’s learning of vocabulary through reading. Most important, the researchers maintain that comprehension is dependent on the reader’s ability to integrate information in text with existing knowledge structures, which explained clearly why teachers should provide students with input which is slightly above their current level to help them acquire new knowledge. Besides, Nagy and Herman highlighted the importance of contextual support of a text to enhance learners’ comprehension. Last, the researchers argued that multiple encounters with a word in a variety of meaningful contexts is necessary to produce the depth of word knowledge. And they further indicated that, from this and their earlier studies, “wide, regular reading must be seen as the major avenue of large-scale, long-term vocabulary growth. Next, Nagy and Herman pointed out that “incidental learning from written context represents about a third of a child’s annual vocabulary growth, an increase in absolute vocabulary size that has not even been approached by any program of direct vocabulary instruction. Accordingly, the results showed that normal reading, with a wide range of difficulty & familiarity, there’s sth there for everyone to learn. And id children are given texts they can comprehend, they will eventually gain the meaning of the unknown words. Brett, A., Rothlein, L., & Hurley, M. (1996). Vocabulary acquisition from listening to stories and explanations of target words. The Elementary School Journal, 96, 415-422. 027 Abstract: The study aimed to examine the effects of 3 conditions on elementary school children’s vocabulary acquisition: listening to stories with a brief explanation of the unfamiliar target words, listening to stories with nor explanation of the words, and having not exposure to the stories or vocabulary (the control condition). Students who listened to 2 stories along with a brief explanation of target words learned significantly more new words and still remembered them 6 weeks after the intervention than the children in the other two groups. Design: pre-and post-test, delayed posttest (6 weeks later), 20 items (10 per story) 2 stories, 5 days per story, one hundred and seventy- five 10 years old children (4th graders), L1 Findings: Statistical analysis (3 group x 2 book x 3 time ANOVA & post hoc)showed that pretest scores for the story-with explanation group were lower than the scores for the other two groups, and the posttest and delayed posttest scores of the story-with-explanation group were significantly higher than the scores of the other two groups. More impressively, the experimental group not only outperformed the other two groups but also remembered the words 6 weeks later. The results support the hypothesis that 4th graders can acquire new words from listening to stories if there’s a brief explanation of the new words. Note: In reading aloud, comprehension and enjoyment are optimal. To maximize the benefit of explaining words as they are encountered in the story, it would be helpful to discover which components of the explanation of the unfamiliar words are critical. Jenkins, J. R., Stein, M. L., & Wysocki, K. (1984). Learning vocabulary through reading. American Educational Research Journal, 21, 767-787. 028-1 Abstract: The study examined the effect of reading on young learners’ vocabulary growth. The researcher hypothesized that new vocabulary can be acquired via incidental learning of word meanings from context. Also, Jenkins et al. intended to find out the definite frequency of context exposure to boost learners’ vocabulary. Last, the influence of prior exposure of the target words was also investigated. Design: 120 fifth graders (age ranging from 9 to 11), measures: four posttests: 3 vocabulary tests & 1 reading comprehension, statistical analysis: 2 (preexposure: present or absent) x 2 (ability: more or less able readers) x 3 (context presentations: 2, 6, or 10) x 3 (word set) factorial design Findings: Firstly, results of vocabulary measures showed that higher ability students outperformed their less skilled peers. Also, prior exposure did exert its positive influence on learners’ vocabulary test. The more exposures of the target words, the better the learners’ word learning (2 of the 3 measures showed the differences). Secondly, results of reading comprehension indicated that only 10 context presentations yielded a significant difference on the measure. In sum, the study results are noteworthy in that fifth graders did acquire some new vocabularies from context without explicit instructions. However, context representations did not show its power over the learners’ reading ability in the study. Yet the factor affected the learners’ vocabulary changes. In other words, the better readers were more likely to acquire word meanings, given some experience with the unfamiliar words in context. As Jenkins et al. hypothesized, incidental vocabulary learning during reading may account for a major portion of vocabulary growth during the elementary grades. The study confirmed the hypothesis with positive results in both vocabulary test and reading comprehension measure. Jenkins, J. R., Stein, M. L., & Wysocki, K. (1984). Learning vocabulary through reading. American Educational Research Journal, 21, 767-787. 028-2 To assure the positive outcome, the researchers argued that increased context presentations should be provided. That is, more than 2 context exposures were needed to affect vocabulary acquisition. Jenkins et al. claim conservatively that it was most possible that 10 exposures might boost young learners’ vocabulary growth. With the assertion, the researchers admitted that although they had found that children learned new words incidentally through reading, “this learning apparently does not come easily or in large quantities.” Also Jenkins et al. indicated the important role of reading in a child’s vocabulary base by quoting Nagy & Anderson’s (1982) words that although some vocabulary learning occur through listening, learning new words form context would have to assign an important role to reading in accounting for vocabulary growth. s Word meanings were learned from context, and more frequent presentation in context increased learning. Also, better readers profited more from context and prior exposure informal teaching) resulted in greater learning. Vocabulary effects were also observed on a measure of reading comprehension. Gay, C. (1976) Reading aloud and learning to write. The Elementary School Journal,??, 87-93. 029-1 Abstract: Gay explained the relationship between reading aloud and its contributions on children’s language development. The researcher claims that if people want to help young learners how to write, they have to “stifle the questions and postpone the discussions and simply read aloud.” According to Gat, reading aloud facilitated a child’s vocabulary, sentence structure, sense of structure and organization. More importantly, via reading aloud, the child may gain a motive for writing. Also, Gay pointed out that averagely the lack of writing fluency in learners may be compensated for by reading aloud literary works to the child. Through reading aloud good books, learners would absorb the constructions of written language and thus develop the ability of writing. In other words, learners should be provided with opportunities to assimilate the patterns and the rhythms of writing before they are able to write. Mostly, Gay mentioned that children who read much more than textbooks are children who usually read well and write well, which stressed the importance of reading in a child’s language ability. Furthermore, Gay asserted that language is primarily oral. Namely, we hear language even when it is written of when we are writing it. Accordingly, children are able to write only those patterns they are used to hearing before. To ensure rich qualitative writing, the first step may be to read aloud storybooks to young children. Gay put it that a better reading program should aware of the importance of reading aloud, ant they should try to make provision for it. On the other hand, Gay critically revealed the reasons of a failure of a reading program. Firstly, it is handicapped because it is a program. In detail, such programs usually follow an organized step-by-step way so that children can learn what they need to learn and can learn at the appropriate level and can be tested on to make sure whether they have succeeded. All these go against the nature of reading and reading aloud, which is fun and should be experienced without questions, without artificial stimulants, but Gay, C. (1976) Reading aloud and learning to write. The Elementary School Journal,??, 87-93. 029-2 just reading. With what Gay asserted, one could easily infer why a reading program would lead to negative outcome or even lead to children’s fear or hatred toward reading per se. (with endless questions to check their language ability or even label the child accord to her/ his performance on the one-shot test) “They think of questions, failure; and reading becomes associated with an unpleasant experience. Secondly, the failure of a reading program, even a good one, is because that they are limited to books on or neat the child’s own reading level. The solution to it is the child has to be exposed to the input that is slightly beyond his or her current level to make sure that acquisition would take place. In terms of the contributions of reading aloud, Gay further pointed out that usually the child who is having no difficulty with language, who is reading well and has little difficulty with exercises because the child’s vocabulary already takes in those words. Gay argued that the exercises were not really teaching the child anything but were simply testing what s/he already knew. (it is the input per se teaches a child rather than the output) As for less able children, Gay mentioned that teachers should read interesting book aloud to them often and regularly so that they will hear the words and comprehend them in a natural, effortless way. By so doing, Gay believed that would eventually motivate the less skilled children in learning the language for the words were introduced to in interesting, irresistible contexts. After all, according to Gay, “one can’t read what one hasn’t heard; one can’t write what one hasn’t head.” The second benefit of reading aloud is the ability to distinguish between shades of meaning, which is, as Gay maintained, the ability to use language discriminately and which is critical to good writing. “There are subtle differences in word meaning, subtle differences that increase out power to communicate, but casual speech is not the place a child can become aware of them (p. 91). Gay, C. (1976) Reading aloud and learning to write. The Elementary School Journal,77, 87-93. 029-3 “We can describe the concrete simply by naming, but to talk about a feeling, an idea—to communicate that unseen inner world that is central to our existence。We must have a command of metaphor and analogy. How does a child get such a command? Not by learning a definition, but by hearing sentences like those in Julia Cunningham’s Dorp Dead (p. 92).” Next, through reading aloud, children would have the chance of listening to language which has structure and organization and that is important for them to be able to write like that one day. Ninio, A., & Bruner, J. (1978). The achievement and antecedents of labeling. Journal of Child Language, 5, 1-15. 030-1 Abstract: Ninio and Bruner investigated the influence of labeling during storytelling in a mother-infant dyad. Results of the longitudinal study confirmed the positive influence of labeling in an infant’s language development. Analysis of joint picture-book reading revealed that this activity had very early on the structure of a dialogue. In this study, the researchers have used book-reading as a principal source of data—the mother and the child looking at pictures in a book. Design: mother-infant looking at storybook; the child was 8 months (observation was from the child was 8 months to 1 year and 6 month years old; a longitudinal study (10 months); video-recorded observation, 12 filming session were analyzed Findings: Results indicated that reading dialogue cycles were highly constant on a number of structural characteristics. Also, stable increase of turns per cycle and the mean duration per cycle were seen in the observation—all these released the fact that these constancies in the dialogue were remarkable for the child’s linguistic performance undergoes profound changes, including the appearance of standard lexical words in his communicative repertoire. And with the language-teaching function, the book-reading situation is more central on a standard action format like simple and recurrent joint action patterns in other formats. Active participation, vocalization and lexical utterances all increase steadily, and all these showed that storybook reading elicited a child’s language development. Also, after being read to for 6 months, the appearance of vocalizations that were recognizable approximations to lexical labels in the child convinced the mother to believe that the child possessed a hypothesis about a relation between sound and meaning; that coming from listening to stories read aloud. Moreover, proportion of reading cycles in which the child emitted an active response, proportion of active turns of the child’s containing a vocalization, and proportion of vocalizations which were lexical labels exhibited Ninio, A., & Bruner, J. (1978). The achievement and antecedents of labeling. Journal of Child Language, 5, 1-15. 030-2 greatly that the child progressed steadily through the book-reading experience. Accordingly, Ninio and Bruner concluded that storybook reading was a format well suited to the teaching of labeling for it has few elements and strict ordering rules between them. Also, it is flexible in the sense of accepting a great variety of responses by the child. Last, it is highly repetitive, both in the elements like Look, What’s that? and the labels themselves. Last, as the researchers concluded, a central element in the achievement of labeling by the child is his mastery of the reciprocal rules that govern the exchanges between him and his mother into which labeling is inserted. Note: Labeling was defined as the stressed element in a demonstrative utterance, e.g. “It is an X.” The most striking characteristic of labeling activity is that it takes place in a structural interactional sequence that has the texture of a dialogue. It is greater if the cycle is initiated by the child or by the mother uttering a What’s that? questions than if it is initiated by the mother saying look of offering a label for the question “What’s that” usually elicits a label from the child. Imitation seemed not so effective as labeling. (for by asking what- questions or letting the child initiate would be more likely that the child will label on his own) Elley, W. (1989). Vocabulary acquisition from listening to stories. Reading Research Quarterly, 24(2), 174-187. 031-1 Abstract: Hold the belief of positive influence of reading aloud, the researcher conducted the research to examine the effects of reading aloud on elementary children’s vocabulary acquisition. Two experiments were set up to study the impact of teachers’ explanation in students’ word learning. Pre-and-post tests and follow-up tests were administered. Results showed that story reading was a significant source of children’s vocabulary acquisition regardless of whether explanations of the unknown words were provided by the teacher. Further, Elley found that frequency of the unknown word appeared in the storybook constituted the key element of the learning of that word. Design: 2 experiments, one without teachers’ explanation, the other with teachers’ explanation. The 1st experiment: one hundred sixty- eight 7 year-old children; the 2nd: one hundred twenty-seven 8 year-old children; In Ex.1: one book; in Ex. 2: two books were used; Procedure: Ex.1: pretest(7 days after) reading (3 times in a week)(2 days later) posttest; Ex.2: L1 Findings: Results from EX.1 indicated that children scored higher on most target words on the posttest than on the posttest, for a mean increase of 15 percent overall. Three of the 6 word-related variables showed significant positive correlations with the children’s mean gain for each word, and the correlations for all 6 were positive. Namely, the words that were most readily learned in the story were those for which the surrounding context was helpful, those that occurred more than once in the story, and those that were illustrated in at least one picture. Most encouragingly, it has been found that the low group showed the most gain, which was odds with the richget-richer syndrome. In other words, through reading aloud, the less able learners had the chance to compete with their more able peers. Also, Elley found that, as research on silent reading, repeated exposure and helpful context of an unknown word were the perquisites of the learning of those new words. Elley, W. (1989). Vocabulary acquisition from listening to stories. Reading Research Quarterly, 24(2), 174-187. 032-2 In order to adjust the limitations of the first experiment, Elley conducted the second experiment using different books, under different teaching conditions, different children, with a more elaborate design. The difference between the two experiments was that the second one employed teachers’ explanations of the unknown words. Results in Ex.2 showed that children in the explanation group benefited most in reading aloud; with more than double gains compared with the without-explanation group. The findings confirmed the power of providing explanations either by giving synonyms, role-playing, or point to the corresponding picture in the storybook with children for learning new items. As Ex.1, Elley found that the same 3 most correlated variables with word learning were strength of meaning cues, number of occurrences, and number of times illustrated. Further, post hoc analysis found that nouns were the easiest form of words for learning than adjectives or verbs. The reversal phenomenon of the Matthew effect was also found in Ex.2. In other words, the lowest group improved more than the other three groups, and the highest group improved the least (concerning regression effect and ceiling effect). The delayed posttest showed only negligible decline of 2-3 percent indicated that the effects of reading aloud on young children’s vocabulary acquisition were permanent. In general, the two experiments supported the assumption that young children can learn new vocabulary from listening to stories read aloud. With teachers’ additional explanations of unknown words can even double vocabulary gains. Moreover, less proficient learners gain at least as much from the readings as the other more proficient ones and the learning is relatively permanent. Frequency of occurrence of the word in the story, the helpfulness of the context, and the frequency of occurrence of the word in pictorial representation are the three most critical elements influencing children’s word learning. Elley, W., & Mangubhai, E. (1983). The impact of reading on second language learning. Reading Research Quarterly, 19, 53-67. 033-1 Abstract: The researchers hypothesized that reading program could eliminate the five critical differences between the first and the second language learning. The five differences are strength of motivation, emphasis on meaning versus form, amount of exposure to language, type of exposure to language, and the quality of models. Thus, Elley and Mangubhai suggest that when children read interesting storybooks, the five differences diminished. Design: 380 class 4 and class 5 children (9 to 11 years old); 3 groups: the Shared Book group, Sustained Silent Reading group, and the control group; contrast 1: the book reading groups vs. the controlBook Flood hypothesis: exposure to large numbers of storybooks will have an effect on general language competence; contrast 2: the Shared Book group vs. Sustained Silent Reading group hypothesis: exposure to new language in the Shared Book method is more persistent and concentrated, and that children would become more actively involved in the learning experience than in the Silent Reading group. Last, it would be expected that effects on receptive language is more significant than expressive language skills. Pre-and-posttest and follow-up 12 months were administered. (L2) Findings: Class 4 Results indicated that the Book Flood groups outperformed the control group dramatically in reading comprehension (the groups produced 15 months reading growth in 8 months; the control: 6.5 months growth only, which indicated that the reading groups were improving their general reading comprehension skill at over twice the normal rate!) However, there’s no significant effect of the Shared Book group over the Sustained Silent Reading group in that measure. On the Word Recognition test, the reading groups also had higher means than the control yet didn’t reach the level of significance. And there’s small difference between the two reading groups. And the oral sentences tests favored the Shared Book group than the other two groups. Elley, W., & Mangubhai, E. (1983). The impact of reading on second language learning. Reading Research Quarterly, 19, 53-67. 033-2 Findings: Class 5 results showed that in all measures, the Book Flood groups outperformed the control group. In Reading Comprehension, the Shared Book group significantly ahead of the Silent Reading group. In detail, the Shared Book group produced 15 months reading growth in 8 months, comparing to Silent Reading group’s 9 months and the control group’s 2.5 months. The same patterns were also seen in Listening Comprehension. In general, the comparisons between Shared Book and Silent Reading methods showed significantly greater benefits for the former in Class 5, but not in Class 4. The observed differences were greater in children’s receptive skills than in expressive skills. Follow-up 12 months showed that both reading groups made greater progress in their language growth on all tests. But in no case, however, did the Shared Book group significantly surpass the Silent Reading group. Namely, the sharp contrasts between the reading groups and the control group were, according to the researchers, repeated exposure to print, pictures, and story lines. Last, it is worth noting that teachers in the Silent Reading method provided no follow-up activities to support the reading done, whereas those in the Shared Reading method did. The lack of difference between the two methods indicated that those follow-up activities were not as effective as their advocates believed. Anderson, R. C., Wilson, P. T., & Fielding, L. G. (1988). Growth in reading and how children spend their time outside of school. Reading Research Quarterly, XXIII, 285-303. 034-1 Abstract: The study investigated how much time elementary school children spent on reading outside of school and further examined the relationship between the time spent and their reading proficiency. Still, the researchers were interested in answering the question whether out-of –school activities are in the causal nexus that produce reading growth. Among all the ways children spent their time, reading books was the best predictor of several measures of reading achievement. However, the study also found that on most days most children did little or no book reading. Design: 155 fifth-grade students; the use of activity forms (filled in by the children, asking them to write down either the title or the author’s name of both more valid indicator of actual reading); 3 reading tests: reading comprehension test, a checklist vocabulary measure, and measure of reading speed Findings: Although most children do little reading, successive groups of children read for increasingly long periods of time and cover increasingly large numbers of words. Notably, results found that teachers have a significant influence on the amount of book reading children do out of school. Results showed that the class that did the most reading in class read 3.6 times as much on the average as the class that did the least reading. In other words, the more the teacher read aloud to students or provide them time for reading during the school day, the more time children spent on reading after school. This findings were so encouraging when consider promoting reading, reading aloud and providing time for reading during the school day were the two ways teachers did in class. Also, children who were good readers in the second grade did more reading in the fifth grade. In regard of sex difference, girls read more than boys. Moreover, reading books was the out-of school activity that proved to have the strongest association with reading proficiency in children. Time spent reading Anderson, R. C., Wilson, P. T., & Fielding, L. G. (1988). Growth in reading and how children spend their time outside of school. Reading Research Quarterly, XXIII, 285-303. 034-2 books was fairly strongly correlated with the measures of a child’s status as a reader in the fifth grade. Most importantly, time spent reading books was the best predictor of a child’s growth as a reader from the second to the fifth grade. The findings may infer the fact that reading aloud not only increases a child’s language growth but also enhances the possibility of encouraging the child to do more out-of-school reading and that gives big returns in more advanced language outcomes to the child. According to Anderson et al., “there is a moderately storing association between out-of school reading and reading achievement, a relationship of about the same magnitude as the strongest relationships reported with in-school use of time (Barr & Dreeben, 1983; Rosenshine & Stevens, 1984).” The researchers also found that a typical child in the middle grades reads less than 25 minutes a day out of school. Again, from the results, one can easily find that book reading is a significant predictor of reading growth. In terms of reckoning the influence of book reading in a child’s reading achievement, 6.6 % of the variance in fifth-grade reading comprehension is uniquely explained in terms of reading books. Last, the researchers concluded that the amount of time a child spends reading books is strongly related to the child’s reading level in the fifth grade and growth in reading proficiency from the second to the fifth grade. The findings of the present research indicated that, according to the researchers, reading books is a cause, not merely a reflection, of reading proficiency. And home reading experience has influencing effect on fifth graders’ reading comprehension ability after second grade comprehension ability was partialed out. Juel, C. (1988). Learning to read and write: A longitudinal study of 54 children from first through fourth grades. Journal of Educational Psychology, 80, 437-447. 035-1 Abstract: The study focused on elementary school children’s literacy development, investigated the relationship between reading and writing. The probability that a child would remain a poor reader at the end of fourth grade if the child was a poor reader at the end of first grade was .88. But early writing skill did not predict later writing skill. Good readers read considerably more than the poor readers both in and out of school, which appeared to contribute to the good readers’ growth in some reading and writing skills. Poor readers tended to become poor writers. Juel argued that children who have experienced decontextualized language by having been read to, by hearing language used for purely conceptual discussions, or in other abstract contexts are better prepared for the language used in classroom. And such children are likely to be familiar with story structures and complex syntax and have richer vocabularies and developed concepts that will foster reading, as well as writing, comprehension of books. Design: 54 children(from first through fourth grade), several measure on language aspects, including phonemic awareness, decoding, word recognition, listening comprehension, reading comprehension, spelling, and writing were administered. Also children’s IQ have been tested. And children’s ideas of story have also been investigated. Last, interviews about children’s home reading, and attitude toward reading have been probed. Findings: The findings were provided to answer the following questions. Do the same children remain poor readers year after year? Do the same children remain poor writers year after year? What skills do the poor readers lack? What skills do the poor writers lack? What factors seem to keep poor readers from improving? And What factors seem to keep poor writers fro, improving? Results indicated that a child wound remain a poor reader at the end of fourth grade, if the child was a poor reader at the end of first grade was .88; the probability Juel, C. (1988). Learning to read and write: A longitudinal study of 54 children from first through fourth grades. Journal of Educational Psychology, 80, 437-447. 035-2 that a child would become a poor reader in fourth grade if he or she had at least average reading skills in first grade was .12. The probability that a child would remain an average reader in fourth grade if the child had average reading ability in first grade was .87; the probability that a child would become an average reader in fourth grade if he or she was a poor reader in first grade was only .13. The explanation for these correlations was that the poor first-grade reader almost invariably remains a poor reader by the end of fourth grade. However, results failed to report that early writing skill was a predictor of later writing skill as well as early reading ability predicted later reading ability. Moreover, Juel found that poor reader entered first grade with little phonemic awareness and their knowledge of spelling-sound correspondence was initially low comparing to their more able peers. What’s worse, poor readers never reached the level of the average and good readers. Poor writers seemed to lack the ability of generate a real story. Namely, they were still writing descriptions rather than stories at he end of fourth grade. In the research, it has also found that those poor writers who also had poor spelling and poor ideas. As for the ability that poorer readers lack, the results indicated that it was their poor decoding skill that prevented them from reading and thus impeded them from improving. Juel showed that by the end of first grade, the good readers has seen, on average, 18,681 words in running text in their basal readers. In contrast, the poor readers had seen, on average, 9975 words, about half as many. Later in school years, the good readers, on average, had read about 178000 words in running text in their basal readers by the end of fourth grade, whereas the poor reader had read less than half of that—about 80000 words. The bid contrast explained why the difference of reading ability between a good and a poor reader was so wide and so hard to make up for years of lost experiences with words and concepts embedded in print. Juel, C. (1988). Learning to read and write: A longitudinal study of 54 children from first through fourth grades. Journal of Educational Psychology, 80, 437-447. 035-3 When probing the children’s reading at home, children, neither group of readers, uncovered the fact that they read Much after school; but in third and fourth grade after school reading became quite frequent for the good readers. And good readers were able to release information about the titles, authors or even the plots of the book they read. However, few poor readers could supply such information. Also, the more frequent reading experiences, both in and out of school, of the children who learned to read well early in school likely contributed to the steadily widening gulf in listening comprehension (i.e. knowledge of vocabulary, concepts, text structures, syntax, and pragmatics) between good and poor readers. Juel inferred that poor readers read little voluntarily, partly because reading was so difficult for them, and reading experiences in school must have been rather aversive. When asking whether they like reading or not 26 of the 29 good reader responded yes, whereas only 5 of the 25 poor readers responded yes. Mostly poor readers perceived reading as a boring activity. Poor reader appear to become poor writers. The correlation between reading and writing at each grade were all reached the level of significance. More clearly, by fourth grade 17 of the 25 poor readers were poor writers, whereas only 4 of the 29 good readers were poor writers. Through the years good readers’ proficiency in producing ideas steadily grew, whereas poor readers made no apparent progress in their ability to tell an oral story from first to fourth grade. Most poor readers were still telling and writing descriptions rather than stories in the fourth grade. On the other hand, by at least fourth grade most good readers were writing stories that included some elements of story grammar (i.e., setting, elaborated description of characters, and at least one episode). The findings led Juel to conclude that the more frequent reading experiences of the good readers probably let to better story ideas, knowledge of story structures and vocabulary with which to express those ideas. In sum, the research revealed Juel, C. (1988). Learning to read and write: A longitudinal study of 54 children from first through fourth grades. Journal of Educational Psychology, 80, 437-447. 035-4 that the poor first-grade reader was almost invariably still poor readers by the end of fourth grade. And the firstgrade good readers almost invariably remained good readers at the end of fourth grade. Last, the researcher maintains that early success with reading is critical and argues that extensive reading or listening to a lot of stories is important to acquiring ideas with which to write one’s own stories for good readers apparently read more and over time have experienced more ideas and vocabulary that can be incorporated into their writing. Cipielewski, J., & Stanovich, K.E. (1992). Predicting growth in reading ability from children’s exposure to print. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 54, 74-89. 036 Abstract: The longitudinal study employed two indicators aimed to examine the relationship between growth of reading ability and print exposure. A checklist-with-foils logic was utilized. Design: 98 participants (54 fourth graders & 44 fifth graders); Print exposure measures: TRT (Title Recognition Test) & ART (Author Recognition Test); standardized measures of reading ability: the Reading Comprehension subtest, the Stanford Diagnostic Reading Test, the Reading Comprehension, Reading Rate, and Phonetic Analysis subtest; 2-year study Findings: Both print exposure measures displayed significant correlations with every measure of reading comprehension ability although the correlations of the ART with measures of reading comprehension was lower than those involving TRT. These results indicated that the two print exposure measures predicted individual differences in third- to fifth- grade growth in reading ability. Results showed that individual differences in reading comprehension growth were reliably linked to differences in print exposure. Therefore, the researchers assumed that print exposure appears to be both a consequence of developed reading ability and a contributor to further growth in that ability. Thus, the results strengthened the importance of reading exposure in terms of reading growth and even general cognitive development. In conclusion, the researchers indicated that the two indicators of print exposure were reliable. Last, according to Stanovich (1986, cited Cipielewski & Stanovich, 1992), a more causal model sees individual differences in basic cognitive processes and knowledge bases as at least in part resulting from the experience of reading itself. Carol, Chomsky .(1972) Stages in Language Development and Reading Exposure. Harvard Educational Review, 42 (1), 1-33. 037-1 Abstract: The paper contains two main parts: firstly, it investigates language acquisition in children between the ages of six and ten, probing their linguistic competence with respect to complex aspects of English syntax. Moreover, of particular interest is the regular order of acquisition of structures, accompanied by wide variation in rate of acquisition in different children. Secondly, the author examines the relationship between the children’s exposure to the written language and the rate of linguistic development. The results show a strong correlation between a number of the reading exposure measures and language development. Design: Part II: reading questionnaires to both children and parents; daily records kept at home of all reading (and listening to books read aloud); 36 children Findings: As mentioned above, both the amount read and the complexity of the material were taken into consideration. Among the pre-readers, listening to books read aloud is positively related to linguistic stage. In this study, Chomsky uncovers the fact that those pre-readers in higher linguistic stages are read to by more people and hear more books per week, at higher complexity levels than children at lower linguistic stages. Chomsky(1972) Stages in Language Development and Reading Exposure. Harvard Educational Review, 42 (1), 1-33. 037-2 The overall picture of the study shows that at each age, reading or hearing books read is a strong factor, with many different individual measures of reading exposure contributing to this trend. The third item makes sense once its implications are considered. The mother who recalls certain books with pleasure from her own childhood may well transmit this enjoyment to her child very early on when she reads to him. The author then may speculate that this child learns to assign a special role to reading, for what his mother enjoys doing with him, he quite naturally comes to enjoy and recognize as a valued activity. This is what Effie intends to do! To show the great pleasure and enjoyment of reading. The study has implications with regard to language programs in the elementary schools, and the philosophy underlying curriculum design and selection of materials. The author mentions that we know very little about the actual processes by which children learn language, but there has been an increasing awareness over the past few years of just how much the child brings to the task by way of his own internal organization and innate human characteristics. He certainly not “taught” language in any formal sense, but acquire it naturally, in the course of maturing and developing in an environment where he is adequately exposed to it. The research results indicate that the child enters the classroom equipped to learn language and able to do so by methods of his own. This suggests that perhaps the best thing that we might do for him in terms of encouraging this learning would be to make more of it possible, by exposing him to a rich variety of language inputs in interesting, stimulating situations. The question is how. Carol, Chomsky .(1972) Stages in Language Development and Reading Exposure. Harvard Educational Review, 42 (1), 1-33. 037-3 The reading results show that exposure to the more complex language available from reading dose seem to go hand in hand with increased knowledge of the language. That is to say, the child could be read to, stimulated to read on his own, not restricted to material deemed “at his level” but permitted access to books well “above his level” to get out of them whatever he may. Accord to i+1 In general it may be that the effort should be towards providing more and richer language exposure, rather than limiting the child with restrictive and carefully programmed materials. This approach would seem to be more closely in accord with the nature of language acquisition as we are coming to understand it. The purpose of the results is to emphasize that the potential relevance of work of this sort to language curricula will lie in its suggestiveness for effective use of classroom time, rather than in its relation to the specifics of grammar teaching. (the main concern here is to provide the students with lots of meaningful input which is interesting and stimulating enough to encourage the learning. Hearing stories read aloud caters to the need! Effie’s remarks!) Terblanche, L. (2002). Read-Alouds: Do they enhance students’ ability to read? (EDRS: ED 456 192). 038 Abstract: To gain information about teachers’ belief of reading aloud on children’s language development, the researcher conducted the study in which a survey of 22 teachers was done and the results confirmed the findings in the literature. What interested the researcher was whether the time spent reading aloud to students is worth its weight in gold as it is the school’s goal to have all students reading at grade level at or before third grade. (the researcher was a second grade teacher) Design: survey of elementary school teachers Findings: When asked whether students enjoyed being read to, 80% of the respondents gave positive response. Moreover, the teachers mentioned that their students, when being read to, became more attentive and practiced active listening. Also, the teachers claim that almost the students were engaged during the read-aloud, which was likely to elicit comprehension of the story or, what’s better, enhance the language development in the children. By these positive responses from the teachers, the researcher concluded that teachers need to be committed to the continuation and implementation of reading aloud and make it daily routines. Last, although discussions are appropriate, the researcher maintains that children should not be forced into discussions. Note: “Reading aloud can provide a wonderful model to children as to how good readers read with intonation and fluency. Additionally, children are exposed to a more descriptive flow of language than that of their everyday language and dialogue, their vocabulary grows larger with each story (p.6)”. The reading materials fro reading aloud should represent the interests of the children and age appropriate. Pictures are an important tool for children who are ESL learners in vocabulary development. Kies, D. A., Rodriguez, I., & Granato, F. V. (1993). Oral language development through storytelling: An approach to emergent literacy. Reading Improvement, 30, 43-48. 039 Abstract: The purpose of the study emphasized the relevance of storytelling as an informal technique that gets children hooked to reading and writing. The technique is highly regarded as providing children with a wide range of conceptual experiences that prepares them for the literacy development. Discussion: the researcher claims that the storytelling must began during in infancy and continue in the home. At school, the teacher continues the development of these skills by providing many opportunities to supplement, enhance and support these readiness skills. The age and grade appropriate stories provide children with language, concepts, and experiences that help to develop and reinforce oral and written language skills. Children learn language in social contexts while interacting with other children and adults by actually constructing or reconstructing language as they learn. Also, the more frequently children experience language use the more successful they will be in producing appropriate texts for that particular context. To help children in their pattern connection it is essential to provide them with many opportunities to hear diverse patterns of language. And these patterns of language are acquired through the variety and number of stories children are read. According to Peck (1989, cited Kies et a., 1993), with repeated storytelling the children began to develop story sense and assimilate the language and the structure of the story. Also, Pucket (1988, cited Kies et al., 1993) pointed out that children who hear many stories develop a strong motivation for reading, they learn that reading is an enjoyable act and that reading ad writing are activities that make sense and have a purpose. Moreover, children also acquire a desire to read on their own after hearing a story. With large numbers of parents unable to read to their children due to time constraints, storytelling in the classroom is an opportunity to share the gift of time from an adult who is caring. Parents are children’s first and ongoing teachers. Early childhood teachers must take the role of supporters and partners to parents in the young children’s emergent literacy development. Ulanoff, S. H.,& Pucci, S. L. (1999). Learning words from books: The effects of read aloud on second language vocabulary acquisition. Bilingual Research Journal, 23, 1-13. 040-1 Abstract: The study compared the gains made in second language vocabulary of 2 different methodologies: concurrent translation and preview-review in a group of 60 ESL learners. Design: 3 groups: the control, concurrent translation, and preview (to build background knowledge in the primary language)-review (also in the first language to reinforce important points) groups/ pre- and post-test and one week delayed post of vocabulary; the book was chosen for its repetitious and pictorial representation of the target words, as well as predictability to facilitate the use of contextual clues (L2) Findings: Students in the preview-review group score significantly higher than the control and concurrent translation group scored the lowest of all three groups and improved slightly one week after treatment. The results supported the use of strategies which build background knowledge as a means of teaching second language vocabulary to English learners. From the results, it is clear that children learn and retain more vocabulary from listening to stories within a schematic framework in which the primary language in a scaffolding type activity is used. Moreover, the gains of the preview and review group (57%) surpass even Elley’s (1989) findings of a growth of 33% with 3 repetitions of a story and brief explanations. In the study, the story was read aloud only once, therefore, it is the preview-review technique that provides enough context to activate the schemata and assist the children to acquire the target vocabulary. Without multiple exposures, the type of mediation offered served the function of providing repeated exposure to target words. However, the findings support previous research confirming that listening to stories read aloud facilitates vocabulary acquisition. Ulanoff, S. H.,& Pucci, S. L. (1999). Learning words from books: The effects of read aloud on second language vocabulary acquisition. Bilingual Research Journal, 23, 1-13. 040-2 Note: the content is previewed to build background knowledge in the primary language, the lesson then taught in the target language The study emphasized the importance of schematic framework in language learners. As the researchers mentioned, it is important to contextualize language within a schematic framework such as in the form of an interesting and predictable story. According to the researchers, it is more important for second language learners to activate schemata to make connections b/w new vocabulary and background knowledge. And the new words are easier to absorb within a contextualized framework (Andersson & Barnits 1984, cited Ulanoff, S. H.,& Pucci, S. L. (1999)). 鄒文莉 (民 92)。說故事英語教學與國小英語學習。師大學報。 第 48 卷,頁 53-67。 (Tsou) 041-1 Abstract: The purpose of the study is to investigate the relationship between storytelling and language development. In detail, the research intended to understand how storytelling shape a teacher’s talk during the reading, how storytelling influence students’ oral language development and their participation in the reading, last, how storytelling exert its impact on classroom atmosphere. Design: qualitative research; 2 fifth-grade classes; a semester study (13 classes); observation, video taping and audio recording, interview with the teacher, the teacher’s reflection Findings: Teachers in the storytelling group used more open-ended questions than their colleagues in the control. More impressing, teachers in the storytelling group put much less attention on classroom management, with 1.27% comparing to 14.79%, which reflected the more attention and participation in the storytelling group. Furthermore, children in the storytelling group showed much higher frequency of active participation compared to their peers in the control, with 34.54 time/day to only 8.38 time/day. The big contrast further indicated the positive influence the storytelling exerted in young EFL children’s language development. Last, the research found that teachers in the storytelling group reflected in the interview that during storytelling, they put less efforts in managing the class. However, in traditional teaching, they would have to take a more proportion of the class time on classroom management. Also, through the observation of classroom atmosphere, classroom order, and students’ participation, the teachers pointed out that storytelling indeed had more positive effects than traditional teaching. With follow-up activities, the teachers assured the fact that students were very likely to absorb new vocabulary in the story. The observers also released the similar findings with the classroom teachers. In addition, the observers added that children in the storytelling group asked more questions and were more willing to share their ideas with their teacher and peers. 田耐青、沈宜屏 (民 92)。國小級任導師運用英語童書輔助學生 英文學習之行動研究。Selected Papers from the Twelfth international Symposium on English Teaching and Learning.,559-569. 042-1 Abstract: the purpose of the research is to probe the influence of storytelling on EFL elementary school children’s language development. After a pilot study, the researcher developed 3P3R reading strategy to help young EFL learners acquired the target language. Design: 12 fifth-grade children in ABCD group (divide based on their conversation class performance); a qualitative and quantitative study; data collected by the teacher’s teaching journal, observers’ observation, the students’ learning diary, students’ reflection, and interview with the students Findings: The researcher found that upper grade elementary children liked to participate in interactive context. Also, the boys showed a preference of making prediction during storytelling. On the other hand, the girls liked to ask questions to further understand the story. Moreover, the researcher pointed out that, via storytelling, children showed higher confidence and preference toward English learning. What’s more, according to the researcher’s observation, the children, after being read to, would go get books from the bookshelf in the classroom, for reading on their own. Some parents even told the researcher that they found their children loved reading and would read aloud storybooks, which were borrowed from the classroom library, to their younger siblings at home. When asking whether they would like the storytelling program continued, 100% of the participants gave the positive response, which indicated the growth of love for English learning after the storytelling. In addition, the researcher pointed out that, after the action research, she would like to change the way she teaches young learners. Other than traditional teaching, she would like to try other teaching approach, such as storytelling because of the positive results gained from the study. Also, the researcher pointed out that teachers would become more confident after the trial of choosing storybooks, which indicated that teachers would be more experienced in the selection of storybooks. S 田耐青、沈宜屏 (民 92)。國小級任導師運用英語童書輔助學生 英文學習之行動研究。Selected Papers from the Twelfth international Symposium on English Teaching and Learning. 559-569. 042-2 The researcher also confirmed that fact that storytelling would broaden the students’ background knowledge and enrich their life by learning different cultures through the storybooks. Still, as for the follow-up activities, the researchers found that more able students liked self-reading, on the other hand, less able students preferred the teacher read aloud for them. Last, the research suggested that when doting storytelling, teachers might interact with the students through prediction—having students predict the following page or event, or questioning— asking the students some relevant questions. When it comes to choose an appropriate storybook, the researchers mentioned that first, predictable books might be considered. Second, the storyline should be able to link to the students’ background. Third, language level of the storybook should also be taken into consideration. For example, a storybook with complex language might decrease less proficient learners’ interests, yet an interesting book with easy language level would fail to attract those more able students. For lower achievers, repetitious pattern in a storybook may be suitable for them. (陳秋蘭,民 92) cited 田耐青、沈宜屏 (民 92)。 王怡雯 (民 93)。從閱讀英語文圖畫書看幼兒讀寫萌發現象。 Selected Papers from the Thirteenth International Symposium and Book Fair on English Teaching. 198-209. 043-1 Abstract: The purpose of the study is to investigate the relationship between reading aloud storybooks and an infant’s emergent literacy. The researcher intended to answer the following 2 questions: (1) during the parentchild reading aloud, what would the infant respond to the reading behavior? (2)What were the meanings of the topic, plot, and the illustrations to the infant? Design: a case / qualitative study; instruments: participant observation and in-depth interview; audio recording; 3 books; a three year and nine month boy Findings: after mother-infant reading aloud, the researcher found that the infant would like to pick the storybook which his mother read to him by flicking through the pages. Also, when choosing the storybook, the infant would choose a book which could reflect his mood at that moment. Furthermore, the infant would share the story with his grandparents by telling them what the story was. Although confined by limited expressive ability, the infant, after reading a storybook, shared the book with others by role playing, puppets or drawing. Also, the research found that, during the storybook reading, the infant either passively listened to the story, answered the questions posed by his mother, or imitated his mother voice in the reading. Still, the infant showed logic thinking when being asked relevant questions about the story. What’s more, the infant linked the story plot to his personal experience, for example, “David was like me. We both liked to climbed, played toys in the bathtub, or played while eating meals.” The infant showed his curiosity about the story by asking questions like “Are these animals running out of the zoo?” Observation also indicated that illustrations raised the infant’s curiosity for he asked some questions about the given pictures. 王怡雯 (民 93)。從閱讀英語文圖畫書看幼兒讀寫萌發現象。 Selected Papers from the Thirteenth International Symposium and Book Fair on English Teaching. 198-209. 043-2 The researcher concluded that to help the child develop emergent literacy, parents or nursery teachers should read aloud with the child. Also, through interactive context between the adult and the child, s/he would benefit from the reading aloud. Note: factor influenced a child’s emergent literacy, after reviewing literature, Wang pointed out that book access and reading frequency were critical. Additionally, more library visits were found in early readers, and the frequency of library visiting was positively correlated with reading comprehension. Also, the more parents read aloud to their children, the better their reading achievement would be. The more active the interaction between the child and his/her parents, the better reading ability would be found. And, the more the child read, the higher his/her interests are; the higher the interests, the better reading ability would be seen. The more the mother read with the infant, the more possible the infant would read and the frequency would be higher. Yang( 民 84, cited Wang,????) Tsou, W., Wang, W., & Tzeng, Y. (2006). Applying a multimedia storytelling website in foreign language learning. Computers & Education, 47, 17-28. 044-1 Abstract: The researchers developed a multimedia Storytelling Website to study how web-based technology can assist young EFL learners learning English. The website contains an accounts administration module, multimedia story composing module, and story re-playing module. The purpose of the study was to investigate how web-based technology can assist EFL teaching and learning process through storytelling and story recall. Design: 2 classes of fifth-graders(35 students each); 10 weeks; the control group used big books and flash cards, the experimental group the teacher recreated the story with the Story Website to accompany the teacher’s storytelling; questionnaire (for students) and interview (with the instructor); pretest of language proficiency, story comprehension, and sentence complexity Findings: Students in the Story Website group did better in their sentence complexity and post language proficiency test but not significant surpassed the control group in story comprehension. The results indicated that storytelling was appealing to students, with or without the assistance of the storytelling multimedia web, students could easily comprehend the story and recall the story structure. Second, with the multimedia computer-assisted process, students retained more words, phrases and sentences. Students received extra visual and audio stimuli through still pictures, animation, music, and/or sound effects. According to the researchers, these stimuli not only provided easier access for story recalls but also facilitated students’ creativity in recreating stories. Students in the Storytelling Website group tended to include more details in recalls. Furthermore, many details that the students included in their recalls were related to materials coming from their own creations, such as physical descriptions of the characters, their locations, and sound effects. Tsou, W., Wang, W., & Tzeng, Y. (2006). Applying a multimedia storytelling website in foreign language learning. Computers & Education, 47, 17-28. 044-2 Results from the student questionnaire revealed that students of both groups expressed interest in having storytelling as a regular part of the class. When asked about their improvement on English learning, students in the experimental group tended to be more confident than students in the control group, and this result was also confirmed by the post language proficiency test. In addition, students in the experimental group expressed enjoyment of the story recalling. On the other hand, the instructor indicated that greater anxiety when presenting stories in the control group and she felt less confident. The instructor pointed out that students in the experimental group seemed to be more engaged in the storytelling process because of colorful, animated multimedia format of the presentation. Also, the teacher concluded that storytelling was effective on language teaching, but without the Website she would not embrace the storytelling herself. The Website also allows teachers’ stories be played again and again for reviewing purpose, and individual students’ story recalls to be stored into a personal digital portfolio which teachers can access students’ learning progress or to conduct peer comparisons. \ Lickteig, M. J., & Russell, J. F. (1993) Elementary teachers’ readaloud practices. Reading Improvement, 30, 202-208. 045-1 Abstract: The researchers conducted a survey asking 183 elementary school teachers about their read-aloud practice in the classrooms. Findings: The data revealed that reading aloud is a pervasive practice. A majority of teachers read aloud every day at least 20 minutes (71%). The frequency of times per week decreased and the length of the read aloud sessions increased as the grades got higher. And the findings showed that children listened quietly (73% across all grades). The teachers revealed the major reason for reading aloud were enjoyment and with an attempt to foster the love of reading and literature. Still, to exposure students to more reading was another reason for doing reading aloud. In reading aloud, both fantasy and realism are presented. When it comes to educational value of reading aloud, the researchers emphasized that one way of stressing its importance is to expect students’ full attention during literature time. To achieve this goal, the researchers argued that reading aloud should be included into regular curriculum rather than a supplementary activity. To be an experienced read- aloud teacher, the researcher suggested that the more literature the teacher knows the better. Book chosen should be ones the teacher genuinely enjoys and is eager to share with her/ his students. To help students be more attentive to the practice, the teacher may, suggested by Funk & Funk (1989, cited Lickteig & Russell (1993), provide a purpose, set the stage and involve them with follow-up activities. For example, the teacher may provide background of the story before the reading and allow some time for a short discussion after the reading. Moreover, the researchers pointed out that with young learners, allowing participation is a good way to ensure listening. That is, they may participate with discussions of illustrations, ask questions about the story events, or even predict texts. Lickteig, M. J., & Russell, J. F. (1993) Elementary teachers’ readaloud practices. Reading Improvement, 30, 202-208. 045-2 Mendoza (1988, cited Lickteig & Russell (1993) suggested that regard to beginning learners, teachers may start with short picture books and working up to short novels and trying various time/content level combinations until finding one that works well. The quality of reading is another factor to be considered. In sum, the teacher should keep in mind that the goal to communicate a story. Last, the researchers claimed that teachers use reading aloud to cultivate a love of reading, to encourage thinking about important issues that might otherwise not be included in the curriculum, and for enjoyment and relaxation. 簡 郁 娟 、 黃 月 貴 ( 民 89) 。 Starting with predictable stories: EFL children’s oral and literacy development. Selected Papers from the Ninth International Symposium on English Teaching and Learning, 264-273. (Chien & Huang, 2000) 046-1 Abstract: The study investigated the effect of predictable storybooks on kindergartners’ oral and emergent literacy development. Research questions were: (1) What changes occur in EFL children’s oral language use after reading a variety of predictable reading materials? (2) How do predictable reading texts influence EFL children’s emergent reading behaviors before formal reading instruction? And (3) what are the relationships between parents’ involvement and children’s development of second language reading? Design: one school year study; five 5 to 6 years old children; ethnographic research, data collected from observation, parental interview, teaching logs, students’ portfolios, and miscue analysis Findings: Data from field notes and the transcription of videotapes reveal that the patterns in predictable stories were easily absorbed by the children and thus the children produced them into their daily conversation without hesitation or difficulty. Also, the patterns of vocabulary were presented in a meaningful context, and this enables the children transferred what they learned from the storybook reading to their real-life situations. Using The Emergent Reading Behavior Inventory, which included the categories for reading behaviors of book awareness, directionality, print awareness, reading-like behavior, and the use of cueing systems, to investigate what the children did during the storytelling, the researchers found that students made obvious progress in concepts about print (e.g. book awareness, print direction, and print awareness). The results supported the hypothesis that young children construct print awareness through literacy activities before formal reading instruction. Also, through predictable storybooks, young EFL children showed reading-like behaviors, and developed the concepts about print at the emergent literacy level. 簡 郁 娟 、 黃 月 貴 ( 民 89) 。 Starting with predictable stories: EFL children’s oral and literacy development. Selected Papers from the Ninth International Symposium on English Teaching and Learning, 264-273. 046-2 Furthermore, Miscue Analysis was employed to identify the students’ reading strategies in the final evaluation. By doing so, the researchers attempted to learn whether the children could use their graph-phonemic connection to read unknown words without formal phonics instruction. Results indicated that, except for one child, the other 4 children confidently read a whole story even though they sometimes stopped in one or two sentences, and then continued their reading by employing their syntactic, semantic, and graph-phonemic knowledge to make sense of the text or by the prompts of the teacher’s clues or direct language help. Some of the children might have read by resorting to memory and pictorial clues, but some of the children showed that they were making use of other reading strategies such as the sound-letter relationship or graphic identification when reading the story. This finding` further verified that young EFL learners were also able to develop their ability of recognizing letters, words, or even sentences without formal phonemic awareness or phonics training under the meaningful venue of storytelling. Findings of the interview with the parents about their children’s family literacy environment showed that parents’ concepts about children’s literacy development strongly influenced what they did to support their children’s literacy development and how they offered literacy activities to assist their children always received positive feedback from their children. Interestingly to note, parents of the most and the second most advanced children among the five young EFL learners revealed the information that they started the literate activities very early at home to foster their children’s literacy development by reading stories or environmental print like newspaper or TV ads. On the other hand, with the assistance of the parents, these two children, among the 5 participants, showed early finger-point and prediction during the storybook reading. Moreover, book access 簡 郁 娟 、 黃 月 貴 ( 民 89) 。 Starting with predictable stories: EFL children’s oral and literacy development. Selected Papers from the Ninth International Symposium on English Teaching and Learning, 264-273. 046-3 was a critical element influencing the child’s literacy development. In general, children whose parents value the importance of literacy development have positive attitudes toward their children’s emergent literacy and thus encourage their reading-like behaviors. The results of the research revealed that EFL children can establish reading readiness and construct meaning through reading predictable storybooks even if they have limited English proficiency. With exposure to written language meaningfully and purposefully, most of the children developed book concepts, direction concepts and print awareness and, more impressively, they were able to use the graph-phonemic connection to make sense of the text without formal phonemic awareness or phonics training. In addition, parental awareness and involvement of home literacy were positively correlated to the child’s literacy development. Hsieh, L. T. (2000). Reading aloud vs. silent reading in EFL reading. Studies in English Language and Literature, 8, 13-21. 047 Abstract: The study investigated the effects of teacher reading aloud, self-reading aloud and silent reading on college students’ reading comprehension. Findings indicated that the teacher reading aloud group and selfreading aloud group had higher scores than the silent reading group in reading comprehension tests. Also, the intermediate-low proficiency readers preferred reading aloud while intermediate-high proficiency readers preferred silent reading on their own. Design: 44 first-year college students (intermediate-low proficiency learners), their average age was 15.5. & 44 second-year students (intermediate-high proficiency learners) with average age 16.6 years old; 3 reading passages and an after-reading questionnaire; students from the 2 grades were randomly divided into 3 groups self read aloud, read silently, or the teacher read aloud (the teacher read the passages for 2 times) Findings: Results showed that college students benefited more from teacher’ reading aloud and self-reading aloud on their reading comprehension tests than silent reading. Interestingly, the researcher noted that less skilled readers seemed more relied on the reading aloud approach; on the other hand, more skilled readers preferred to read silently, which was coincided with previous findings. The results verified the positive influence of reading aloud on even older EFL learners’ language acquisition. Note that in teacher’s reading aloud group, the teacher read the passages aloud twice to provide more exposure of the reading, which may provide possible definite explanation of the better performance of the group than other groups. Dhaif, H. (1990). Reading aloud for comprehension: a neglected teaching aid. Reading in a Foreign Language, 7, 457-464. 048-1 Abstract: The purpose of the study was to examine the effects of reading aloud on EFL learners’ reading comprehension. Results showed that students in the teacher’s reading aloud group gained significantly higher scores than their peers in silent reading situation, which indicated that reading aloud had a positive effect on EFL learners’ reading comprehension. Moreover, the questionnaire revealed that 77% of the participants reported that they were in favor of the teacher reading aloud to them. Design: EFL; two methods: students’ silent reading & teacher’s reading aloudreading comprehension; 140 first-year crossed disciplinary university students enrolled; 6 passages were used Findings: The reading comprehension tests revealed that students in the teacher reading aloud group had significantly higher scores than their counterparts in the silent reading group. Coincidently with Elley (1989), the less skilled students made the most progress through reading aloud. Dramatically, these students improved by about 74% in the second test. The results showed that poor readers could benefit from a more “supportive” reading condition whereby they were helped with the more knowledgeable readers. On the other hand, 77% of the subjects indicated a preference for the teacher’s reading aloud for them. Among them, 47% mentioned that the reason was that it helps them better understand the overall meaning of the text. According to the researcher, learners with basic level of English proficiency had a better understanding of what they were reading in a teacher reading aloud situation than in a silent reading situation. Furthermore, Dhaif claims that teachers should set aside lesson time for reading aloud to their students, particularly to those poor readers who were unable to read fluently and hence always had reading comprehension difficulties. It is hoped that reading aloud would help those less skilled learners to improve their level of reading comprehension and consequently encourage them to read to break the vicious circle: poor readers do not enjoy reading because they Dhaif, H. (1990). Reading aloud for comprehension: a neglected teaching aid. Reading in a Foreign Language, 7, 457-464. 048-2 do not understand, and because they do not understand, they do not read. Note: This research provided evidence of the effect of reading aloud on EFL learners with basic proficiency level of English. Dhaif argued that a non-native speaker goes through similar learning process as those experienced by a child learning to read in its first language. With the findings, the researcher further pointed out that EFL learners, despite their limited reading proficiency, were able to comprehend the given text when the help from a more advanced reader was available. Specifically, Dhaif revealed the fact that in those beginning readers, confined by their English reading ability, tend to break the sentences into small parts while they read. As a result the sentences lose their integrity and consequently become meaningless. Reading aloud to these learners at eh early sage would therefore restore that integrity and present larger semantic units which lead to better understanding. Thus, with sufficient practice and active participation in the reading aloud, the readers would be encouraged to follow a more holistic approach to reading which would be triggered off by their realization that a higher level of comprehension can only be achieved by reading larger chunks of texts and not by attempting to understand individual words or bits of sentences. Wang, P. L. (2007). The effectiveness and difficulties of implementing children’s literature in EFL writing. Proceedings of the 24th Conference on English Teaching and Learning, 329-341. 049-1 Abstract: The purpose of the study is to determine whether children’s literature can be an effective instructional aid in enhancing the writing competence in vocational college English majors, The participants were provides with opportunities to listen to stories, read stories and write their own stories. Results revealed the progress of the participants’ writing skills and their attitude towards reading storybooks and implementing storybooks into regular curriculum. Design: 1 semester study; 91 college students (39 first graders of the two-year college program and 52 fourth graders of the five-year program majoring in Applied Foreign Language), 35 storybook were utilized; writing analysis and questionnaires analyzed; 3 writings produced by the participants; T-unit: to examine the participants’ syntactic maturity Findings: Results indicated that almost every five-year and two-year college students liked and accepted storybooks as English learning materials According to the positive responses, storybook are lovely and interesting were the most common reasons. Also, storybooks were easy to understand and they helped increase curiosity were all the priorities making by the students. Surprisingly, half (44%) of the English majors of the five-year college had not read storybooks before the intervention. Astonishingly, 85% of the English-majored students in the two-year college had not have the experience of reading a single storybook before. After the reading aloud by the instructor in class, 54% of the students in five-year college have read at least 1 to 5 books, 15% students have read 6 to 10 storybooks and one student have even read more than 10 storybooks. That implied the fact that the implementing of children’s literature motivate approximately 70% of the students to make extra efforts for their English learning. Also, 75% of the participants believed that story-writing tasks helped them enhance their writing competence and 85% of them believed that kind of task enhanced both Wang, P. L. (2007). The effectiveness and difficulties of implementing children’s literature in EFL writing. Proceedings of the 24th Conference on English Teaching and Learning, 329-341. 049-2 their writing ability and creativity. On the other hand, after being read to by the teacher, 56% of the students in tw0-year college have read 1 to 5 storybooks, 15% of them have read 6 to 10 storybooks and only one student has read more than 10 storybooks during the study. The findings implied that implementing children’s literature motivated nearly 75% of the students to read on their own. 79% of the students believed that their writing competence could be improved by continuing to read and write. And nearly 70% of them thought that storywriting tasks not only help boost their writing ability but also increase their creativity. Descriptive statistics showed that, after being read to, the students made significant progress in their writings. Both the groups, the students in five-year and two-year college, performed much better in their second writing than the first one. However, there’s no significant progress comparing the second and the third writing. The findings showed that teacher’s reading aloud storybooks to college students did increase their writing skills. On the other hand, the researcher failed to find the contribution of reading aloud children’s literature in achieving vocational college students’ correctness in grammatical use. Namely, the researcher concluded that the intervention did not help the students developed the ability of correcting grammatical errors in their writing, which should be reconsidered thoroughly in that errors should be regarded common and acceptable in second or foreign learners and should not be intentionally be removed just after a designed intervention. Error correction, after all, has been empirically found not so effective or even harmful in language learners. In addition, Wang proposed that one of the major difficulties teachers would encounter when involving such literature-based program is that it is not easy to find one appropriate storybook that satisfied every student in the classroom. Still, some of the student may perceive reading aloud storybooks as a form of entertainment like watching movies and thus pay less Wang, P. L. (2007). The effectiveness and difficulties of implementing children’s literature in EFL writing. Proceedings of the 24th Conference on English Teaching and Learning, 329-341. 049-3 attention to the teacher’s reading aloud. This, to me, might be explained in the following ways. First, before the program, the teacher should give her students a pre-orientation in which some relative second language theories should be emphasized. Or at least, the contribution of reading aloud in language acquisition should be released to those learners, which provides the opportunity of learning more about language acquisition theory and also have the learners be aware of the self-responsibility of learning English. Next, if the students perceived the activity interesting and low-stress, the outcome of language learning would be expectantly positive. Last, Wang summarized that the reading aloud children’s storybooks to vocational college students boosted their writing competence and instilled positive attitude toward children’s literature, which is worthy note because the study further confirmed the interlinked relationships between reading and writing. Most importantly, the researcher demonstrated that reading aloud, a common literate activity for young children, can even be utilized in older learners and what’s more, in EFL language situation. Lee, S.Y. (民 94). 晨光繪本閱讀活動與字彙習得 050-1 Abstract: The purpose of the study is to investigate the effect of reading storybooks aloud in early morning on EFL elementary children’s vocabulary acquisition. Design: 20-week design; 117 third-grade participants in Taipei county; without control group; vocabulary test (30% of the items are from the textbook used in the school and the other 70% of the items are from the storybooks and the frequency of the items are three to four times appeared in the storybooks) and questionnaire; 13 storybooks with interesting illustrations, storyline, and rhythmic patterns; pretest to select highly-interesting storybooks Findings: After being read to by university students for 20 weeks, information from the questionnaires revealed that 86.3% of the students hoped that the storytelling activity could continue; 72.6% of the participants thought that English learning became more interesting and 65% of the 117 children perceived that the English course in school became easier after the experience. These findings reflected that reading aloud storybooks indeed stimulated young EFL learners’ learning interests in English and reading as well. Also, such reading program facilitated students’ language development. According to the readers (the university students), some observations discussed here. Firstly, in terms of vocabulary learning, the readers found that blackboard use might enhance the vocabulary learning during the reading aloud. Also, the readers may use the story to link to children’s personal life experience, which would help the reader to better understand the perception of the students. Next, L1 might be used to explain unknown words encountered in the storybooks to enhance the comprehension and children’s interests in listening to the story. Besides, as for the design of the activities used during the reading, the readers found that if possible, the second reading would better arouse students’ interest in reading. Reasons for this might be first, after the first reading, the second reading would be easier to comprehend and the second reading reinforce the newly-acquired words. Also, the second reading may elicit discussions between the reader and the Lee, S.Y. (民 94). 晨光繪本閱讀活動與字彙習得 050-2 Children and that enhance the learning subconsciously. As for the discussion, the readers would not force the learners to produce English for response but encourage them to use some simple English or just respond in their mother language. The readers confirmed that discussions were a very important activity after the reading. Though not as expectant, TPR still played an important role in children’s learning English. That is, through the imitation, the children had better interaction with the reader. As for the difficulties, the readers mentioned that the interaction between the reader and the children plays a critical role in reading aloud. Namely, the better the interaction, the better the effect of the reading aloud. Furthermore, the interruption by some irrelevant questions posed by the children during the reading aloud made the reading not fluent and sometimes that would influence the classroom order, which need extra efforts to deal with. Last, due to the not-appropriate seat arrangement, enot every child would have a clear vision of the storybook, which might decrease the interests in listening to the story. To solve the problem, the reader had to move around to make sure that every student had a better vision of the pictures in the storybook. On the other hand, the descriptive statistics showed that all the four classes made significant progress in total vocabulary, school vocabulary and storybook vocabulary. Interesting enough, the researcher found that there’s no significant difference in the vocabulary performance between those go to English cram school or not. Therefore, whether more instruction time would elicit better language outcome need further examination. Also, the results revealed that comprehension and reading per se are intercorrelated to vocabulary acquisition rather than the hours of the class or the activities in class. Students released the information about their preference of classroom activities. The first priority was singing, the second, storytelling, and the third, drams. However, storytelling was also the second least liking activity to those children. One possible explanation for the contrast might be the level of comprehension that made such paradoxical responses to storytelling in learners, which indicated the importance of comprehension in storytelling. Lee, S.Y. (民 94). 晨光繪本閱讀活動與字彙習得 050-3 As for the preference of activities done in outside school English class, as in school, the first priority was singing, the second, drama, and the third, storytelling. Additionally, the most disliking activity was writing, the second, grammar instruction, and the third, vocabulary exercise. From these results, the researcher urged that reading aloud should be included into regular curriculum. Li, S. C. (2003). A study of the effects of reading fairy tales aloud on junior high school students. 051-1 Abstract: The purpose of the thesis were first to explore the application of fairy tales on EFL junior high students’ reading comprehension. Second, to examine whether reading aloud literature successfully enhanced the students’ learning interests. Design: reading comprehension pretest; 78 third-grade junior high students, 2 groups: the Experimental were read to before reading the texts by themselves, the Control reading by themselves; 5 fairy tales; immediate reading response report; post-test questionnaire; a take-a-book-home activity (to instigate whether reading aloud encouraged voluntary reading after the intervention) Findings: After one-semester research, the students conceived that they enjoyed experiences with the stories and became familiar with the language of literature. Overall, students possessed positive attitude toward reading literature. Illustrations, unlike young children, failed to enhance reading comprehension. According to the researcher, pictorial clues served the entertainment role in reading with little facilitating impact for ninth EFL graders. As for the genres the students preferred, jokes and comic ranked the top twp. Generally, they revealed that they tend to prefer interesting, relevant to themes, or stimulating reading materials. To the experimental group, reading aloud was reported as effective in increasing reading comprehension and fostering a love for reading. 74 % of the experimental group showed their appreciation to reading aloud in English reading classes. Moreover, as stated by the participants, reading aloud helped them developed a greater insight into the characters’ feelings and thoughts and thus they could visualize the plot development as the teacher read aloud the story to them. 李淑純 051-2 Abstract: The purpose of the thesis were first to explore the application of fairy tales on EFL junior high students’ reading comprehension. Second, to examine whether reading aloud literature successfully enhanced the students’ learning interests. Design: reading comprehension pretest; 78 third-grade junior high students, 2 groups: the Experimental were read to before reading the texts by themselves, the Control reading by themselves; 5 fairy tales; immediate reading response report; post-test questionnaire; a take-a-book-home activity (to instigate whether reading aloud encouraged voluntary reading after the intervention) Findings: After one-semester research, the students conceived that they enjoyed experiences with the stories and became familiar with the language of literature. Overall, students possessed positive attitude toward reading literature. Illustrations, unlike young children, failed to enhance reading comprehension. According to the researcher, pictorial clues served the entertainment role in reading with little facilitating impact for ninth EFL graders. As for the genres the students preferred, jokes and comic ranked the top twp. Generally, they revealed that they tend to prefer interesting, relevant to themes, or stimulating reading materials. To the experimental group, reading aloud was reported as effective in increasing reading comprehension and fostering a love for reading. 74 % of the experimental group showed their appreciation to reading aloud in English reading classes. Moreover, as stated by the participants, reading aloud helped them developed a greater insight into the characters’ feelings and thoughts and thus they could visualize the plot development as the teacher read aloud the story to them. Statistical analysis revealed that reading aloud facilitated the participants’ reading comprehension. That is, regard the comprehension test, the experimental group outperformed the control group in linger texts. results of the study further confirmed the hypothesis. The experimental group demonstrated more favorable attitudes toward readings than the control group and, outperformed on the longer and more difficult readings 李淑純 051-3 texts. This may result from the fact that students were more able to comprehend given reading texts while the written language was delivered orally and simultaneously. Also, the more positive attitude toward reading and the better performance of the experimental group in reading comprehension verified the hypothesis that the positive relationship between reading interests and reading performance. Unexpectedly, the take-a-book-home activity seemed not so stimulating to encourage more outside reading after the program. Reasons might be that for these ninth-grade junior high students, the Basic Competence Exam is approaching, and this caused the students lot of pressure and therefore failed to do extra reading outside of class. Besides, the students’ proficiency level might be another impediment to the infrequency of outside-class reading. And this indicated the important role of the teacher in young EFL learners’ language development. To encourage more outside reading, Lee proposed that teachers should convince students of the value of outside reading and prepare a plan for maintaining their interests in reading. In a word, the teacher should also be a counselor, an observer, and a partner in such literate activity to achieve reciprocal relationship and promising language outcome. Valdez-Menchaca, M. C., & Whitehurst, G. J. (1992). Accelerating language development through picture book reading: A systematic extension to Mexican day care. Developmental Psychology, 28, 1106-1114. 052-1 Abstract: The study introduced dialogic reading in 20 Mexican 2-year-olds from low-income backgrounds. Children in the intervention group were read to individually by a teacher using dialogic reading techniques. The control group children were given individual arts and crafts instruction by the same teacher. Effects of the intervention were assessed through standardized language tests and by comparing the children’s spontaneous language while they shared a picture book with an adult who was unaware of their group assignment. Differences favoring the intervention group were found on all posttests and one some measures of languge production. Design: twenty 2-year-old Mexican children;2 groups: the Expe.--> one-on-one reading with an adult; pretestsPPVT-R & EOWPVT; posttest: PPVT-R, EOWPVT & ITPA (expressive test); 5 storybooks with rich illustrations and short, descriptive text; 6- to- 7 week intervention duration; children’s spontaneous verbalization (the children interacted with an unfamiliar adult who asked specific or open-ended questions to elicit the children’s language production); ESL Findings: Outcome from the standardized tests revealed a significant group effect, indicating a higher performance by children in the experimental group than by children in the control group. These positive language outcome in the intervention group showed that the picture book reading produced large effects on performance. Moreover, verbal production data showed that children in the experimental group produced a significant greater number of utterances than children in the control group. Also, the intervention group produced longer and more complex sentences (i.e. number of compound sentences, reflecting a higher level of syntactic complexity of their speech. Valdez-Menchaca, M. C., & Whitehurst, G. J. (1992). Accelerating language development through picture book reading: A systematic extension to Mexican day care. Developmental Psychology, 28, 11061114. 052-2 Furthermore, the intervention group produced more diverse word categories and greater various nouns and verbs. Next, children in the experimental group were more able in answering questions posed by the responsible adult and continuing topic-related discussions. That is, the children in the experimental group produced higher rates of these types of verbalizations than control children. The results of the research extended those of Whitehurst et al. (1988) by demonstrating that a shared picture book reading can work for Mexican children of low-income parents with below-normal language abilities and with a day-care teacher rather than a parent in the adult role. These two studies demonstrated that large and enduring effects on children’s language can be obtained from an intervention that encouraged the child to talk about the pictures in shared picture books and provided the child with appropriate models and feedback for progressively more sophisticated language use. With the success of language outcomes appeared in the experimental group in the MLU, the diversity of verbs and nouns, and the children’s efforts to initiate and continue conversation, the researchers concluded that picture book reading as a critical component of preschoolers’ language development by demonstrating that active participation in picture book reading produced language gains for children of low-income families. More importantly, Valdez- Menchaca and Whitehurst believed that continuous exposure to picture book activities would produce larger and more lasting effects than a 6-week intervention.