INVESTIGATING THE CWH: AN N400 MICROSUMMARY Ellen Lau LING646 May 23, 2005 I. Introduction The N400 effect was introduced to the language literature with a within-sentence implausibility paradigm (Kutas & Hillyard, 1980), and so we often tend to think of the effect as one which indexes ease of semantic integration, at some level. A provocative alternative hypothesis recently argued by our colleague, Carol Whitney, is that the N400 effect indexes simple lexical activation—in other words, “it’s just priming” and doesn’t relate to any higher level integration processes. She suggests that it only looks like it does because, in many of the paradigms used, words within the sentence associatively prime the lexical item involved in plausible and implausible continuations differentially. This hypothesis is supported by studies which have shown the same level of N400 amplitude reduction for sentences incorporating strong semantic category relationships (A carrot is a vegetable) as false sentences incorporating the same type of relationships at the lexical level (A carrot is not a vegetable) (Fischler, Bloom, Childers, Roucos, & Perry, 1983). Similarly suggestive are recent studies which fail to show an N400 effect for sentences with semantically compatible arguments but whose argument structures are completely implausible (e.g. van Herten, Kolk, & Chwilla, 2005; Kim & Osterhout, 2005; Hoeks, Stowe, & Doedens, 2004). In this paper, I investigate some selected work from the massive N400 literature which can bear on Whitney’s hypothesis (henceforth, CWH). For this review, I chose to focus on findings regarding the N400 effect to priming at the lexical vs the sentence-level; however, another important area of work that speaks to this topic is that which has shown N400 effects modulated by changes at the discourse level, which presumably will be somewhat harder to capture with a lexically-based story (e.g. van Berkum, Hagoort, & Brown, 1999; Nieuwland & van Berkum, submitted; Hagoort, Hald, Bastiaansen, & Petersson, 2004). II. Automatic vs Conscious/Strategic Priming Because CWH reduces sentence-level N400 effects to lexical-level ones, it makes sense to take a look at the N400 work on lexical priming. Within the lexical priming literature itself, an influential distinction is made which is somewhat analogous to CWH’s integrative-vs-associative one: this is the distinction between ‘automatic’ and ‘strategic’ priming effects. By ‘automatic’, researchers are usually referring to the spreading activation theory of priming (Collins & Loftus, 1975). On this theory, lexical 2 representations are imagined as nodes in a network, and lexical activations that arise in response to prime perception spread through the “nearby” nodes of the network, such that full activation of “primed” nodes is faster subsequently. ‘Strategic’ in general covers all processes which require some extra step besides simply prime perception for priming to occur. For example, one strategic process thought to be responsible for semantic priming in lexical decision tasks is semantic matching (e.g. Neely & Keefe, 1989; De Groot, 1985). Here, in order to make a rapid lexical decision, the target is quickly compared to the prime, and if they share some amount of features, an affirmative decision can be made. Another example is expectancy priming (e.g. Becker, 1980; De Groot, 1984), in which a prediction (or predictions) are made for the target based on the prime, and the lexical decision is speeded to the extent that the target matches the predictions. Automatic priming has traditionally been thought to be fast and ‘early’, and strategic priming to be slower or ‘late’, for obvious reasons—strategic priming by definition requires more processing steps. Several manipulations have been used in the behavioral literature to try to dissociate automatic from strategic effects. Word naming tasks are thought to be less sensitive to strategic effects than lexical decision tasks (Balota & Lorch, 1986; de Groot, 1984; Seidenberg, M., Waters, G. S., Sanders, M. & Langer, P.,1984). Using a double lexical decision task rather than a non-response prime followed by a lexical decision target has also been argued to reduce strategic effects (McNamara & Altarriba, 1988). However, the main technique used within the lexical decision paradigm is masked priming. In masked priming, a briefly presented prime is immediately followed by a mask (e.g. #####), and then the target. Using this technique, researchers can present primes without conscious perception by participants (Marcel, 1983; Cheesman & Merikle, 1984; Forster & Davis, 1984). It’s not completely clear how the CWH aligns with the automatic-strategic distinction with respect to N400 effects—namely, I’m not sure whether CWH would lump processes like expectancy priming together with sentence-level integrative processes, or whether these would still be considered within basic lexical priming processes. However, I think that CWH would at the least expect the automatic, spreading activation priming to be underlying some of the N400 effect. What is interesting is that, in the early literature on N400 priming, the results appeared to be exactly the opposite—it looked like the N400 was only sensitive to strategic effects, while not showing any sensitivity to automatic, spreading activation priming. Once the N400 was shown to be sensitive to lexical priming effects (McCarthy & Nobre, 1993), the question naturally arose whether the N400 amplitude was reflecting strategic processes, automatic processes, or both. Early work relevant to this question showed that the N400 effect was larger when participants were encouraged to use strategic processing within a lexical decision priming experiment (Holcomb 1988), a finding that suggested that the N400 did reflect strategic processing, but that didn’t speak directly to the relevance of the automatic priming component. This question was addressed by a very influential paper by Brown and Hagoort (1993), who tested masked priming effects in ERP. Brown and Hagoort reported no N400 effect when primes were masked below the 3 level of conscious perception, in contrast to the conditions in which primes were unmasked. This seemed to settle the issue; the N400 reflects ‘strategic’, ‘late’ effects only, not early ‘automatic’ effects. Furthermore, this was actually a promising finding for the lexical priming field, as until this point there were no paradigms that were able to isolate the ‘strategic’ stage, in contrast with the ‘automatic’ stage. Indeed, Brown and Hagoort followed these results with several studies that took the ‘strategic’ nature of the N400 effect as a given and used the presence vs absence of the N400 effect as a dependent measure to determine what kinds of processes were underlying various types of priming manipulations (e.g. mediated priming, Chwilla, Kolk, & Mulder, 2000; backwards priming, Chwilla, Hagoort, & Brown, 1998). As I suggested above, these results would seem to be at odds with at least the spirit of the CWH, which claims that all N400 effects, even sentence-level, arise from manipulations of relative lexical activation; although perhaps we could argue that some ‘strategic’ processes like expectancy priming or semantic matching could directly lead to differences in relative ease of activation. However, in recent years, more and more studies have begun to cast doubt on Brown and Hagoort’s (1993) seminal finding. Firstly, Rolke, Heil, Streb, and Henninghausen (2001) showed that N400 effects showed up for prime words presented during the attentional blink, suggesting that conscious perception of the prime was not a necessary component. More directly contradictory, Kiefer (2002) showed N400 modulation by masked primes presented with a very short SOA, and showed with various behavioral measures that participants seemed to show no conscious perception of the prime. Interestingly, however, the latency of the N400 difference was delayed by about 80 ms in the masked prime case relative to the unmasked case. Kiefer argued that Brown and Hagoort failed to find the effect because they used too long of an SOA (500 ms) to capture the shortlived masked priming effect. If this account is correct, then Kiefer’s finding may not make much different for CWH regarding sentence-level effects, because crucially, if lexical priming is causing the effects at the sentence-level, it is really going to need to be long-lasting enough to exert influences over at least several words. Most recently however, Misra and colleagues using masked primes have showed repetition priming effects on the N400 which span over as many as four trials, casting doubt on this hypothesis (Misra & Holcomb, 2003; Holcomb, Reder, Misra, & Grainger, 2005). They suggest that the inconsistency with Brown and Hagoort (1993) because their task—semantic monitoring—may have made processing deep enough that the effect was measurable, in contrast to a task like lexical decision. The results suggest that it is at least plausible that the N400 effect seen in sentence-level paradigms is driven in part by spreading activation/associative priming. However, Misra and colleagues do find that the N400 effect is larger in the unmasked cases, which is also consistent with the earlier work by Holcomb (1988) showing a larger effect with more strategic processing. Therefore, I think that the CWH is going to have to admit that at least some other mechanism besides simple associative priming feeds into the N400. At the same time, however, it is still quite possible that this other mechanism is much more basic than integrating the incoming word with the entire semantic structure of the preceding sentence fragment. 4 III. Associatively based priming vs other relationships If, as CWH postulates, the N400 effect is fundamentally a lexical priming effect, we would like to get a better idea of exactly what mechanisms are underlying this priming effect. Therefore, is there any work which could speak to what ‘strategic’ mechanisms are behind the increased size of the effect in the unmasked case? In the priming literature, some work has tried filtering out associative priming effects by using word pairs which are not semantically related, but are not strictly speaking “associates” of each other, in the sense that they are not elicited by participants in free-association norming tasks. Priming effects between such pairs is thought to be more likely to be caused by a strategy like semantic matching than either expectancy-based or spreading activation, both of which would presumably be correlated with free-association responses. In principle, then, we could look at parallel N400 studies in which the N400 effect was compared for associative priming and purely semantically-related priming. An early paper by Hagoort, Brown, and Swaab (1996) compared N400 effects to associatively and semantically related prime-target pairs and purely semantically related pairs in aphasics and elderly controls. In the controls, they showed equal-sized N400 priming effects for the two kinds of pairs. More recently, Kovisto and Revonsuo (2001) made a more direct comparison, in which they compared the N400 response to purely semantically related primes (sofa-bed) and purely ‘associative’ primes, for which they used opaque compound pairs (wind-mill). They also showed a similarly sized decrease in N400 amplitude for both pairs. Interestingly, however, the purely associative pairs showed an effect of a longer duration than the semantically related pairs, and this effect in the later part of the N400 window seemed to have a different topography than earlier. Therefore, Kovisto and Revonsuo hypothesize that the N400 in the associative case is a combined effect of several different processes. This is an intriguing idea, and, were it to hold up, it could provide an interesting diagnostic of the mechanisms underlying the sentence-level effects that CWH is questioning—however, this is the only work I could find making such a comparison so far, so it is quite preliminary. Several other papers which speak to the mechanism underlying the N400 lexical priming effect are a pair of papers by Chwilla and colleagues (Chwilla, Hagoort, & Brown, 1998; Chwilla, Kolk & Mulder; 2000). As mentioned above, these papers assumed based on Hagoort and Brown (1993) that the N400 did not reflect automatic spreading activation priming, and so they used it as a dependent measure to test the contribution of automatic priming to specific types of priming relationships. Although the base assumption turned out to be wrong, the effects that they find can still tell us something about the mechanisms that the N400 reflects. Chwilla, Kolk, and Mulder (2000) investigate the N400 in conjunction with two-step or mediated priming. Mediated prime pairs are not associated with each other, but rather are linked by a shared associate (e.g. lion-stripes, where tiger is the mediator). Mediated priming can be explained either by spreading activation or by certain versions of semantic matching (Gillund & Shiffrin, 1984). The authors show N400 effects for both regular (one-step) and mediated (two-step) priming, but the effect for the mediated 5 primes is much smaller. Given that we know from later work that the N400 reflects both automatic and strategic effects, this difference between one- and two-step priming could mean that the N400 varies parametrically with semantic-network distance (which is assumed to be greater in the two-step case) or that the N400 is a summed contribution of the automatic and strategic effects, only one of which (automatic) is present in the mediated case. The other paper in the pair lends more support to the second idea. In Chwilla, Hagoort, and Brown (1998), the authors examine a priming effect known as backwards priming between word pairs that are associated in one direction but not the other (stork-baby vs baby-stork) (Koriat, 1981). Priming effects on RTs have been found in both directions, although sometimes they are found to be larger in the forwards cases (Thompson-Schill, Kurtz, & Gabrieli, 1998; Peterson & Simpson, 1989; Seidenberg, Waters, Sanders, & Langer, 1984; Koriat, 1981). Chwilla and colleagues showed N400 effects for bidirectional, forwards-only, and backwards-only pairs, although the effect was larger for bidirectional pairs; additionally, they showed that these effects were robust at both short (0 ms) and longer (500 ms) ISIs. How can we understand the difference in the size of the effect in the bidirectional vs the unidirectional cases, when both the bidirectional and forwards cases should have been equally associated? The authors don’t explain this difference. However, the forwards and backwards pairs were largely made up of partially opaque compounds, which means they may have been mainly associatively related, rather than semantically related1. In contrast, the bidirectional pairs were made up from first or second associates from free association norms, which probably resulted in an increased amount of semantic relatedness in this condition. Therefore, if we take the Kovisto and Revonsuo result to indicate that both associative and semantic relatedness contribute to the N400 effect, then we can predict the result that the bidirectional condition shows a larger effect, if we assume that these contributions are at least partially additive. IV. Direct comparisons of word and sentence level effects CWH argues that effects of sentence-level contextual fit of words on the N400 can be explained by lexical-level priming. A few studies have looked at the similarity between lexical- and sentence- level effects; for example, Kutas (1993) compared the profile of N400 effects for lexical priming and sentential anomaly within the same experiment, and found their topography, amplitude, and timing to be almost indistinguishable. But, perhaps most relevant to the CWH question, Van Petten (1993) actually directly manipulated the association between words within a sentential context. She examined sentences like the following: 1 Partially opaque compound pairs actually seems in retrospect like a strange choice for the materials, given that the authors thought that the N400 reflected a semantic matching strategy! 6 In the congruent associated case, the critical word was both congruent with the context and strongly associated with a word in the previous context (moon-stars). In the congruent unassociated case, the critical word was congruent with the context, but did not have any strong associations with other words in the sentence. To complete the design, van Petten also compared cases in which the critical word was incongruent with the context in either the presence or absence of lexical association. For each condition, van Petten compared the waveforms of the first italicized word (the associate or control) and the second, critical word. Under CWH, N400 reduction should take place solely on the basis of lexical association. Therefore, the simplest prediction of the hypothesis would be that the associated conditions would show an N400 reduction, and the unassociated ones would not. On the other hand, if the N400 also reflects ease of contextual-sentential integration of the lexical item, N400 reduction should also take place in the congruent unassociated condition, as the increased amount of preceding context for the second word of the pair should make integration faster. 7 The results showed that all conditions except the anomalous unassociated condition showed a reduction in N400, with the same latency. This would seem to support the hypothesis that the N400 is sensitive to contextual integration, contra CWH. However, the relative amplitudes of the effects are suggestive; the congruent unassociated N400 effect looks smaller than in either the congruent associated or the anomalous associated conditions. Furthermore, the effect in the anomalous associated condition does not look smaller than the effect in the congruent associated condition, although we would expect a difference in the relative size of effect if the associative and contextual effects were somewhat additive as we saw in the case of associative and strategic effects in the lexical priming N400 work. These facts suggest a way in which the spirit of the CWH could perhaps be reconciled with these results. We can maintain that the N400 crucially reflects lexical activation, not integration, if we can describe all the sentence-level effects as expectancy based, by which the sentence-level effect becomes a special case of expectancy-based priming. In the congruent associated condition, the critical word is within a small field of lexical items being weakly predicted in that position, and this increases the base activation on this item so as to create a priming effect when it is encountered, similarly to the expectancy-based priming effect in lexical decision (de Groot, 1984). The only caveat is that since expectancy-based priming is seen as a strategic, ‘post-lexical’ effect, we may be forced to add the extra assumption that reading comprehension involves something analogous to the overt ‘decision’ that differentiates access from decision stages in lexical access. The idea that the N400 is at least partly expectancy-driven has a long history in the literature, which I won’t delve into here (see, for example, Kutas, Lindamood, & Hillyard, 1984). In particular, recent work by Federmeier and colleagues has demonstrated N400 reduction for semantically anomalous continuations which belong to the same semantic category as a highly probable continuation (Federmeier & Kutas, 1999; Coulson, Federmeier, Van Petten, & Kutas, 2005). Thus, this version of CWH has reasonable empirical support. V. Conclusion In this paper, I have examined if and how a version of the hypothesis that the N400 indexes solely lexical-level effects (i.e., lexical activation ‘effort’) could be implemented given existing findings. Based on the literature on lexical priming and the N400, I conclude that, in such an implementation, we must define the relevant ‘lexical-level effects’ that the N400 indexes so as to include so-called ‘late’ priming processes such as semantic matching or expectancy, as well as ‘early’ processes, namely spreading activation. Based on Van Petten’s (1993) study, I conclude that we must adopt a completely expectancy-based explanation of sentence-level effects in order to maintain the idea that the N400 indexes only effects of lexical activation, not integration. However, the hypothesis thus implemented seems to be compatible with the areas of the 8 literature we have reviewed here. However, as I alluded to in the introduction, the hypothesis may still face a challenge from the research on discourse-level effects on the N400. 9 BIBLIOGRAPHY Balota, D. A. & Lorch, R. F. (1986). Depth of automatic spreading activation: Mediated priming effects in pronunciation but not in lexical decision. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 12, 336-345. Cheesman, J., & Merikle, P.M. (1984). Priming with and without awareness. Perception and Psychophysics, 36, 387-395. Collins, A.M. & Loftus E.F. (1975) A spreading–activation theory of semantic processing, Psychol. Rev. 82, 407–428. Coulson S, Federmeier K, Van Petten C, & Kutas M (2005). Right hemisphere sensitivity to word and sentence-level context. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 31,129-147.) De Groot, A.M.B. (1985). Word-context effects in word naming and lexical decision. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 37A, 281± 297. De Groot, A.M.B. (1984). Primed lexical decision: Combined effects of the proportion of related prime target pairs and the stimulus-onset asynchrony of prime and target. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 36A, 253- 280. Federmeier, K.D. and Kutas, M. (1999) A rose by any other name: long-term memory structure and sentence processing. J. Mem. Lang. 41, 469–495 Fischler, I. Bloom, P.A., Childers, D.G., Roucos, S.E., & Perry, N.W. (1983) Brain potentials related to stages of sentence verification. Psychophysiology 20, 400–409 Forster, K.I., & Davis, C. (1984). Repetition priming and frequency attentuation in lexical access. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 10, 680-698. Hagoort, P., Hald, L., Bastiaansen, M.C.M., & Petersson, K.M. (2004). Integration of Word Meaning and World Knowledge in Language Comprehension. Science, 304 (5669), 438-440. Gillund, G., & Shiffrin, R. M. (1984). A retrieval model for both recognition and recall. Psychological Review, 91, 1-65. Hagoort, P., Brown, C.M., & Swaab, T.Y. (1996). Lexical-semantic event-related potential effects in patients with left hemisphere lesions with aphasia, and patients with right hemisphere lesions without aphasia. Brain, 119, 627-650. Holcomb, Reder, Misra & Grainger (in press). The effects of prime visibility on ERP measures of masked priming. Cognitive Brain Research. Hoeks, J.C.J., Stowe, L.A., & Doedens, L. H. (2004). Seeing words in context: the interaction of lexical and sentence level information during reading. Cognitive Brain Research, 19(1), 59-73. 10 Kim, A. & Osterhout, L. (2005). The independence of combinatory semantic processing: Evidence from event-related potentials. Journal of Memory & Language, 52, 205-225. Kutas, M. and Hillyard, S.A. (1980) Reading senseless sentences: brain potentials reflect semantic incongruity. Science 207, 203–205 Koivisto & Revonsuo (2001). Cognitive representations underlying the N400 priming effect. Cognitive Brain Research, 12, 487–490 Koriat, A. (1981). Semantic facilitation in lexical decision as a function of prime-target association. Memory and Cognition, 9, 587-598. Kutas, M., Lindamood, T. E., & Hillyard, S. A. (1984). Word expectancy and event-related brain potentials during sentence processing. In S. Kornblum & J. Requin (Eds.), Preparatory States and Processes (pp. 217-237). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Kutas, M. In the company of other words: Electrophysiological evidence for single word versus sentence context effects. Language and Cognitive Processes, 1993, 8(4): 533-572. Marcel, A.J., (1983). Conscious and unconscious perception: Experiments on visual masking and word recognition. Cognitive Psychology, 15, 197-237. McCarthy, G. and Nobre, A.C. (1993) Modulation of semantic processing by spatial selective attention. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 88, 210–219 McNamara, T. P. & Altarriba, J. (1988). Depth of spreading activation revisited: Semantic mediated priming occurs in lexical decisions. Journal of Memory and Language, 27, 545-559. Misra, M. & Holcomb, P.J. (2003). Event-related potential indices of masked repetition priming. Psychophysiology, 40, 115-130. Neely, J.H., & Keefe, D.E. (1989). Semantic context effects on visual word processing: A hybrid Prospective-retrospective processing theory. In G.H. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation: Advances in research and theory (pp. 207- 248). New York: Academic Press. Nieuwland, M.S. & Van Berkum, J.J.A. (submitted). Testing the limits of the semantic illusion phenomenon: ERPs reveal temporary change deafness in discourse comprehension. Peterson, R. R. & Simpson, G. B. (1989). The effect of backward priming on word recognition in single-word and sentence contexts. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 15, 1020-1032. Rolke, B., Heil, M., Streb, J., Henninghausen, E. (2001). Missed prime words within the attentional blink evoke an N400 semantic priming effect, Psychophysiology 38, 165–174. Seidenberg, M. S., Waters, G. S., Sanders, M. & Langer, P. (1984). Pre-and postlexical loci of contextual effects on word recognition. Memory & Cognition, 12, 315-328. Thompson-Schill, S. L., Kurtz, K. J., Gabrieli, J. D. E. (1998). Effects of semantic and associative relatedness on automatic priming. Journal of Memory and Language, 38, 440-458. 11 Van Berkum, J.J.A., Hagoort, P., & Brown, C.M. (1999). Semantic integration in sentences and discourse: Evidence from the N400. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 11(6), 657-671. van Herten, M., Kolk, H. H. J., & Chwilla, D. (2005). An ERP study of P600 effects elicited by semantic anomalies. Cognitive Brain Research, 22, 241-255. Van Petten, C. (1993). A comparison of lexical and sentence-level context effects and their temporal parameters. Language and Cognitive Processes, 8, 485-532.