MEMORY: HOW DOES THIS CURIOUS BRAIN FUNTION WORK? Simone Miguez Cunha (1) (2) & Adriana Benevides Soares (2) (1) Instituto Militar de Engenharia - Seção Psicopedagógica Praça General Tibúrcio, 80 - 22290-270 Rio de Janeiro, RJ (2) Universidade Gama Filho – Programa de Pós-Graduação em Psicologia Rua Manuel Vitorino, 533 – 20740-280 Rio de Janeiro - RJ Memory is an extremely important brain function because it is the basis of all learning. Our knowledge and abilities are stored in our memory, allowing us to orient ourselves in space and time. Knowledge representation happens in this memory context and its processes help us to store and retrieve this knowledge for present and future activities. According to Sternberg (2000), "memory is the way we recall our past experiences in order to use this information in the present." Recent neuropsychological investigations about the workings of memory have yielded a better understanding of its structure, organization and information processing capabilities. Up to fairly recently, cognitive psychologists thought that memory was an almost static process, capable only of passively receiving information to store and subsequently retrieve. In other words, information processing in memory would happen sequentially, with information being coded first, then stored for some time and finally extracted. In the mid-1960s, investigations seemed to show that memory was characterized by the information contained in its storage structures. However, the investigations proceeded and yielded new data which led cognitive psychologists to review the concept of memory and its mechanisms. In this way, new models were developed due to different ways of interpreting the results or just as a means to take new data into account. In the last two decades, investigators were led to believe that memory is a process which involves several operations that occur simultaneously and in parallel. Its functioning allows these operations (coding, storage and retrieval) to interact reciprocally and interdependently in information treatment. MEMORY THEORETICAL MODELS Most theoretical studies of the memory are based on one of two models, the traditional and the alternative, which regard the memory structure and processes from different points of view. One is focused on the study of the memory general structure, while the other seeks to understand the structural organization of the semantic memory (words or concepts). Figure 1 summarizes the evolution of research according to its specific goals. MEMORY Traditional model Alternative model Two Structures Model - 1963 Three Storages Model - 1968 Memory General Structure Semantic Memory Structure Structure by Treatment Hierarchical Structure of Levels Semantic Memory 1972 1969-1974 Multiple Memory Systems Model Network Model 1972 1975 Memory Alternative Taxonomy Connectionist Model 1986 Working Memory 1990 Figure 1: Evolution of research according to specific goals The beginning of memory research is closed related to a "traditional" (Sternberg, 2000) memory model based on three kinds of storage: sensorial, short-term and longterm. Sensorial storage: a structure that can store a very limited quantity of information for brief periods. This structure was considered to be the initial store for many data that subsequently were transferred to short-term and long-term storage. Short-Term storage: a structure than can store information for somewhat longer periods, but also with a very limited capacity. It can hold only seven items plus or minus two. Due to this limited capacity, we developed mechanisms to control the flow of information to short-term storage, repeating it, grouping it or making associations. Long-Term storage: a structure that can store information for long periods of time. This memory holds all the information we need in order to perform our day-to-day activities: people's names, concepts, daily planning, etc. This model emphasizes these structures as passive containers where information is stored and assumes that memory processes have only a limited control on the transfer of information from one storage structure to another. The “alternative” model has as its key feature the concept of working memory, a brief, transient storage structure that keeps the information active during several minutes while it is being processed. This idea emphasizes the control functions performed by the memory, such as coding and integration of long-term storage. This dynamic and continuous character of manipulation and coordination in information integration is what distinguishes the alternative model. The main goal of the researchers who use the alternative model has been to understand how the words and concepts are stored in our long-term memory, having in mind that the fundamental unit of knowledge is the concept, an idea about something. This interest of researchers in understanding the structural organization of semantic memory contrasts with preceding theories, which sought to explain only the memory general structure. Semantic memory deals with concepts, ideas a person can associate with several features and to which she can connect other ideas. Several models were developed with this goal in mind, but none could explain how the concepts are stored in our semantic memory. Based on reviews and critics of these models, an alternative model called connectionist model (parallel distributed processing or PDP) was proposed. According to this model, we manipulate several mental operations simultaneously in order to organize our concepts, store them and ultimately recover them from storage. This information storage is based on the idea of a network, i.e., a series of labeled relationships between nodes, which represent words or concepts. In this model, knowledge is represented by connections between nodes and not by the nodes themselves. When we try to evoke some knowledge, we activate a node, which can activate another node connected to it; this process of spreading activation leads to the activation of additional nodes. This effect is called priming. The PDP model is consistent with the concept of working memory, which is the structure that takes the most recently activated part of the long-term memory and transfers these activated elements in and out of transient memory storage. In other words, the working memory creates a working area where bits of information are used for immediate reasoning, problem solving or behavior guidance, but are soon forgotten. According to Sternberg (2000), these models are hypothetical and are not associated with real physiological structures, serving only as a mental model to describe the mechanisms of memory. Many cognitive psychologists use this model to describe memory phenomena because it is closer to the functioning of the human brain, with it massive parallel processing capabilities. DIFFERENCES BETWEEN THE TWO MODELS Traditional model (three kinds of storage) Sequential processing of information Can be simulated in individual computers Alternative model (parallel processing, working memory) Parallel processing of information in neural networks Cannot be simulated in individual computers, but only in networks MEMORY SYSTEMS Could it be that driving a car and knowing the name of a famous Brazilian writer are abilities that are processed exactly in the same manner? And how do we remember them? According to Larry Squire (in Sternberg, 2000), there is a difference between declarative (explicit) memory and several kinds of non-declarative (implicit) memory (Figure 2). Other authors had already suggested the existence of more than one memory system to organize and store information. They proposed making a distinction between the semantic memory (concerned with words that describe time-independent events), episodic memory (concerned with dates and similar time-dependent events) and procedural memory (concerned with habits and abilities). Different kinds of information are stored and retrieved by different mechanisms. Semantic and episodic memories belong to the declarative (explicit) class; they deal with words and the conscience is involved. Implicit (non-declarative) memory deals with nonverbal concepts and the conscience is not involved. This is the case with procedures, abilities, habits, conditioned behavior and priming. In this way, we can see a clear difference between memorizing numbers or dates and activities such as driving a car, solving a puzzle, playing volleyball etc. Memory Declarative Non-declarative Semantic Episodic Procedural skills Priming Conditionin Non- (facts) (events) (motor, (semantic g associative procedural, and (habituation, cognitive) perceptive) sensitization) Figure 2: Difference between declarative and non-declarative memory MEMORY DEVELOPMENT Memory capacity can be affected by the physiological development of the brain, by the acquisition of information, by new experiences, by environmental conditions and by the use of metacognitive strategies. The physiological development of the brain provides an increase in the complexity of the neural networks, allowing for a more efficient information storage and retrieval. Bartlett (in Sternberg, 2000) has found that knowledge and a priori expectations have a considerable influence on information retrieval, leading in some cases to interference and distortion and in other cases to intensification of memory processes. Our mental framework affects the way we remember what we have learned, making our memory constructive. When we integrate a new information to knowledge already stored in our memory, we are associating something new to a preexisting mental framework; this process is called consolidation. We can increase our storage capacity during the consolidation process by using several metamemory strategies. We store different kinds of information in different ways; therefore, we have to use specific mechanisms to transfer these different kinds of information into long-term storage. Repetition is a well-know strategy to facilitate this storage. The longer the time dedicated to repetition, the longer will people be able to retain the information. However, Sternberg (2000) points out that this transfer must be performed stimulating memory organization in a way that new knowledge it significantly integrated to previous information. When this is the case, the information will be retained for a longer time and will be more easily retrieved. The arrangement of information in memory is also important, since it can facilitate memory operations, significantly decreasing the time needed for retrieval. The above mentioned strategies are widely known, but there are some others, such as the use of associated words, keywords and interactive images, which can also improve memory efficiency. The conclusion is that when you choose an information-coding strategy, you should also worry about the strategies that will be used to recover this information. All these factors contribute to memory development, leading to a more efficient performance. In particular, this kind of study shows how people can use memory more efficiently. MEMORY PHYSIOLOGY Neuropsychological research has shown that some brain structures are involved in memory-related activities; memory, however, is not located in a specific region of the brain. Its functions are probably shared by many different structures, located in different regions, and therefore the loss of a certain brain function can sometimes be compensated by another brain region. The main regions associated with memory are (Figure 3): Cerebral cortex: responsible for the long-term storage of information. Hippocampus: responsible for the coding of declarative information. However, other brain regions are responsible for other forms of declarative memory. Basal ganglia: responsible for procedural memory. Cerebellum: responsible for remembering classically conditioned responses. Basal ganglia Cerebral cortex Cerebellum Hippocampus Figure 3: Main regions associated with memory Neurotransmitters (serotonin, acetylcholine, noradrenalin and the hormones), according to their production in the organism, may contribute positively or negatively to memory function. Memory loss can be due to the consumption of alcohol, which disturbs serotonin activity, as well as to the altered psychological states of stress, anxiety and depression and also to long-term use of some drugs and to vitamin B1 deficiency; all these factors interfere with our learning capacity. MEMORY AND LEARNING Memory and learning are closely related; it would be impossible to acquire new knowledge and modify it subsequently without being able to retain information. When we store information, we construct a mental representation that helps us use what has been learned to solve present and future problems. According to Pozo (1998), research on information handling by the brain is not enough to provide us with a theory of learning, since these studies approach learning and memory as a whole, without studying each of these processes separately. However, many scientific studies suggest a very strong correlation between the two processes. These studies show that information retrieval is affected by the way it is coded. When we try to learn something, our performance will improve if try to relate the new knowledge to old knowledge in several different ways. If one takes care to diversify the codification context, retention can be significantly improved. It is possible to use this strategy to facilitate the development of this principle through project pedagogy. For long-term retention of information, learning must be distributed along several sessions. This concept can be integrated in the curricular grid so as to foster the organization of courses and classes. Research has shown that we remember better when we can relate the information to our mental constructs and our personal experience. In other words, when we make our own clues we have a much higher retention rate than when other people make clues for us. Therefore, one has to ponder about the necessity of revising the teacher's role so that he does not become a central and exclusive element in the learning process and so that learning is not confused with temporary memorization of information utterly divorced from the student daily activities and social reality. School practice oriented so that the student can think and reflect about his own process of knowledge construction enables him to elaborate the new information is a way that is significant to him, integrating it into his previous knowledge. In this way, memory performance is improved and the student becomes more independent. Finally, these discoveries can contribute to the processes of teaching and learning, helping the student to construct his knowledge and improving his performance. The contribution of these conquests to Education and Psychology seem to be uncontestable, having in mind the broadness of their applicability. BIBLIOGRAPHY POZO, J. I. (1998) - Teorias Cognitivas da Aprendizagem. Porto Alegre: Artes Médicas Sul, 1998. STERNBERG, R. J. (2000) - Psicologia Cognitiva. Porto Alegre: Artes Médicas Sul, 2000.