S1 Supplemental Materials Longitudinal Associations Between Social Anxiety Disorder and Avoidant Personality Disorder: A Twin Study by F. A. Torvik et al., 2015, Journal of Abnormal Psychology http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/abn0000124 Table S1 Occurrence and Stability of SAD and AvPD at Wave 1 and Wave 2, Number of Cases SAD W1 no W1 yes Sum AvPD W2 no W2 yes Sum W2 no W2 yes Sum 1,358 56 1,414 1,420 21 1,441 35 18 53 25 10 35 1,393 74 1,467 1,445 31 1,476 Note. SAD = Social anxiety disorder. AvPD = Avoidant personality disorder. Table S2 Co-occurrence of SAD and AvPD at Wave 1 and Wave 2, Number of Cases Wave 1 No AvPD AvPD Sum Wave 2 No SAD SAD Sum No SAD SAD Sum 1,666 51 1,717 1,382 58 1,440 30 14 44 14 17 31 1,696 65 1,761 1,396 75 1,471 Note. SAD = Social anxiety disorder. AvPD = Avoidant personality disorder. Factor analysis of diagnostic criteria for SAD and AvPD The diagnostic criteria for SAD and AvPD were analyzed with exploratory factor analysis (EFA) with oblique geomin rotation and confirmatory factor analysis (CFA). We Longitudinal associations between SAD and AvPD S2 used the robust maximum likelihood estimator (MLR) in Mplus 7.3. The criteria were analyzed as ordered categories. The EFA yielded two factors with eigenvalues clearly above one, at 5.29 and 3.37, and one factor with an eigenvalue of 1.05. The scree plot is shown in Figure S1. This suggests the presence of two or three factors. Fit indices for the one-, two-, and three-factor solutions are provided in Table S3. The AIC improved from the model with one factor to the model with two factors, and further from two factors to three. The samplesize-adjusted BIC favored the two-factor model compared to one and three factors. The threefactor model lacks theoretical foundation. The statistical indices did not favor the one-factor model, and the two-factor model has good theoretical foundation and easily interpretable results. The three-factor solution provided similar results as the two-factor solution, except that AvPD symptom 1 seemed to fit best within a separate factor, which had no strong loadings on other items (all below .19). Thus, the EFA suggested a model in which the DSM criteria for SAD and AvPD loaded on distinct, but correlated factors, with low cross-loadings. Figure S1. Scree Plot for Exploratory Factor Analysis. Table S3 Fit Indices for Exploratory (EFA) and Confirmatory Factor Analysis (CFA) for the Diagnostic Criteria for SAD and AvPD Longitudinal associations between SAD and AvPD S3 AIC BICSSA 1. One factor 31629.96 31804.64 2. Two factors 31159.84 31372.37 3. Three factors 31130.00 31377.47 4. One factor 31603.68 31778.37 5. Two correlated factors 31193.78 31371.38 6. Bifactor with two group factors 31098.39 31313.84 7. Bifactor with two correlated group factors 31089.80 31308.16 EFA CFA Note: SAD = Social anxiety disorder. AvPD = Avoidant personality disorder. AIC = Akaike Information Criterion. BICSSA = Sample-Size Adjusted Bayesian Information Criterion. EFA = Exploratory Factor Analysis. CFA = Confirmatory Factor Analysis. Lower values of AIC and BICSSA indicate better model fit. Fit indices for the CFAs are shown in Table S3. The model fit was better for the model with two correlated factors, compared to a one-factor model, suggesting that SAD and AvPD did not reflect the same latent factors. The SAD and AvPD factors correlated .640 in the twofactor model. An alternative model is the bifactor model (Jennrich & Bentler, 2012; Reise, Moore, & Haviland, 2010; Reise, 2012). In this model, all items load on a general factor; in addition, all SAD items load on a SAD factor and all AvPD items on an AvPD factor. All three latent factors are orthogonal to each other. The bifactor model (model 6) had better fit than the twofactor model. In the bifactor model, the general factor explained 38.0% of the variance in Longitudinal associations between SAD and AvPD S4 SAD and AvPD items (53.0% of AvPD items and 23.0% of SAD items), whereas the group factors explained 19.1% of the variance (8.9% of AvPD items and 29.3% of SAD items). We also tested a bifactor model where the group factors were allowed to correlate (model 7). This model had the best fit. In this model, the general factor explained 23.6% of the variance (39.7% of AvPD items and 7.4% of SAD items), whereas the AvPD factor explained 33.9% of the variance (22.5% of AvPD items and 45.4% of SAD items). The two group factors correlated .61. It should be noted that bifactor models with correlated group factors have unclear interpretations and may suggest the presence of unmodeled general factors (Jennrich & Bentler, 2012; Reise et al., 2010). The implications of this model are therefore unclear. The results of the two bifactor models are shown in Table S4. The bifactor model may be interpreted in different ways. First, it is possible that the group factors of the bifactor model reflect methodological variance associated with the two different interviews. The interpretation that the group factors reflect method variance seems implausible in the present analyses, because the AvPD items do not uniformly load on this potential method factor. In addition, the general factor showed higher loadings for the AvPD items than for SAD items, which is inconsistent with the view that AvPD is primarily a more severe form of SAD. Second, the bifactor model may have a substantive interpretation where a general psychopathological factor influences the symptoms directly in addition to the disorder factors. Third, good fit does not necessarily imply that the bifactor model is true. In a simulation study, Morgan, Hodge, Wells, and Watkins (2015) found that model's fit indices were biased toward favoring bifactor models when data were generated from a true higher-order structure. Longitudinal associations between SAD and AvPD S5 Table S4 Factor Loadings From Bifactor Models 6 (Bifactor With Two Orthogonal Group Factors) and 7 (Bifactor With Two Correlated Group Factors). Standardized Model Results. Model 6 Model 7 Estimate p Estimate p Avoid 1 .79 < .001 .59 .011 Avoid 2 .85 < .001 .59 .032 Avoid 3 .67 < .001 .45 .031 Avoid 4 .80 < .001 .50 .073 Avoid 5 .67 < .001 .83 < .001 Avoid 6 .68 < .001 .78 < .001 Avoid 7 .61 < .001 .58 < .001 CD20S .62 < .001 .40 .032 CD22S .47 < .001 .24 .187 CD23 .45 < .001 .27 .075 CD25 .41 < .001 .20 .230 CD24D .28 < .001 .26 < .001 CD29 .52 < .001 .23 .347 CD25P .53 < .001 .25 .289 Avoid 1 .15 .060 .53 .041 Avoid 2 .12 .478 .64 .012 G by AvPD by Longitudinal associations between SAD and AvPD S6 Avoid 3 .05 .526 .49 .008 Avoid 4 .05 .738 .64 .004 Avoid 5 .53 < .001 .21 .541 Avoid 6 .48 < .001 .28 .405 Avoid 7 .27 .005 .33 .182 CD20S .55 < .001 .73 < .001 CD22S .49 < .001 .65 < .001 CD23 .38 < .001 .53 < .001 CD25 .52 < .001 .64 < .001 CD24D .25 < .001 .31 .001 CD29 .66 < .001 .81 < .001 CD25P .78 < .001 .89 < .001 SAD by Note. SAD = Social anxiety disorder. AvPD = Avoidant personality disorder. Avoid 1 ... 7 = diagnostic criteria for AvPD according to DSM-IV. CD20S … CD25P = section and number of questions in the Composite International Diagnostic Interview (CIDI). Two-tailed p-values. AvPD and SAD group factors correlate .61 in model 7. In order to determine whether the group factors merely reflect methodological variance, we tested whether they correlated with something substantive. We chose alcohol use disorder (AUD), which was available at Wave 1 and Wave 2 as a single ordinal variable (no AUD, subthreshold AUD, full AUD). We added this variable to the bifactor model without correlations (model 6). In this model, AUD does not load on the g factor, but AUD is allowed to correlate with the group factors for SAD and AvPD. If the group factors were merely method factors, the correlations should be close to zero. However, the model-estimated Longitudinal associations between SAD and AvPD S7 correlation between the latent SAD group factor and AUD was .55 (p < .001), and the correlation between the latent AVPD group factor and AUD was −.33 (p = .090). Thus, the group factors are most likely not merely reflecting methodological issues. In conclusion, the analysis suggested a model in which the DSM-IV criteria for SAD and AvPD loaded on distinct, but correlated factors, with low cross-loadings. The CFA supported the conclusion that SAD and AvPD do not reflect the same latent factors. The bifactor model had the best fit, but does not suggest that the differences between SAD and AvPD are merely due to methodological issues. Attrition analyses The prevalence of full SAD at Wave 1 was 3.6% (53 of 1465) among individuals who participated at Wave 2, and 4.0% (12 of 302) among dropouts. Neither full SAD (OR = 0.91, 95% CI [0.48, 1.72], p = .760) or subthreshold SAD (OR = 0.98, 95% CI [0.65, 1.47], p = .916) was statistically significantly related to continued participation. The prevalence of full AvPD at Wave 1 was 2.4% (35 of 1467) among individuals who participated at Wave 2, and 3.3% (10 of 303) among dropouts. AVPD diagnosis was not significantly related to continued participation (OR = 0.71, 95% CI [0.35, 1.46], p = .359). However, subthreshold symptoms of AvPD were associated with lower rates of participation at Wave 2 (OR = 0.92, 95% CI [0.84, 0.99], p = .033). Zygosity was also related to dropout. Whereas 85.8% (774 of 902) of MZs participated at Wave 2, the rate among DZs was 79.7% (697 or 875) (OR = 0.65, 95% CI [0.50, 0.83], p = .001). The polychoric correlation between SAD and AvPD traits was 0.51 (95% CI [0.36, 0.65]) among individuals who dropped out from the study and 0.49 (95% CI [0.42, 0.56]) among those who continued to participate in the study. Longitudinal associations between SAD and AvPD S8 References Jennrich, R. I., & Bentler, P. M. (2012). Exploratory bi-factor analysis: The oblique case. Psychometrika, 77, 442–454. doi:10.1007/s11336-012-9269-1 Morgan, G., Hodge, K., Wells, K., & Watkins, M. (2015). Are fit indices biased in favor of bi-factor models in cognitive ability research? A comparison of fit in correlated factors, higher-order, and bi-factor models via Monte Carlo simulations. Journal of Intelligence, 3, 2–20. doi:10.3390/jintelligence3010002 Reise, S. P. (2012). The rediscovery of bifactor measurement models. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 47, 667–696. doi:10.1080/00273171.2012.715555 Reise, S. P., Moore, T. M., & Haviland, M. G. (2010). Bifactor models and rotations: exploring the extent to which multidimensional data yield univocal scale scores. Journal of Personality Assessment, 92, 544–559. doi:10.1080/00223891.2010.496477