Updating Allport’s and Batson’s Framework of Religious Orientations: A Refreshing Look from the Perspective of Self-Determination Theory and Wulff’s Model of Approaches towards Religion Bart NEYRINCK Willy LENS University of Leuven, Belgium Maarten VANSTEENKISTE Bart SOENENS Ghent University, Belgium Correspondence concerning this article can be addressed to Bart Neyrinck, Department of Psychology, Tiensestraat 102, 3000 Leuven, Belgium. E-mail address: Bart.Neyrinck@psy.kuleuven.be Religious Orientations 2 Abstract The aim of the present contribution was to theoretically and empirically evaluate and, if needed, reconsider Allport’s intrinsic- extrinsic and Batson’s quest religious orientations through the lens of self-determination theory and Wulff’s social-cognitive model. In line with our theoretical analysis, this study showed that Allport’s intrinsic-extrinsic dichotomy fails to correspond empirically to the differentiation between intrinsic and extrinsic motivation within self-determination theory (SDT). Whereas Allport’s intrinsic religious orientation was strongly positively related to one type of extrinsic motivation, that is, internalized (i.e., autonomous) extrinsic motivation, Allport’s two extrinsic (i.e., personal and social) religious orientations were not systematically related to any of the discerned motives within SDT. Furthermore, Batson’s quest orientation was unrelated to any of the SDT concepts but was positively related to symbolic unbelief. The present findings suggest that Allport’s framework might need both refinement and relabeling of its motivational orientations such that research adopting Allport’s framework fits with recent theoretical evolutions in the field of motivation psychology. KEY WORDS: Religious orientation, Allport, Batson, self-determination theory, internalization, religious behavior, Post-Critical Belief Scale, literal symbolic. RUNNING HEAD: Religious Orientations Religious Orientations 3 Updating Allport’s and Batson’s Framework of Religious Orientations: A Refreshing Look from the Perspective of Self-Determination Theory and Wulff’s Model of Approaches towards Religion Since the pioneering work of Allport (1950; Allport & Ross, 1967), dozens of articles have relied on the distinction between intrinsic and extrinsic religious orientation (e.g., Batson, Schoenrade, & Ventis, 1993; Donahue, 1985). This differentiation grew out of Allport’s (1950) seminal conceptualization of mature and immature forms of religious sentiment and intends to measure intrinsic versus extrinsic motivation for religion (Gorsuch, 1997). Later on, Batson (1976; Batson, Schoenrade, & Ventis, 1993) introduced the quest orientation to provide a fuller picture of the original concept of mature religiosity. More than four decades after the initial introduction of Allport’s orientations, we believe it is time to re-evaluate its theoretical relevance in light of broader motivational frameworks in which a distinction is made between intrinsic and extrinsic motivation in general (Deci, 1975; Deci & Ryan, 2000; Kirkpatrick & Hood, 1990). The overall aim of the present contribution is to evaluate and, if needed, to reconsider Allport’s distinction through the lens of self-determination theory (SDT; Deci & Ryan, 2000; Ryan & Deci, 2006; Vansteenkiste, Ryan, & Deci, 2008). SDT is a well grounded and empirically validated general theory of motivation and personality, which has been under development since Allport’s seminal work (e.g., Deci, 1971). In mapping the distinction between intrinsic and extrinsic religious orientation to the motivational concepts forwarded in SDT, this study aimed to heed the call to provide more conceptual depth in research on the motivational dynamics involved in religiosity. Kirkpatrick and Hood (1990, p. 448), for instance, stated that “a serious approach to researching the topic of motivation for religious involvement … should begin with a rich psychological theory of human motivation”. In this regard, one of the important issues Kirkpatrick and Hood (1990) raised was the conceptual meaning of Allport’s scales: do they deal with motivation, personality or cognitive style? To examine associations between Allport’s and Batson’s concepts and dimensions of religious cognitive style, Wulff’s (1997) and Hutsebaut and colleagues’ (e.g., Duriez & Hutsebaut, in press; Fontaine, Duriez, Luyten, & Hutsebaut, 2003) model of social-cognitive approaches towards religious contents. After introducing Allport’s and Batson’s perspectives and briefly discussing their critics, we present the basic tenets of SDT and Wulff’s model. The subsequent attempt to contrast and integrate these frameworks will then allow us to derive a number of specific hypotheses with respect to the relation between the introduced concepts. Religious Orientations 4 Intrinsic, Extrinsic and Quest Religious Orientation Allport’s (e.g., Allport, 1966; Allport & Ross, 1967) distinction between intrinsic (IR) and extrinsic religious orientation (ER) originated from Allport’s (1950) conceptualization of mature and immature religious sentiments. Although some alternative conceptualizations such as personality or cognitive style have been proposed, his writings show a clear evolution towards a motivational conceptualization of these religious orientations (Gorsuch, 1997; Hunt & King, 1971; Kirkpatrick & Hood, 1990). An intrinsically oriented person considers religion as an ultimate end in itself; it is a master motive in life. Religious beliefs and values (e.g., humility, compassion, etc.) are internalized “without reservation”, and other needs and goals are accommodated, reorganized and brought in harmony with these religious contents. Importantly, an intrinsic religious orientation “floods the whole life with motivation and meaning” (Allport, 1966, p. 455). In contrast, an extrinsically oriented individual approaches religion in a utilitarian or instrumental fashion: it helps one to attain “self-centered” ends, such as safety, solace, or sociability. Furthermore, with an extrinsic orientation, religion is lightly held, oversimplified, not reflected upon, and “not well integrated in the deeper life of the subject” (Allport, 1950, p. 59). Importantly, Kirkpatrick (1989) differentiated ER into extrinsic-personal (Ep) and extrinsic-social (Es), as two different categories of non-religious goals which are strived for through religiosity. Whereas an extrinsic-personal religious orientation (Ep) points to the use of religion to gain comfort, security or protection, an extrinsic-social religious orientation (Es) marks the use of religion to gain social contact. Although IR and ER were intended to represent the original concept of mature religion, Batson (1976, Batson et al., 1993) argued that IR and ER missed several components of that original concept. More specifically, Batson missed the mature elements of a well differentiated, critical and open-ended cognitive reflection in dealing with religious issues, hence, “an organization that has emerged from repeated creative changes in response to existential conflicts” (Batson, et al., 1993, p. 160). Maturity in religious issues is characterized by flexible cognitive structures that are responsive to new information. In an attempt to operationalize these characteristics, Batson (1976; Batson et al., 1993) proposed Quest as a third religious orientation. A quest orientation is expressed in (1) posing complex existential questions without reducing their complexity, (2) regarding doubt as important and positive, and (3) emphasizing tentativeness and incompleteness in formulating answers for religious questions. Questions have been raised about the validity of the quest-scale: given that it generally does not correlate to measures of religiosity, Donahue (1985) suggested that quest measures agnositicism rather than religious belief. In his opinion, it can only be Religious Orientations 5 called religious if religion is defined as “having existential concerns” without necessarily adhering to religious values or believing in a transcendental God. In contrast to this criticism, Batson et al. (1993) argue that quest does measure a religious orientation, given that religiously active people (e.g., seminarians), relative to less religiously involved people (e.g., undergraduates with a moderate interest in religion) score higher on the quest-scale In addition to Donahue’s criticism that IR does not fully capture mature religiosity, one other important criticism pertains to the conceptual clarity of the religious orientation construct: is it a motivational concept, a personality feature or a cognitive construct (Kirkpatrick & Hood, 1990)? In the present contribution, we will evaluate the IR-ER distinction against a well-validated motivational framework, that is, SDT, and against the social-cognitive approach towards religious contents, as articulated in Wulff’s model. Self-Determination Theory In SDT (Deci & Ryan, 2000), intrinsically motivated behavior is seen as the hallmark of autonomous functioning, that is, functioning with a sense of psychological freedom, volition and perceived choice. Intrinsic motivation refers to the enactment of an activity in absence of any external incentives. Intrinsically motivated behavior is autotelic because it is performed for no other reason than the feelings of satisfaction, fun, and pleasure that are associated with the enactment of the behavior itself. Intrinsic motivation is differentiated from extrinsic motivation which pertains to carrying out an activity to attain an outcome that is separate from the activity itself (Lens, 1997; Ryan & Deci, 2000). Interestingly, SDT does not treat extrinsic motivation in a homogeneous way, instead distinguishing four types of extrinsic motivation as a function of the degree to which the reason for performing the activity has been internalized (Ryan & Deci, 2002). Internalization is defined as an active process through which beliefs, values, attitudes or behavioral regulations are gradually transformed into self-endorsed attributes, values or regulations such that they become part of one’s sense of self (Grolnick, Deci, & Ryan, 1997). As the reason for performing a certain behavior becomes more internalized, the extrinsically motivated behavior will be experienced as relatively more autonomous. The following four types of extrinsically motivated religious behaviors can be differentiated. First, a behavior is externally regulated when it is induced by others; the behavior is forced upon the person by the promise of an incentive or the threat of a punishment. As the behavior is controlled by social forces alien to the self, the reason for enacting the behavior is not internalized at all; hence, the behavior will be emitted Religious Orientations 6 with a sense of pressure. An adolescent who attends church only to meet parental obligations is said to be externally regulated. Second, in case the external reasons for performing the activity are partially internalized, the behavioral regulation is said to be introjected. Because the behavior is not wholeheartedly endorsed but only partially taken in, a conflict will likely arise between those introjects and other personal inclinations. Such an internal conflict will engender negative feelings such as anxiety, guilt and shame, which one will try to suppress by performing the behavior. Going to church to avoid feelings of guilt for not doing so is an example of introjected regulation. The behavioral regulation will be more internalized when one comes to identify with the reason for enacting the behavior. In the case of identified regulation, the behavior is perceived as personally relevant and, hence, will be enacted with a sense of autonomy and genuine personal commitment. Finally, through critical self-reflection an identified reason or value can become fully integrated, that is, flexibly brought in congruence with other personally relevant self-structures (Deci & Ryan, 1985; Hodgins & Knee, 2002). Integrated regulation is conceptualized as the most autonomous form of extrinsic motivation as the enactment of the activity is fully endorsed and will emanate from one’s sense of self. Social work instigated by a strong identification with Christian core-values such as “loving thy neighbor” represents an example of identified regulation. This value can be brought in line with other values and regulations such as the value one attaches to helping behavior, thus representing an integrated regulation. In sum, controlled motivation reflects a pressured engagement in the activity, with the pressure either originating from external forces (i.e., external regulation) or residing in the person (i.e., introjected regulation). Both internalized forms of extrinsic motivation (i.e., identified and integrated regulations) and intrinsic motivation are in SDT referred to as autonomous motivation as one willingly engages in the activity in both cases. However, it is important to note that intrinsic motivation and internalized extrinsic motivation differ in some respects. A person’s inherent interest in the task at hand forms the starting point for intrinsic motivation to develop. Because people feel naturally attracted to perform an activity which is intrinsically enjoyable or interesting, they will experience a sense of spontaneity and psychological freedom when following their interests. While intrinsically motivated behavior lacks instrumentality in its characteristics, identified and integrated regulation are both instrumental in obtaining some outcome and, as such, are both forms of extrinsic motivation. The conceptual distinction between controlled and autonomous motivation and between qualitatively different types of extrinsic motivation has been examined in several life domains, including the domain of religion (see Neyrinck, Lens, & Vansteenkiste, 2005 for an overview). Research has shown that a more Religious Orientations 7 internalized behavioral regulation of religious behaviors is positively related to well-being, a more frequent engagement in religious behaviors (Ryan, Rigby, & King, 1993), and a more flexible approach towards religious contents (Assor, Cohen-Malayev, Kaplan, & Friedman, 2005; Neyrinck, Vansteenkiste, Lens, Duriez, & Hutsebaut, 2006). Social-cognitive Approaches towards Religion In addition to considering the motivation underlying religious behaviors, the question can be raised how religious contents are cognitively approached. According to Wulff (1997), all possible approaches to religion can be located in a two-dimensional space, organized around two orthogonal bipolar dimensions. The vertical axis “exclusion versus inclusion of transcendence” refers to the degree to which a transcendental reality is thought to exist, and as such, pertains to whether people believe or not. The horizontal axis “literal versus symbolic” indicates the way in which religious contents are interpreted and processed, that is, in a flexible, symbolic or in a more rigid, literal way. The Post-Critical Belief Scale (PCBS, e.g., Fontaine, et al., 2003) was developed to measure these two dimensions with respect to Christian belief contents. In the PCBS, exclusion versus inclusion of transcendence refers to the degree to which one does (not) adhere to Christian thought, that is, the degree to which one believes in or rejects a transcendental realm as conceived in this religion. The literal versus symbolic dimension refers to a rigid versus more flexible interpretation of these religious contents. Crossing both orthogonal dimensions (i.e., exclusion versus inclusion and literal versus symbolic) results in four different approaches towards religion. Symbolic inclusion entails an open-minded processing of religious contents through critical reflection and a flexible integration of these elements with other internal cognitive structures (Ricoeur, 1970). In the case of symbolic exclusion, religious contents are not personally adhered, although they are not straightforwardly rejected. Instead, religious beliefs are viewed as one meaning system among many other possible meaning systems, although people scoring high symbolic exclusion tend to rely on other meaning systems than religiosity to find purpose in life. In contrast to these symbolic approaches, literal approaches entail a more rigid, one-sided view of Christian religious contents. Literal inclusion of belief contents means that only ideas, norms and values that are in accordance to Holy Scriptures are acknowledged and that perspectives of disbelief are defensively rejected. Literal exclusion entails a similar defensive functioning. However, rather than rigidly sticking to the Religious Orientations 8 Catholic belief system, this system is now defensively rejected; it is blocked as a possible meaningproviding framework. Drawing Connections between Allport, SDT and Wulff Against the background of SDT and Wulff’s framework, we argue that Allport’s IR and ER are essentially motivational in nature, while Batson’s quest rather represents a social-cognitive approach towards religion. Regarding Allport’s IR, several, if not all, descriptions of this construct indicate that it yields considerable overlap with an autonomous regulation of religiosity, as conceived within SDT. To illustrate, Gorsuch (1997, p. 13), reframes IR in terms of intrinsic motivation: “the motivation for experiencing and living one’s religious faith for the sake of the faith itself. The person’s religion is “an end unto itself, a goal pursued in the absence of external reinforcement”. This quote indicates that IR represents intrinsic motivation, as defined in SDT. However, this description of IR pertains to only one part of what Allport originally described. Allport (1950) defines a mature religious person as having a well-developed and differentiated view on religion. Moreover, needs and motives are brought into harmony with religious beliefs and prescriptions, which are organised and integrated in the deeper life of the person (Allport & Ross, 1967). As such, processes of internalization and accommodation also seem to be characteristic of IR, indicating that Allport’s IR shares considerable overlap with SDT’s internalized and, hence, autonomous forms of extrinsically motivated behavior. In sum, based on these descriptions of Allport’s IR, it can be predicted that IR will be closely related to both intrinsic motivation and the autonomous forms of extrinsic motivation for religious activities, as conceived within SDT. Allport’s description of ER clearly indicates that ER represents a form of extrinsic motivation as defined within SDT. Religious behavior is not performed as its own end, but to achieve outcomes separable from the activity itself (i.e., security and affiliation). Although it is clear that Allport’s ER reflects extrinsic motivation, it is less clear whether it represents a relatively more autonomous or a relatively more controlled type of extrinsic motivation. On the one hand, it has been argued that an extrinsic religious orientation is lightly held and “not well integrated in the deeper life of the subject” (Allport, 1950, p. 59), suggesting that ER represents a non-internalized and, hence, non-autonomous form of extrinsic motivation. On the other hand, Allport does not mention pressure as a critical component or descriptor of ER. Allport even stated that people with this orientation “feel no obligation to attend church regularly” (Allport, 1966, p. 455, italics Religious Orientations 9 added). Hence, ER seems to be related to neither autonomous nor controlled regulations of religious behavior. Finally, given that Batson’s concept of quest orientation is primarily social-cognitive rather than motivational in nature, quest can be clearly linked to Wulff’s model of social-cognitive approaches towards religion. More specifically, as Batson’s quest-orientation represents an open exploration of the many different meanings that can be found in religious contents, it seems to involve a flexible and symbolic socialcognitive approach as defined in Wulff’s model. Present Research The overarching aim of this study is to examine how Allport’s IR and ER orientations and Batson’s quest dimension map onto two well-established frameworks for conceptualizing and measuring motives for religiosity (SDT) and cognitive processing of religious beliefs (Wulff’s model). Two sets of conflicting hypotheses regarding the relationships between Allport’s orientations and SDT’s regulations can be forwarded. First, a superficial analysis of Allport’s intrinsic-extrinsic religious orientation distinction suggests that this pair of concepts can be directly equated with the intrinsic-extrinsic motivation distinction within SDT. Hence, Allport’s IR should correlate positively with SDT’s intrinsic motivation, while both Ep and Es should correlate with both SDT’s autonomous and controlled extrinsic motivation. However, a more thorough conceptual analysis of Allport and SDT indicates that a different pattern of correlations might emerge. First, given Gorsuch’s definition of IR in terms of intrinsic motivation, IR should correlate positively with SDT’s intrinsic motivation. Moreover, given that elements such as internalization and organization characterize Allport’s IR, we predict that IR will correlate positively with SDT’s identified and integrated regulation. We do not predict a clear pattern of relations of Extrinsic-personal and Extrinsicsocial with autonomous and controlled regulations. Finally, given that Batson’s quest orientation is not motivational in nature, we do not expect the quest-scale to relate to SDT’s regulation items. In contrast, because the quest orientation primarily entails cognitive flexibility in dealing with religious issues, we expect the quest orientation to correlate positively with a symbolic approach of religious contents. Furthermore, we will explore whether quest does or does not measure religiosity, that is, whether it will correlate positively or negatively with inclusion of transcendence. Furthermore, some specific hypotheses regarding the relations between the religious orientations and social-cognitive approaches towards religion can be forwarded. On the one hand, given that Allport’s IR includes a strong commitment to Religious Orientations 10 religious beliefs (e.g., Donahue, 1985), we expect a positive correlation between IR and inclusion of transcendence. We expect the correlation between both Ep and Es scales and inclusion to be positive also, because Ep and Es both represent religious orientations. However, this relation will be less strong than the former correlation between IR and inclusion. Furthermore, we do not have clear expectations regarding the correlations between Allport’s constructs and literal versus symbolic components. The proposed hypotheses will be tested cross-sectionally in a broad sample of religiously active participants. Method Participants A total of 144 participants of a two-day theological conference in Leuven, Belgium completed our questionnaire. Sixty-seven participants (47%) were male. The average age of the participants was 53 years (SD = 15), ranging from 21 to 83 years. Twenty-three percent of the participants prayed very often, 39% often, 26% from time to time, 11% seldom and 1% never. Thirty-three percent attended church very often, 41% often, 19% from time to time, and 7% seldom. Measures All measures were presented in Dutch, the participant’s mother tongue. All scales were 5-point Likert scales ranging from 1 (completely disagree) to 5 (completely agree), except the PCBS, which was rated on a 7-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (completely disagree) to 7 (completely agree). Age-Universal I-E Scale-12. Allport’s religious orientations were measured with the twelve item Age Universal I-E Scale (Maltby, 1999). Both the eigenvalue > 1 criterium and the scree-plot of a principal component analysis (PCA) pointed to a three-component solution (eigenvalues 3.34, 2.30, and 1.32), explaining 58% of the variance. Interestingly, after an oblique Promax rotation, only three out of the six IRitems loaded on one component. The other three IR-items loaded on one component together with the three Ep-items. The three Es-items clearly constituted the third component. However, we decided to compute our scales consistent with the literature. Accordingly, three scales were computed: IR (six items, e.g., “My whole approach to life is based on my religion”, M = 4.30, SD = 0.54; Cronbach’s alpha = .70), Ep (three items, e.g., “What religion offers me most is comfort in times of trouble and sorrow”, M = 2.84, SD = 0.83, Cronbach’s alpha = . .67) and Es (three items, e.g., “I go to church mostly to spend time with friends”, M = 2.05, SD = 0.88, Cronbach’s alpha = .85). The IR- and Ep-scale were positively correlated: r(144)=.25, p < Religious Orientations 11 .01; also the Ep- and Es-scale correlated positively: r(144)=.28, p < .01. IR and Es did not correlate significantly. Batson’s quest orientation. Participants completed the twelve item quest-scale of Batson, Schoenrade, and Ventis (1993). The scale was translated and back translated by three psychologists who are well familiar with the literature on religiosity and motivation. The scale contains three subcomponents, each measured with four items: openness to change (e.g., “As I grow and change, I expect my religion also to grow and change”), perception of religious doubt as positive (“For me, doubting is an important part of what it means to be religious”), and readiness to face existential questions without reducing their complexity (e.g., “God wasn’t very important to me until I began to ask questions about the meaning of my own life”). However, the scree-plot pointed to a one-component solution (eigenvalue = 3.35), explaining 28% of the variance. Accordingly, the twelve-item quest-scale was computed (M = 3.32, SD = 0.52, Cronbach’s alpha = .72). Batson’s quest did not correlate with any of Allport’s religious orientations. Religious Motivation Scale. Participants were asked to report their most important religious activity, that is, the activity that is most helpful in expressing their religious belief attitude (see Neyrinck, et al., 2006, for more details). Examples of listed activities included “reading and discussing religious literature”, “going to church”; “teaching religion”, and “living life with full attention”. After generating this activity, intrinsic (e.g., “Because I simply enjoy it”), internalized (a combination of integrated and identified regulation, e.g., “Because I fully endorse it”) and controlled (i.e., a combination of introjected and external regulation, e.g., “Because I would feel guilty if I don’t”) regulation were measured with three items each. Both the scree plot and the eigenvalue > 1 criterion of the PCA on these nine items indicated a three-component solution (eigenvalues 2.50, 1.88, and 1.48), explaining 65% of the variance. The promax rotated component pattern was clearly interpretable as intrinsic, internalized, and controlled regulation. Accordingly, three scales were computed: intrinsic motivation (M = 3.80, SD = 0.90, Cronbach’s alpha = .75), internalized regulation (M = 4.36, SD = 0.54, Cronbach’s alpha = .73) and controlled regulation (M = 1.73, SD = 0.77, Cronbach’s alpha = .64). Intrinsic motivation and internalized regulation correlated positively [r(144) = .23, p < .01], while controlled regulation did not correlate to any other regulation. Post-Critical Belief scale (PCBS). Participants completed the shortened (18-item) Post-Critical Belief Scale (Dutch version, Duriez, Soenens, & Hutsebaut, 2005) measuring four approaches towards religion: Literal inclusion (five items; e.g., “I think that Bible stories should be taken literally, as they are written”); symbolic inclusion (four items; e.g., “Despite the high number of injustices Christianity has caused people, the original message of Christ is still valuable to me); literal exclusion (five items; e.g., “Faith is an Religious Orientations 12 expression of a weak personality”); and symbolic exclusion (four items; e.g., “I am well aware my ideology is only one possibility among so many others”). To control for individual differences in acquiescence, the individual average score over all items was subtracted from the raw item scores (for a detailed description of this procedure, see Fontaine, et al., 2003). A PCA was then carried out on the corrected scores. A scree test pointed to a two-component solution (eigenvalues 3.82 and 2.38), explaining 36 percent of the variance. After orthogonal Procrustes rotation towards the estimated average structure computed across 16 samples (Fontaine et al., 2003), these two components could be interpreted in terms of (exclusion versus) inclusion of transcendence and a (literal versus) symbolic approach of religious contents. Tucker’s Phi indices were .96 for Inclusion and .94 for Symbolic, suggesting good congruence between the sample specific and the average configuration (Bentler & Bonett, 1980; Van de Vijver & Leung, 1997). The higher the score on inclusion, the more the Christian message is adhered to. The higher the score on symbolic, the more religious contents are processed in a flexible, symbolic fashion. Scores on both components were standardized, hence M = 0 and SD = 1. Results Preliminary Analysis Age and Gender. Preliminary analyses were conducted to examine relations of the study variables with gender and age. A significant multivariate effect of gender was found, Wilk’s Lambda = 0.83, F(9,134) = 3.00, p < .01, partial η² = .17. Females (M = 3.00, SD = 0.80) obtained higher scores on Allport’s Ep in comparison to males, M = 2.65, SD = 0.82, F(1,142) = 6.82, p < .05, η² = .05. The same gender differences were found for Batson’s quest, females: M = 3.42, SD = 0.50; males: M = 3.20, SD = 0.52, F(1,142) = 9.05, p < .01, η² = .05; and SDT’s intrinsic motivation, females: M = 3.95, SD = 0.85, males: M = 3.62, SD = 0.93, F(1,142) = 4.84, p < .05, η² =.03. Furthermore, age correlated significantly with Allport’s IR, r(144) = .32, p < .001; with Batson’s quest , r(144) = -.20, p < .05, with SDT’s intrinsic motivation, r(144) = -.29, p < .001; and symbolic approach, , r(144) = -.25, p < .01. Given these age and gender relations, we will control for these background variables in the regression analyses. Primary Analysis Correlations. Correlations between the study variables are presented in Table 1. Allport’s IR correlated significantly positively with internalized regulation and inclusion. Ep showed a positive relation with intrinsic motivation, internalized regulation, and inclusion. It correlated negatively with symbolic Religious Orientations 13 approach. Es correlated positively with both intrinsic motivation and controlled regulation. Batson’s quest scale was uncorrelated with any motivational regulation. However, it correlated negatively with inclusion and positively with symbolic. Regression Analyses. To examine the empirical relation between the three SDT regulations (intrinsic, internalized, and controlled) and the two PCBS dimensions (inclusion and symbolic) and Allport and Batson’s constructs, a series of hierarchical regression analyses was performed. In a first step, Allport and Batson’s religious orientations were simultaneously regressed on the background variables age and gender and the three SDT-based regulations. In a second step, the two PCBS dimensions were added as predictors (see Table 2). IR was positively predicted by internalized regulation and both PCBS dimensions inclusion and symbolic. Further, none of the associations with Allport’s Ep stayed significant controlling for gender and age. The only significant (positive) predictors of Es were intrinsic and controlled regulation. Consistent with the pattern of correlations obtained, Batson’s quest was negatively predicted by inclusion and positively by symbolic. General Discussion The aim of this article was to relate the concepts of intrinsic, extrinsic and quest orientations that have been extensively examined within the psychology of religion to (a) the behavioral regulations discerned within self-determination theory and (b) Wulff’s model of cognitive approaches towards religion. By empirically linking these different frameworks, we aimed to address the more general question whether Allport’s and Batson’s concepts represent a motivational regulation, a cognitive style or a mix of both. Intrinsic Religious Orientation One of the interesting findings of the present research concerns the quite strong positive relation between Allport’s concept of IR and internalized regulation as conceptualized within SDT. Within SDT, internalized regulation represents an autonomous form of extrinsic motivation, because the reason and value for enacting the behavior is personally endorsed, such that one engages in the activity willingly or volitionally. The positive association between both constructs is in line with Allport’s own descriptions of IR as the internalization of religious beliefs and values, through which religion becomes the central motive in one’s life. Although both scales were quite strongly correlated (i.e., .45), both are characterized by a substantial amount of unique variance, suggesting that both concepts are similar, yet distinct. Whereas IR and SDT’s internalized regulation are similar in the sense that psychological material is internalized into Religious Orientations 14 one’s self-structures, they are likely to differ from one another with respect to the content that gets internalized. Whereas in IR religious contents are personally subscribed to and fully endorsed, in the case of internalized regulation, the regulation or value for performing a religious behavior is fully accepted and internalized. Further attesting to the interpretation of IR as valuing religious contents, a unique positive relation between IR and inclusion of transcendence was found. As the latter measures the degree of adherence to Christian religious contents, Allport’s IR also measures the strength of religious belief, next to motivation for religiosity or religious behaviors. In this vein, we align ourselves with Kirkpatrick and Hood’s (1990) and Donahue’s (1985) argument of IR representing a commitment to religiosity. Remark that the correlation of .45 could also be influenced by our specific way of measuring. Measuring the SDT-based regulations, we asked participants openly for their most important religious activity. If we look at Allport’s IR items, then we recognize themes such as “to live my life according to my religious beliefs”, “to base my approach to life on my religion”, “searching for answers about the meaning of life” and “to spend time in private thought and prayer”. If we would have probed for the SDT-based motivations of these “activities”, the correlation between IR and identified regulation could have been stronger. Different from the strong positive correlation between Allport’s concept of IR and internalized regulation, it is quite puzzling that Allport’s IR was unrelated to SDT’s concept of intrinsic motivation. Within SDT, intrinsic motivation refers to the enactment of the religious behavior for the mere satisfaction or enjoyment it provides and is considered, in addition to internalized motivation, as a highly autonomous form of motivation. Several elements can be put forward to better grasp the null relation between IR and intrinsic motivation. First, at the conceptual level, it should be noted that although Gorsuch (1997) redefines IR in terms of intrinsic motivation, Allport himself did not use elements such as fun, pleasure or interest to describe IR. Given the lack of such descriptions in Allport’s writings, it seems logical that IR is less strongly related to intrinsic motivation compared to internalized motivation. Second, with respect to the operationalization of IR, only one single item in Allport’s IR scale (i.e., ‘I enjoy reading about my religion’) pertains to the enjoyment of a religious activity, whereas all other items refer to the personal endorsement of one’s religious beliefs. Given the imbalance in the IR items tapping into pleasure and enjoyment vs. personal valuation of religion, it is logical that IR is more strongly related to internalized than to intrinsic motivation, as conceived within SDT. Third, the lack of similarity between both constructs could also be due to the different measures used. To measure SDT’s intrinsic motivation, we presented reasons such as fun, enjoyment or interest of their personally important religious activity. The scale measuring Allport’s IR contains only one item pertaining to Religious Orientations 15 the enjoyment of a religious activity: “I enjoy reading about my religion”. Hence, if we would have measured different reasons for a predefined activity such as “reading about religious themes”, we could have obtained a stronger correlation between intrinsic motivation and IR. In short, because of all of these reasons, it seems premature to draw any conclusions from the (lack of) relation between IR and intrinsic motivation. This issue deserves further investigation. What can be concluded, however, is that the term intrinsic in Allport’s IR refers to the fully acceptance and endorsement of one’s religious contents, which would be labelled a as internalized, an autonomous form of extrinsic motivation within SDT. A further question is whether Allport’s IR orientation also represents a particular cognitive style of dealing with religious contents. The current research points out that IR was positively related to a symbolic approach towards religious contents, suggesting that IR also entails a more flexible approach of religious issues. Overall, the current findings thus point out that of Allport’s IR might represent a mix of (a) a religious belief orientation (as it correlates with inclusion), (b) an internalized regulation for religion or religious behaviors, and (c) a flexible, symbolic interpretation of belief contents. However, our correlational findings do not exclude the possibility that Allport’s IR construct primarily taps into a motivational orientation that goes along with a stronger adherence to religious contents and a more flexible approach towards religious contents rather than representing a cognitive style per se (see Neyrinck, et al., 2006 for a similar argument). Finally, it is interesting to note that the positive relation between Allport’s IR and symbolic approach contradicts Batson et al.’s (1993) suggestion that an intrinsic believer is compulsive, conservative and uncritical. Of course, a replication in a sample of more literal believers would be even more strong evidence. A high score of IR in a sample of more literal believers would entail the internalization of more conservative religious contents. Possibly, in such a sample, that would result in a positive correlation between IR and literal approach of religious contents. Extrinsic Religious Orientation The significant correlations between Ep and Es and the motivational regulations within SDT were substantially lower than the relation between IR and internalized regulation. These findings point to an empirical divergence between Allport’s ER and SDT’s regulations, which we had theoretically predicted. Religion is lightly held in an extrinsic religious orientation, pointing that a regulation for religious behaviors will not be internalized. However, an extrinsic religious person does not experience any obligation to perform religious activities, suggesting no correlation with an external or internal psychological pressure entailing a controlled regulation. A similar point was made by Neyrinck, et al. (2005), who argued that both Religious Orientations 16 Allport’s Ep and Es cannot clearly be placed in the SDT framework of regulations of behavior. Both extrinsic religious orientations refer to certain goal contents pursued through religion. One tries to find a certain form of security or comfort in religion, or religion serves the goal of social affiliation. In SDT, next to the regulations of behaviors, behaviors serve the attainment of different goals. As such, intrinsic goals (e.g., self-development, affiliation or community contribution) are theoretically contrasted with extrinsic goals (e.g., financial success, fame and physical attractiveness; Kasser & Ryan, 1993, 1996; Vansteenkiste, Lens, & Deci, 2006). Importantly, the regulations of and the goal contents (pursued through behavior) are conceptualized to be orthogonal (Sheldon, Ryan, Deci, & Kasser, 2004). Going to church to attain social affiliation can be regulated autonomously or controlled, just like one can feel free or obligated to contribute to the community by donating money. Attesting to this relative orthogonality, Sheldon, Ryan, Deci, and Kasser (2004) have shown a relatively low correlation between relative autonomous functioning and intrinsic versus extrinsic goal pursuit. Moreover, both relative autonomy and goal pursuit showed an independent effect on well-being. Following Neyrinck, et al. (2005), Allport’s IR can be conceptualized as autonomous behavioral regulations, while both Ep and Es can be framed as goal-pursuits through behavior. In our opinion, this answers the “thorny issue” of bipolarity or orthogonality of IR and ER. From an SDT-perspective, both are clearly orthogonal, which is fully in line with several empirical studies (e.g., Kirkpatrick, 1989). Furthermore, regarding the question whether Allport’s religious orientations would measure cognitive styles, we only found a small and unreliable negative correlation between Ep and symbolic approach. Hence, both ER scales are not clearly related to cognitive styles in approaching religious contents. Batson’s Quest Orientation Regarding Batson’s quest orientation, our findings clearly point into the interpretation of this as a measure of cognitive style and not motivation. Surprisingly, Batson’s quest scale showed to consist of two components: an openness to change and doubt component, next to posing complex existential questions. Interestingly, both components showed to be quite different, attesting their zero-correlation and different correlations to external variables. As expected, the quest scales did not relate to motivational regulations, attesting them to be not motivational in nature. As expected, the openness/doubt scale correlated significantly positive with a symbolic approach of religious contents. Interestingly, given our measure of belief-unbelief (inclusion versus exclusion of transcendence), this openness/doubt scale showed to be correlated with symbolic unbelief. This is in line with Donahue’s (1985) concerns about quest measuring unbelief, and can be seen as evidence against Batson’s claim of quest indeed measuring a religious Religious Orientations 17 orientation. Again, one sees the advantage of including the PCBS measure to disentangle belief and unbelief. Of course, this first finding relating quest and unbelief awaits empirical replication. Conclusion It was our intention to underline Kirkpatrick and Hood’s (1990, p. 448) point that “a serious approach to researching the topic of motivation for religious involvement … should begin with a rich psychological theory of human motivation”. Psychologists of religion need to look further than their own backyard, reducing the distance with mainstream theoretical and empirical psychology. Kirkpatrick and Hood (1990) go even further suggesting to drop religious orientation items, replacing them with new items based on theoretical frameworks. In our modest opinion, the qualitatively more differentiated SDT-based regulations of religious behaviors can be a candidate, just like Wulff’s model that qualitatively differentiates styles of (un)belief. As we have shown, these frameworks can be meaningfully compared with Allport’s model. Further research needs to show their possible independent effects on external variables, or the possibility to reduce some of the effects to one or more of the measures, giving a more parsimonious solution. Religious orientations 18 References Allport, G. W. (1950). The individual and his religion. NY: Macmillan. Allport, G. W. (1966). The religious context of prejudice. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 5, 447457. Allport, G. W., & Ross, J. M. (1967). Personal religious orientation and prejudice. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 5, 432-443. Assor, A., Cohen-Malayev, M., Kaplan, A., & Friedman, D. (2005). Choosing to stay religious in a modern world: Socialization and exploration processes leading to an integrated internalization of religion among israeli jewish youth. In M.L. Maehr & S. Karabenick (Eds.). Advances in motivation and achievement, Vol. 14. Motivation and Religion (pp. 105-150). Greenwich, Conn.: Jai Press Inc. Batson, C. D. (1976). Religion as prosocial: Agent or double agent? Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 15, 29-45. Batson, C. D., Schoenrade, P., & Ventis, W. L. (1993). Religion and the individual: A social-psychological perspective. New York: Oxford University Press. Bentler, P. M., & Bonett, D. G. (1980). Significance tests and goodness-of-fit in the analyses of covariance structures. Psychological Bulletin, 88, 588-606. Deci, E. L. (1971). Effects of externally mediated rewards on intrinsic motivation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 18, 105-115. Deci, E. L. (1975). Intrinsic motivation. New York: Plenum Publishing Co. Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1985). Intrinsic motivation and self-determination in human behavior. New York: Plenum Publishing Co. Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2000). The "what" and "why" of goal pursuits: Human needs and the selfdetermination of behavior. Psychological Inquiry, 11, 227-268. Donahue, M. J. (1985). Intrinsic and extrinsic religiousness: A review and meta-analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48, 400-419. Duriez, B., & Hutsebaut, D. (in press). A slow and easy introduction to the Post-Critical Belief Scale. Internal structure and external relationships. In D. M. Wulff (Ed.), Handbook of the Psychology of Religion. Oxford University Press. Duriez, B., Soenens, B. & Hutsebaut, D. (2005). Introducing the shortened Post-Critical Belief Scale. Personality and Individual Differences, 38, 851-857. Religious orientations 19 Fontaine, J.R.J., Duriez, B., Luyten, P. & Hutsebaut, D. (2003). The internal structure of the Post-Critical Belief scale. Personality and Individual Differences, 35, 501-518. Gorsuch, R. L. (1997). Toward motivational theories of intrinsic religious commitment. In B. Spilka & D.N. McIntosh (Eds.). The psychology of religion: Theoretical approaches. (pp. 11-22). Boulder, CO: Westview. Grolnick, W. S., Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1997). Internalization within the family: The self-determination theory perspective. In J. E. Grusec & L. Kuczynski (Eds.), Parenting and children's internalization of values: A handbook of contemporary theory (pp. 135-161). New York: Wiley. Hunt, R. A., & King, M. B. (1971). The intrinsic-extrinsic concept: A review and evaluation. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 10, 339-356. Kasser, T., & Ryan, R. M. (1993). A dark side of the American dream: Correlates of financial success as a central life aspiration. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 410-422. Kasser, T., & Ryan, R. M. (1996). Further examining the American dream: Differential correlates of intrinsic and extrinsic goals. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 22, 280-287. Kirkpatrick, L. A. (1989). A psychometric analysis of the Allport-Ross and Feagin measures of intrinsicextrinsic religious orientation. In M. Lynn & D. Moberg (Eds.), Research in the social scientific study of religion (Vol. 1, pp. 1-30). Greenwich, CT: JAI. Kirkpatrick, L. A., & Hood, R. W. (1990). Intrinsic-extrinsic religious orientation: The boon or bane of contemporary psychology of religion? Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion,29, 442-462. Lens, W. (1997). Studiemotivatie. [Study Motivation]. De Psycholoog, 32, 53-59. Maltby, J. (1999). The internal structure of a derived, revised, and amended measure of the religious orientation scale: the ‘age-universal’ I-E scale-12. Social Behavior and Personality, 27(4), 407-412. Neyrinck , B., Lens, W., & Vansteenkiste, M. (2005). Goals and regulations of religiosity: A motivational analysis. In M.L. Maehr & S. Karabenick (Eds.). Advances in motivation and achievement, Vol. 14 Motivation and Religion (pp. 77-106). Greenwich , Conn.: Jai Press Inc. Neyrinck, B., Vansteenkiste, M., Lens, W., Hutsebaut, D., & Duriez, B. (2006). Cognitive, affective and behavioral correlates of internalization of regulations for religious activities. Motivation and Emotion, 30, 321-332. Ricoeur, P. (1970). Freud and Philosophy: An essay on interpretation. New Haven, Conn.: Yale University Press. (Translated by D. Savage, Original French edition 1965). Religious orientations 20 Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2000). Intrinsic and extrinsic motivations: Classic definitions and new directions. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 25, 54-67. Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2002). An overview of self-determination theory. In E. L. Deci & R. M. Ryan (Eds.), Handbook of self-determination research (pp. 3-33). Rochester, NY: University of Rochester Press. Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2006). Self-regulation and the problem of human autonomy: Does psychology need choice, self-determination, and will? Journal of Personality, 74, 1557-1586. Ryan, R. M., Rigby, S., & King, K. (1993). Two types of religious internalization and their relations to religious orientations and mental health. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 586-596. Sheldon, K. M., Ryan, R. M., Deci, E. L., & Kasser, T. (2004). The independent effects of goal contents and motives on well-being: It's both what you pursue and why you pursue it. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, 475-486. Van de Vijver, F. & Leung, K. (1997). Methods and data analysis for cross-cultural research. London: Sage. Vansteenkiste, M., Lens, W., & Deci, E. L. (2006). Intrinsic versus extrinsic goal-contents in selfdetermination theory: Another look at the quality of academic motivation. Educational Psychologist, 41, 19-31. Vansteenkiste, M., Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2008). Self-determination theory and the explanatory role of psychological needs in human well-being. In L. Bruni, F. Comim, & M. Pugno (Eds.), Capabilities and happiness (pp. 187-223). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. Wulff, D. M. (1997). Psychology of religion: Classic & contemporary. New York: Wiley. Religious orientations 21 Table 1 Correlations between main study variables Allport IR Allport Ep Allport Es Batson Quest -.02 .17* .31*** .12 Internalized .45*** .17* .09 .09 Controlled .07 .11 .23** .04 Inclusion .32*** .18* -.15 -.28** -.17* -.10 .28** Intrinsic Symbolic .11 Note: * p <.05; ** p <.01; *** p <.001. Religious orientations 22 Table 2 Regression analyses predicting religious orientations Allport IR Allport Ep Allport Es Batson Quest Step 1 Step 2 Step 1 Step 2 Step 1 Step 2 Step 1 Step 2 -.08 -.09 .22* .22** .06 .07 .17* .17* .28*** .32*** .17 .11 .06 .05 -.16 -.07 -.02 .04 .16 .15 .34*** .30** .03 .03 Internalized .44*** .37*** .11 .09 -.01 .04 .08 .14 Controlled .04 .05 .10 .10 .26** .25** .04 .05 Gender Age Intrinsic Inclusion .22** .15 -.15 -.28** Symbolic .21** -.14 -.08 .22** R² .31*** .38*** .12** Note: * p <.05; ** p <.01; *** p <.001. .16** .17*** .19*** .08* .21***