Handbook on Reading Disabilities Research Edited by Richard Allington and Anne McGill Franzen Taylor & Francis 2009 November The Political Contexts of Reading Disabilities Patrick Shannon and Jacqueline Edmondson – Penn State University The study of politics is the investigation of power within particular contexts. Power circulates through discourses among various groups who make use of and are used by values and language to participate in on-going events (e.g., Foucault, 1980; Gonick, 2003; Peet, 2007). These discourses and uses of discourse set parameters, influence actions, and position participants within events. Those who wield power in some contexts are powerless in others as negotiations push and pull participants in ways of their own making, but not entirely within their control. The discourses surrounding DIBELS can serve as a short introduction to the political contexts of reading disabilities, demonstrating how power works. Each summer our campus supports a reading program for children and youth who are experiencing difficulty in learning to read at school. The program serves as a practicum for masters degree students seeking reading specialist certification. Working from a three-to-one students/teacher ratio, we enroll between 20 and 30 children each year. Traditionally, the enrollment process begins in late spring after teachers and parents have conferred about a student’s progress throughout the academic year and their projections for success in the next grade. In the past, parent phone calls would trickle in 1 during late May and early June with discussions about “summer regress” and “ a boost going into next year.” Over the last three years, however, our program is full with a waiting list by the end of January. Parents call with panic in their voices, reporting that their kindergarten and first grade children are “reading disabled” because they have not “passed the DIBELS tests”. The Dynamic Indicators of Basic Early Literacy Skills (DIBELS) are a set of six fluency tests (letter names, initial sounds, phoneme segmentation, nonsense words, oral reading, and retelling) designed to enable regular monitoring of “pre-reading and early reading skills” (www.dibels.org). The purpose, content, and format of DIBELS are built upon the evidence-based conclusions of the National Reading Panel (2000) and National Research Council (1998) and are pronounced valid and reliable based on their correlations with other established tests. In these ways, DIBELS performs the discourse of experimental science – its language, logic, appearance and values – constructing reading abilities and disabilities in its wake. At the same time, DIBELS is a product that competes in a market created when the need for regular monitoring of these skills became generally accepted within the reading field. Although the basic materials of DIBELS can be downloaded from a website, and students’ scores can be processed and packaged into reports for one dollar per student, the tests are also available commercially in several forms along with test preparation materials and technical and human support as well. These products and services are advertised through professional journals and the internet. In these ways, DIBELS incorporates the discourse of business, working for a market share and to maximize profits, complicating what it means to determine reading ability and disability. 2 The market for the regular measurement of early reading was officially sanctioned when the Bush Administration implemented its Reading First Initiative of the No Child Left Behind education law of 2002. In order to insure that all students would test “proficient in reading” by 2014, the Department of Education connected federal funding to state and school district compliance with testing systems that could track schools’ progress toward that goal. With the discourses of science and business firmly underlying modern policy making, federal officials searched for a valid and reliable technology to standardize the practices and outcomes of reading education across the country. According to a Department of Education Inspector General’s Report (September, 2006), Reading First officials pressured states and school districts to adopt DIBELS as the appropriate technology in order to comply with federal policy and qualify for funding. In these ways, DIBELS projects a government discourse, framing the use of its tests as lawful behavior and a commitment to helping all students become proficient readers. Through these three (and other) discourses, DIBELS positions participants within reading education, replacing local knowledge and practices with the universal values, language, and rules of science, business and government. For example, adults’ familiarity with students’ interest in text or children’s questions around meanings are discounted in favor of students’ speed and accuracy when decoding sound and print. School traditions and teacher decisions give way to technologies that direct students’ attention to code in printed texts. Although these discourses are sometimes contradictory, they provide new possibilities for the participants as well as limit others. DIBELS enables administrators, 3 teachers, parents, and students to be “more effective”, “more efficient”, and “ more accountable” during reading instruction. However, DIBELS also defines these participants by the same terms. Each becomes defined as an abled or disabled administrator, teacher, parent or reader according to the six measures in the DIBELS battery, and their subsequent actions are disciplined by the meanings assigned and performed through the authority of these discourses. All other relations with text become irrelevant. To the extent that participants internalize these discourses, the power of DIBELS becomes invisible and natural, and local administrators make policies, teachers label students, parents worry about their children, and readers are made or unmade accordingly. And the reach of these discourses comes knocking on the door of our campus reading program with early calls from anxious parents, who have been warned by concerned teachers, who work in schools that must prove that they have a technology to produce proficient readers within specified time limits. Our program’s enrollment becomes younger each year – filled with kindergarten graduates and first grade repeaters. Although the parents’ reactions have changed, the discourses behind the politics are not new. DIBELS is only the most recent amplification for these discourses. In order to consider the political contexts of reading disabilities, we will examine the construction, maintenance and uses of the discourses of science, business and the government that have and continue to swirl around reading education in the United States. Although reading disabilities appears to be a psychological state of being, we understand the term to be ripe with politics at many levels. Our intention is to provide histories, locating the origins and consequences of these discourses within the emergence 4 of disabilities in the reading field during the 20th century and into the 21st. Within those histories, we shall search for values and interests that moved or move the term in various ways with consequences that ripple through clinics, schools, states, and national contexts. Science discourses In An Elusive Science: The Troubling History of Educational Research, Lagemann (2000) argues that scholars’ efforts to apply the principles of the Enlightenment to education resulted in the creation of psychology as an academic field. In order to be recognized as a field, “scientists of the mind” had to distinguish their work from previous philosophic and religious considerations on mental activity (Shore, 2001). Toward that end, would-be psychologists secularized the Christian virtues of faith and hope in terms of science and progress and operationalized the metaphysical questions about the mind—What can I know? What ought I to do? For what can I hope?—to: How does the brain work? Accordingly a science of the mind, psychology, would provide the positive knowledge that would lead human beings out of the problems of theological fictions and metaphysical egotism toward the natural laws of learning, increasing human capacity to make life easier and securing individual and social freedom. (Ward, 2002) Although efforts to separate science from philosophy and religion began during the French Enlightenment and accelerated with August Comte’s 1830 call for a social science to make human nature comprehensible, experimental psychology began in William Wundt’s laboratory in Leipzig, Germany during 1879 (Danziger, 2001). James McKeen Cattell (Wundt’s first assistant) and G. Stanley Hall are often credited for extending Wundt’s experimental work and bringing it to the United States, where it met 5 the burgeoning applications of science to industry, medicine, and the military. William James’s (1890) Principles of Psychology is considered the first American book on psychology. Twelve years in the writing, James’s two volumes included chapters on the functioning of the brain and brain activity. Yet, James’s work can also serve as a metaphor for the struggle among discourses of science, philosophy, and religion within the scholarly discussions of the mind. (Tolman, 2001) For example in 1902, James published The Varieties of Religious Experience: A Study of Human Nature in which he rationalized a belief in God, not on ontological or teleological grounds, but as therapeutic. He cautioned psychologists, “Science must be constantly reminded that her purposes are not the only purposes, and that the order of uniform causation which she has use for, and is therefore right in postulating, may be enveloped in a wider order, on which she has no claims at all. (PP, 1179). In 1904, he published his interpretation of the central crisis in American psychology, “Does Consciousness Exist?” Behind this ambiguity, American researchers interested in psychology worked to separate themselves from philosophy (Koch, 1992). In 1883, Hall established a psychology laboratory at Johns Hopkins and began to publish his results in the American Journal of Psychology in 1887. During the late 1880s, psychology departments opened at many established universities and became the founding discipline for the new Clark University (which hired Hall as its first president). There were ten laboratories by 1890 and twenty by 1893. The American Psychological Association (APA) was formed in 1892 and held its first meeting that year. In 1895, Cattell became the editor of the Psychological Review with the first recognized editorial review board. Because 6 philosophers found it difficult to present their papers at the APA conferences and then publish them in the psychology journals, they split from APA to form the Western Philosophical Association in 1901 and the American Philosophical Association in 1902. Appeals to science and the use of science methods were the primary reasons for tension between these groups (Toulmin and Leaery, 1992). In 1896, Karl Pearson explained, The scientific method consists in the careful often laborious classification of facts, the comparison of their relationships and sequences, and finally in the discovery by aid of the disciplined imagination of a brief statement or formula, which in a few words resumes a wide range of facts. Such a formula is called a scientific law. (p. 22) E. L. Thorndike (1906) explained the social advantages of this disciplined imagination and named psychologists as agents of this work. The judgments of science are distinguished from other judgments by being more impartial, more objective, more precise, and more subject to verification by any competent observer and being made by those who by their very nature and training should be better judges. Science knows or should know no favorites and cares for nothing in its conclusions but the truth. (p. 265) Starting with Wundt and Cattell, psychologists looked for scientific laws that would explain reading. (Venezky, 1984) Although Wundt was most interested in physiology, Cattell pursued his interests in understanding individual differences by focusing on observable behaviors he thought relatable to reading, including letter and word recognition, legibility, and attention span. His interest in differences led him to extend Galton’s work through the development of mental tests (a term he coined in 7 1890), which could be used to establish a normal range of intelligence by sampling individual behaviors. In the same study, Cattell predicted “experimental psychology is likely to take a place in the educational plan of our schools and universities” (p.390). Although Cattell’s efforts to capture human difference through mental testing proved futile, his students (E L. Thorndike, Walter Dearborn, and Arthur Gates) and others would bring the concept of mental testing to their experiments on learning in general and on reading in particular. In fact, Venezky (1984) labeled this era “The Golden Years” of reading research, leading to the publication of Edmond Burke Huey’s The Psychology and Pedagogy of Reading in 1908 and Thorndike’s establishment of the Journal of Educational Psychology in 1911. Kolers would note in 1968 “remarkably little empirical information has been added to what Huey knew (about the reading process), although some of the phenomena have now been measured more precisely” (Huey, 1908/1968, p. xiv.). In these golden years, the language about reading and reading education changed from contemplation of how reading fitted into moral development, stimulated thought, or captured beauty to analyses of perception, speed, and precision. The language of reading instruction changed from historical and descriptive accounts or personal evaluations of classroom practices and texts to statistical comparisons of basic perception among able and less able subjects and of experimental interventions against traditional methods. For example, Ruskin’s philosophical words were often quoted: “To use books rightly is to go to them for help; to be led by them into wider sight, purer conceptions than our own, and to receive from them the united sentences of the judges and councils of all time against our solitary and unstable opinions” (e.g., Brown, 1905). Such sentiments gave way in the 8 early 20th-century to a different discourse substituting scientific rationality and technological advances for the experiential basis of tradition treatments of reading and reading instruction. Consider the following statements. After all we have thus far been content with trial and error, too often allowing publishers to be our jury, and a real rationalization of the process of inducing a child with the practice of reading has not been made. (Huey, 1908/1968, p. 9) When the mechanics of reading, if we may use that phrase, are mastered, the whole attention may now be concentrated on the significance of the passage. (Judd, 1914, p. 366) Any progress toward measuring how well a child can read with something of the objectivity, precision, co-measurability, and conveniences which characterize our measurements of how tall he is, how much he can lift with his back, squeeze with his hand, or how acute his vision, would be a great help in grading, promoting, and testing the value of methods of teaching. (Thorndike, 1914, p. 1) Standard reading tests supply information concerning all phases of instruction from broader issues involved in the course of study to the detailed difficulties encountered by individual pupils. (Gray, 1915, p. 59) The Standard Test Lessons in Reading are offered to the teachers in our school [Lincoln School at Teachers College Columbia University] with confidence that 9 their use will give to pupils a rate of speed and power to comprehend exceeding that yielded by ordinary methods of teaching silent reading…Every lesson is a test and every test is a lesson…Not only is every test a lesson, but every test is a standard test; that is, it shows how well the normal or typical pupil would read these same lessons. (McCall and Crabbs, 1925, p. xi) One of the most potent factors in spreading the results of research is through a well-prepared set of readers and manuals, yet we find teachers still instructing children as they themselves were taught, absolutely ignorant and oblivious that science had discovered for us truths and that little children are entitled to he benefits of these discoveries. (Donovan, 1928, p. 106-07) In these remarks, the discourse of science positions teachers and publishers as impeding the translation of the laws of reading into classroom practice. The process of reading is divided and sequenced. Technology is declared to be the solution to individual variation, leading from accurate measurement to classroom instruction to individual remediation. The discourse established a normal range in the process of reading, the ways in which it is learned, and the speeds with which it is acquired. Students within this range become able readers, and those outside this normal range are disabled. Presuming all student capacities being equal, only teacher error kept these disabled readers from the normal able process, approach and speed. At the same time, the discourse of science positioned psychologists as experts within this field of reading and reading education, applying scientific methods to new issues of concern. Philosophers, theologians, 10 historians, literary scholars, or even other social scientists became less important, less powerful in the discussions and actions surrounding reading ability and disability. The rise of the scientific discourse in reading education has been neither straight nor smooth, but to the extent that evidence-based or scientifically based policy and practice are currently considered the norm, it has been successful. Across the 20thcentury, its increasing influence can be mapped in professional organizations (e.g., National Society for the Study of Education, American Educational Research Association, National Council of Teachers of English, International Reading Association, National Reading Conference, and Society for the Scientific Study of Reading), their journals and meetings, and state-of-the-field reports (NSSE Yearbooks Horn, 1919; Gray, 1925; Gray, 1937: Gates,1949; Austin & Morrison, 1963; Barton and Wilder 1964; Durkin, 1978; Anderson et al. 1985; Adams, 1990; Snow, 1998; National Reading Panel, 2000). Throughout, there has been a tone of certainty – now coming full circle back to physiology. Reading reflects language, and reading disability reflects a deficit within the language system….Using functional brain imaging, scientists around the world have discovered not only the brain basis of reading but also a glitch in the neural circuitry for reading in children and adults who struggle to read (Shaywitz and Shaywitz, 2004, p. 7 & 8) Discourses of Business The rhetoric of A Nation at Risk (1983), and that of the reports of crisis that followed, described reading ability and disability in economic terms (Shannon, 1998). 11 Such crisis-based analyses suggest that those who are able to meet the literacy demands of a global economy are those who will prosper and help the United States prosper. Those who are unable to meet those demands are those who will face difficult times, becoming social and economic liabilities. The ability to read in socially acceptable ways, then, becomes capital – something that can be accounted for and spent personally and socially. In this way, the financial well-being of the individual and society are embedded within the framing, infrastructure, and practices that surround reading ability and disability. Literacy skills required within particular economies set the parameters of who is considered able or disabled. As demands or perceived demands shift, the numbers in each group change accordingly. The technology and organization required to ensure ability and prevent disability move as well, creating markets and new areas of expertise in their wake. Teachers’ and students’ daily classroom practices are altered by these expectations, organizations, and technologies – even their relationships with one another and society transform. Of course, the economic rationale for reading instruction is not new. Northern colonies in the New World were taught reading in order to save individual souls, but used the metaphor of home economies to describe the responsibilities and practices of early public reading instruction (Smith, 2002). Thomas Jefferson and Horace Mann, nearly fifty years apart, argued that public schooling would create active citizens and able workers in order to develop democracy and build an economy one citizen at a time. In the 1800s, even the content of the school readers encouraged Americans to be industrious, entrepreneurial, and efficient (See Mosier, 1965). In this way, students became consumers of values as well as literacy skills through reading instruction. As the 12 economy turned more and more from agriculture to industry, the organizational schemes of business and industry were considered to be the primary solutions to consequent social challenges – industrialization, urbanization and immigration. The first sustained effort to bring business and industrial principles to public education is found in the work of the Committee on the Economy of Time in Education. Members of that committee sought to rationalize school curricula and instruction according to Frederick Winslow Taylor’s scientific management (Taylor, 1912), replacing personal judgment and rule of thumb with scientifically developed technology and standard practices. Their logic was that importing these business principles would increase both efficiency and the quality of the teachers’ work and product, running more smoothly, cheaply, and productively. Toward that end, the practices of the most productive teachers were analyzed in terms of tested results, useless movements discarded, and divided into elemental parts that could be described completely on instructional cards. These cards would enable any literate person to follow the one best system of teaching reading. Within these moves, students became more than metaphorical products of teachers’ labor. Their learning was subject to quality control of tests, and teachers’ efforts were standardized in order to increase their continuous fidelity to that system. The four reports of the Committee demonstrate the incomplete application of this business model to schooling in general and reading in particular. The first report presented a national survey in order to establish standards of expectations for teachers in each grade. Issues such as subjects within the curriculum, time devoted to reading instruction, rates of reading, vocabulary loads in textbooks, and the tests available to 13 determine student progress were listed to establish norms of expectations (Wilson, 1915). In the second and third reports, William S. Gray delved deeper into daily practices of reading instruction, advocating that silent reading instruction was preferable to oral because of its utility and efficiency in everyday tasks and provied a first glimpse of his tests – Standardized Oral Reading Paragraphs and Silent Reading (Wilson, 1917). The fourth report was to be the equivalent of the instructional card for scientific management. The effort throughout has been to put its recommendations in simple, direct language, that its report may constitute a handbook and guide for the use of teachers and supervisors who are interested in planning classroom procedures with due regard for both economy and efficiency in teaching and learning. (Wilson, 1919, pp. 7-8) Gray deduced forty-eight principles for reading instruction from thirty-five studies, covering norms for student progress across grade levels, suggestions for oral and silent reading, and specifications for printed materials. He emphasized that the experiments demonstrated that no single textbook method of teaching reading was necessarily superior to all others in terms of test results. Rather he argued that instructional efficiency and productivity varied according to how well teachers used the materials available to them. Just as Taylor had found in the steel mills, master teachers of reading were working in classrooms beside teachers who demonstrated little talent. Even before the publication of the fourth report, the NSSE formed the Committee on Materials in Education, combining the Committee on the Measurement of Educational Products and 14 the Committee on the Economy of Time in Education. “At this point, the Society assigned to the present Committee the task of embodying, in concrete materials to be used in classrooms, the principles arrived at by the earlier committees” (Bagley, 1920, p. 11). In short, the new Committee was to develop a technology that would raise the methods of struggling teachers to the same productivity of the master teachers. In this way, teachers became consumers and subjects of tests and materials in order to promote reading ability and prevent reading disability. The need for more effective technologies of quality monitoring of student learning and teacher instruction created new markets within the reading community. Tests within reading lessons, tests of reading progress over short and long periods, and diagnostic tests became ubiquitous in classrooms and schools across the next decades. Experts took pains to explain the benefits and limits of informal sampling of students’ reading that teachers could implement and analyze themselves, and standardized reading tests which required outside monitoring to be used effectively in order to identify specific areas of weaknesses among students. As early as 1906, existing textbook companies (e.g., World Book, Lippincott, and Public School) and companies created to publish tests (e.g., Courtis and Thorndike published their own tests before selling them to companies) entered this market. As with the textbook market, many of the tests were modest variations of a few popular products. The authors’ intended purpose of these tests was (as it is to this day) to sort students into categories of able and disabled readers (typically with scales within each category) in order to direct teachers’ attention and instruction to the areas designated as the causes of individual reading disability. The publishers’ purpose, however, was (as it is to this day) to insure that every reader was tested in every 15 way necessary in order to maximize the companies’ profits. Increased economic demands for literate workers fueled, as it continues to fuel, the need for more and better tests, keeping the test publishing market lucrative. Currently it is a multi - billion of dollars per year industry (PBS February 18 2008, www.pbs.org/nbr/site/onair/transcripts/080218i). The prevention and remediation of reading disability also created markets for publishers. The need for standardized instruction across classrooms led to the production of teachers’ manuals, which set the path for able development (Shannon, 1989). Although most teachers used textbooks during reading instruction prior to 1920, those books printed only brief directions for teachers within the students’ books. During the 1920s, psychologists’ calls for explicit directions to standardize teaching practices created a market for textbooks enhanced by lengthy teachers’ guidebooks. Reminiscent of Taylor’s instructional cards, the tone and content of directions set the scope, sequence, and expected outcomes of each lesson. Across the decades to the present, advances in reading psychology led publishers to identify new markets for “better and better” materials – manuals, anthologies, practice books and sheets, and informal and standardized tests – in order to monitor students’ flow through its system. Most reading experts proclaimed the scientific basis of each new set of materials and accessories, increasing the power of businesses in schools. Many of the more prominent experts worked for basal (later core) reading program publishers. Although periodically some experts have criticized the conservative influence of publishers on classroom practices and conceptions of ability and disability (e.g., Durkin, 1978; Goodman et al., 1988), most continued and continue to support the use of basal or 16 core reading programs during reading instruction, citing the quality control over teaching (e.g. Anderson, Osborn & Tierney, 1984). Adjusting quickly to changes in the professional rhetoric surrounding reading education to supply new supplementary products, basal publishers, however, have not changed the basic structure of these materials since the 1920s. New information is added and the nomenclature changes in order to gain new customers, but the structures and formats remain remarkably the same to maintain the old market (Chall & Squires, 1991). Early in the century, publishers maintained their influence through direct involvement in NSSE committee work in order to provide recommendations on reading education and instruction. In the 1950s, publishers contributed to the start-up capital of the International Reading Association, and continue to fund the professional meetings of that and other organizations concerned with reading and reading instruction (Jerrolds, 1977; Sears, 2007). For the educational materials publishing industry, all these efforts and expenses are simply marketing. A recent successful marketing venture concerns the consequences of testing of reading ability. As described by Garan (2005), readers who fail to reach the able range require more and perhaps different attention because standardized technological solutions have been unsuccessful. Prior to taking the tests, readers designated as “at risk” of failure can be served by extra preparation. Test publishers, basal publishers, and independent entrepreneurs have moved rapidly into this market, supplying goods and services to improve the odds of passing. Those who still fail provide a market for more materials and services as well. Since public schools are responsible for students becoming able readers, the tutoring market provides a new conduit for public education funds to flow toward 17 private companies and businesses. The continued flow has apparently been lucrative (see PBS 2008). Although there were scores of basal programs at the beginning of the 20thcentury, there are only five major programs and three publishers that control the reading materials market today. The news media designated some publishing companies as “Bush stocks” when the No Child Left Behind legislations passed (Metcalf, 2004). Government Discourses In North America, government discourse entered reading education in 1642 when the Massachusetts legislature passed a law requiring towns to make certain that “all youth under family Government be taught to read perfectly the English Tongue, have knowledge in capital laws, and be taught some orthodox catechisms, and that they be brought up to some honest employment, profitable to themselves and to the Commonwealth.” (quoted in Cubberly, 1934, p. 18). In this statement, the colonial legislature presaged six values to be found in later US government discourse: 1) the US is a republic and not a democracy (legislators made the decision), 2) rule of law (all youth – knowledge of capital laws), 3) equal protection of those considered citizens (under family Government), 4) building infrastructure to develop the economy (taught to read – some honest employment), 5) accountability (perfectly), and 6) ideology of party in power rules (English – orthodox catechisms). In 1789, with the ratification of the US Constitution and its first ten amendments, education was secured as a state’s right, continuing what had been the uneven commitment to public education and reading education across the various colonies. By necessity, each state legislature has had to address a series of questions: 18 What type of education should the state sponsor? Who should pay for that education? Who should determine what is taught and how it is taught in a state sponsored school? Who should have the opportunity to study? For how long? Toward what end? Who should decide and how should they decide when students are sufficiently educated? Who should decide who is qualified to teach? Variability in state legislature’s answers to these questions resulted in beautifully idiosyncratic consequences yet also standardized outcomes (On the one hand, think of the wonderful classroom libraries that developed in California schools when the state legislature argued that students should read more in the 1980s, but would not fund school libraries. On the other hand, consider the effects of large state textbook adoptions on the production of basal or core reading programs from which smaller states must also choose). Currently, definitions of reading ability and disability by state vary greatly because, under No Child Left Behind (P.L. 107-100), states have the right to determine these categories according to state standards and examinations. Students can change their classification simply by crossing a state boundary. Arguments for district, municipal, or other local control to address these questions would only increase such differences. However, as Coffey (1933) made clear, it is state governments that have the right to regulate local school districts. Local control is a courtesy, not a right. The maintenance of public schools is a matter of state, rather than of local concern. School districts exist because the state finds this is a convenient way to 19 carry out its educational program. It may require these districts to do any act, which it might perform directly. It may place restrictions upon them as it seems essential. It has full authority, unless its constitution provides otherwise, to prescribe the subject matter that may or shall be taught in its schools. (Coffey, 1931, p. 386). Although the states have the authority to enforce their educational decisions through sovereign power, economic sanctions have been the primary incentive since the end of the Second World War. When local communities’ tax bases decline, school districts become more dependent on state funding, and thereby are compelled to comply with state regulations and demands. The federal government may lack a constitutional mandate to manage public education, but its direct involvement began after the Soviet launch of Sputnik in 1957 when it appeared that the United States was losing the technological advantage that it had demonstrated during the World War II (Kaestle & Smith, 1983). The National Defense Education Act (P.L. 85-864) provided substantial funding for research on, and development of, school curricula deemed vital to the national defense and security – science, second language and mathematics curriculum – and asserted for the first time that general curriculum could be improved as well. With this legislation, two new values were added to the government discourses. First, public schools were recognized as a national security concern, and therefore, the federal government had a direct interest in schooling and curriculum. Second, funding would be the primary federal incentive to gain state compliance with its education initiatives. The federal government would fund research as well as direct payments to schools to enable programs. Subsequent federal 20 legislation (e.g., Elementary and Secondary Education Act P.L. 89-10, and the Education of All Handicapped Children Act P. L/ 94-142) reinforced these and the original government values and practices. The role of ideology within government discourses becomes most apparent in the shift of the hierarchy of values over the last half century. All administrations demonstrated governmental values by their choice of rhetoric, legislation, and research funding, but each positioned those values according to its underlying political ideology (Shannon, 2007). Contrast the emphasis in the rhetoric surrounding Project Head Start (Economic Opportunity Act of 1964, P.L. 88-452), a program designed to promote equal opportunity for disadvantaged children through federal funding, during the Johnson as compared to the Reagan administrations. Consider the following two quotes from each in turn. Archimedes told us many centuries ago: give me a lever long enough and a fulcrum strong enough and I can move the world. Today, at last, we have a prospect of a lever long enough and support strong enough to do something about our children of poverty. The lever is education, and the fulcrum is federal assistance.” (Commissioner of Education Francis Keppel, 1965) First the President wanted to reduce substantially federal spending for education. Second, he wanted to strengthen local and state control of education and to reduce dramatically the federal responsibility in this area….Fourth, the President wanted to encourage the establishment of laws and rules that would offer greatly expanded parental choice and that would increase the competition for students 21 among schools in newly created public and private structures patterned after the free market system that motivates and disciplines U. S. business and industry. (Secretary of Education Terrell Bell, 1986). Although both the liberal and conservative official expressed his commitment to schooling, the difference in values is explicit expressed in their statements. Ideology is also evident in the rhetoric employed by the neoliberal Clinton Administration (Edmondson, 2000) and the neoconservative G. W. Bush Administration (Goodman et al., 2004). Regardless of the ideology, Head Start was never sufficiently funded under any administration to enroll all eligible children. Furthermore, consider two federal attempts to protect minorities’ rights in public schools. In 1965, the Elementary and Secondary Education Act (P.L. 89-10, which has passed through several iterations to its current form No Child Left Behind) was originally the educational weapon in the War on Poverty, supporting other federal programs for food, health care, and housing. Gaps in achievement between the poor and the not poor were attributed to social inequalities that were to be addressed with a comprehensive and national plan. In schools, this required compensatory supplemental instruction for demonstrably poor, including racial minority, students. Lack of progress in reading was defined as a social disadvantage. In 1975, the Education for All Handicapped Children Act (later renamed Individuals with Disabilities Education Act, P.L. 108-446) sought services for a non-visible or demographically recognizable minority – children with physical and cognitive handicaps. This legislation marked the official entry of the language of disability into federal law (McGill-Franzen, 1987). To qualify for special educational services, students had to be tested and score outside the normal range, and 22 gaps in scores were attributed to individual traits and not social conditions. Clearly the testing since the beginning of the standards movement in the 1990s to the testing of NCLB follow this latter definition of reading disability as a personal deficit with a simple instructional solution. To overcome those gaps, the federal government began to fund research on best methods to assure success in reading before and during school. Such Federal funding of research is a third way in which ideology influences government discourses on reading education. The influence has not been singular or linear. Contrast the eclectic approaches of the First Grade Studies)(Bond and Dykstra, 1967) and Project Head Start (1960s), the information processing of The Center for the Study of Reading (1970s), the disciplined agenda of the National Institute for Child Health and Development (1970s-90s), and the “contextual” work of the Center for the Improvement of Early Reading Achievement (1997-2002). Federal and state administrations spent tens of millions of dollars to discover which methods would teach all American children to read. In order to translate those results for school personnel, the federal government funded a series of state-of-the-field reports: Becoming a Nation of Readers (Anderson, et al., 1985), Beginning to Read (Adams, 1990), Preventing Reading Difficulties in Young Children (Snow, Burns, & Griffin, 1997), and the National Reading Report (1998). In each, the definitions of reading ability and disability are contingent on who was at the table as the content of the reports were decided. The areas of focus and the methods of analyses were decided by who was selected to the panel. The five areas of tthe [National Reading Panel] Report do not capture all there is to reading. Rather they are the specialties of the panel members. Tom Trabasso in comprehension, Linnea Ehri in phonics, me for 23 fluency. I fought for my topic as did the others. The outcome could not have been otherwise. That does not compromise the report. It simply demonstrates its limits. (Samuels, 2006) Connections, Contradictions, Constants As demonstrated by the foregoing review, the political contexts of reading disabilities are mediated by the discourses of science, business, and government. The power of these discourses circulate through and among participants near and far in ways that establish opportunities and limits, impact actions, and position participants within their immediate circumstances. It is circumstance, not position, that determines participants’ power (See Fraatz, 1987). In order to understand the political contexts of reading disabilities, researchers must locate these discourses and map the landscape of the situation accordingly. Note that the discourses do not always work in conjunction with one another, because the values of one discourse can trump those in another. Consider for example, the struggles over the definitions of reading disabilities since the turn of the 21st century. Researchers, teachers, politicians and pundits with competing conceptions of reading and literacy presented their positions to professional and public audiences. Some affirmed traditional definitions and methods, while others called for higher standards and higher stakes for both. Some researchers and teachers disputed the assumptions inherent in how the categories of reading abilities and disabilities could and should be determined, while others denied the categories altogether. The ebb and flow of these debates have played out in several ways. For example, Dressman (2007) tracked a move from psychological theory to social theory in the 24 majority of articles published in three major research journals in the field of reading education research. A second example is the split within the membership of the National Reading Conference when in 1993 disgruntled members formed the Society for the Scientific Study of Reading in order to reinforce the traditional values of science and progress in the field of reading. A third example is University of Oregon Professor Douglas Carnine’s call for the professionalization of the reading field through “systematic aggressive action” to increase “the use of scientific methods to determine efficacy” in order to discredit nonexperimental methods (1999, p. 6). As director of the federally funded National Center to Improve the Tools for Educators, Carnine’s solution channeled all three of the discourses. Recent use oDuring second Clinton Administration to the present,f government discourses marchedwaded into this struggle with atypical consistency. As reviewed previously, the standards movement set standardized tests as the measurement of learning. NICHD, by its own acknowledged criteria, has funded only research proposals that have employed reliable and replicable experimental methods in the study of reading instruction during Reid Lyon’s tenure. Member selection for national panels on reading favored experimentalists over social theorists throughout the decade (Cunningham, 2001;Department of Education Inspector General, 2006; Kennedy, 2007. Finally, the federal government sought to end the struggle over the definition of reading disabilities with the Education Science Reform Act of 2002 (P.L. 107-249). Although the National Academy of Sciences was successful in negotiating somewhat inclusive language, many educational researchers continue to interpret that law as decidedly biased in favor of experimentalism (See Feuer, Towne & Shavelson, 2002). Taylor and her colleagues 25 expressed their frustration that the discourses of science, business and government overlap to produce a powerful but irrational force in the reading education field. Policymakers and educators feel the urgency of finding an easy answer and producing results. Foorman and her colleagues appear to present just such an easy answer in the last line of their article by suggesting that widespread reading failure might be prevented through explicit teaching of the alphabetic principle. Further, when the authors of this widely publicized study use their results as the basis for highlighting specific commercial programs such as Open Court and SRA Reading Mastery, they contribute to the impression that students’ reading problems will be solved if a school simply buys the right program. (Taylor et al., 2000, p. 23) In contrast to the variability and struggles within the discourse of science and government, business discourses have been consistent across the entire 20th century: Schools are responsible for and to the economy; they should be organized and run like businesses; and they provide markets for businesses. Accordingly, higher standards are needed because business demands on reading have increased. Straightforward practices based on the measurement of goals will be effective and efficient – business like. Both these values are implied in Taylor and her colleagues’ statement. Explicit in their statement, however, is the third value – the capitalization of reading disabilities. Publishing companies, testing companies and their parent companies as well as new entrepreneurs are heavily invested in the identification, maintenance, and practices of reading disabilities. “Discoveries” of new methods to identify and address reading disabilities are quickly transformed into commodities for sale. When the market is 26 saturated with products, then like other businesses, new types of disabilities are discovered and new (or repackaged) remedies marketed. This is not a recent phenomenon (Altwerger, 2005; Larson, 2007). The current billion-dollar test, textbook, and test prep industries rest upon those million-dollar textbook industries of the past. The Department of Education Inspector General’s report of September 22, 2006 (ED OIG/I13-F0017) described how discourses of science and government swirl inside the most powerful business discourse within the Reading First Initiative of the federal No Child Left Behind education law. Echoing Edmond Burke Huey and members of the Committee on the Economy of Time in Education, the Reading First Initiative policy requires “scientifically based research” as the criterion for making instructional decisions. The phrase appears over 100 times in the policy. Yet the Inspector General’s report found that science and research were not the criteria used to determine the test, materials, and practices that would define reading abilities and disabilities on a daily basis in American schools. Rather, materials favored (and sometimes authored) by external groups appointed by the Federal government were forced upon school personnel and students without benefit of rigorous research, and other programs which did have such scientific support were excluded systematically. The report provides the names of some officials who were implicated, but does not offer the names or titles of the questionable commodities or their publishers. The Washington Post was not so discrete (Grunwald, 2006). They report that “the vast majority of the 4,800 Reading First schools have now adopted one of five or six top selling commercial textbooks….most of the schools use the same assessment program…with little research backing.” That assessment program is DIBELS. The same 27 one that brings the “reading disabled” kindergarten and first grade “failures” to our Reading Camp. We have practiced the politics of reading disabilities in our own context by sharing comments with parents from the newspapers concerning the Inspector General’s report on the conflict of interest in the Reading First Initiative, Then we begin to work with the reading abilities that the children have brought with them to camp. References Adams, M. (1990). Beginning to Read. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Altwerger, B. ed. Reading for Profit: How the Bottom Line Leaves Kids Behind. Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann. Anderson, R. Heibert, E. Scott, J. & Wilkinson, J. (1985). Becoming a nation of readers. Washington D C: National Institute of Education. Anderson, R. Osborn, J. & Tierney, R. (1984). Learning to read in American schools: Basal readers and content texts. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbalum. Austin, M. & Morrison, C. (1963). The first r. New York: Wiley. Bagley, W. (1920). Introduction. In Whipple, G. ed. The New Materials of Instruction. 19th Yearbook of the National Society for the Study of Education. Bloomington, IL: Public School Publishing. Barton, A. & Wilder, D. (1964). Research and practice in the teaching of reading. In M. Miles, ed. Innovation in education. New York: Teachers College Press. Bell, T. (1986). Education policy development in the Reagan administration. Phi Delta Kappan, 68, 481-487. 28 Brown, G. (1906). Point of view. In G. Brown ed. On the teaching of English in elementary and secondary schools. Bloomington, IL: Public School Publishing. Education Sciences Reform Act of 2002, HR3801, 107th Congress, see Thomas.loc.gov. Cattell, J. M. (1890). Mental tests and measurement. Mind, 15, 373-380. Carnine, D. (1999). Campaign for moving research into practice. Remedial and Special Education, 20, 2-6-35. Chall, J. & Squires, J. (1991). The publishing industry and textbooks. In Barr, R. Kamil, M., Monsenthal, P. & Pearson, P. D. eds. Handbook of reading research vol. 2. New York: Longman. Coffey, W. (1931). Judicial opinion on textbook selection. In G. Whipple, ed. The Textbook in American Education. 30th Yearbook of the National Society for the Study of Education. Bloomington, IL: Public School Press. Cubberley, E.. (1934). Public education in the United States: A study and interpretation of American educational history. Boston: Houghton-Mifflin. Cunningham, J. (2001). The national reading panel report. Reading Research Quarterly, 36, 326-355. Danziger, K. (2001). Sealing off the discipline: Wilhelm Wundt and the psychology of memory. In C. Green, M. Shore, and T Tao, eds. The Transformation of Psychology: Influences of 19th Century Philosophy, Technology, and Natural Science. Washington D C: American Psychological Association. Donovan, H. (1928). Us of research in the teaching of reading. Elementary English Review, 14, 106-107. 29 Dressman, M. (2007). Theoretically framed: Argument and desire in the production of general knowledge about literacy. Reading Research Quarterly, 42, 332363. Durkin, D. (1978). What classroom observation reveal about reading comprehension instruction. Reading Research Quarterly, 14, 481-538. ED-OIG/I13-F0017 (2006, September). The Reading First Program Grant Application Process: Final Inspection Report. Office of Inspector General, United States Department of Education: Washington, DC. Edmondson, J. (2000). America reads: A critical policy analysis. Newark, DE: International Reading Association. Feuer, M., Ttowne, L., & Shavelson, R. Scientific culture and educational research. Educational Researcher, 31, 4- 29. Foucault, M. (1980). Power/Knowledge. Edited by C. Gordon. New York: Pantheon. Fraatz, M. (1987). The politics of reading: Power, opportunity, and prospects for change in American public schools. New York: Teachers College Press. Garan, E. (2005). Scientific flimflam: A who’s who of entrepreneurial research. In B. Altwerger, ed. Reading for Profit: How the Bottom Line Leaves Kids Behind. Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann. Gates, A. ed. (1949). Reading in the elementary school. 48h Yearbook of the National Society for the Study of Education. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Gonick, M. (2003). Between Femininities: Ambivalence, Identity and Edcuation of Girls. Albany, NY: SUNY Press. 30 Goodman, K. Shannon, P. Freeman, Y., & Murphy, S. (1988). Report card on basal readers. Katonah, NY: Richard C. Owens. Goodman, K., Shannon, P. Goodman, Y & Rappaport, D. (2004). Saving Our Schools. San Francisco, CA: Liveright. Gray, W. S. (1915). Selected bibliography upon practical tests of reading ability. In Wilson, H. ed. (1915). Minimum essentials in elementary school subjects – Standards and current practices. 14th Yearbook of the National Society for the Study of Education. Bloomington , IL: Public School Publishing. Gray, W. S. (1919). Principles of method in teaching reading as derived from scientific investigation. In Horn, E. ed. (1919). Fourth report of the committee on the economy of time in education. 18th Yearbook of the National Society for the Study of Education. Bloomington, IL: Public School Publishing. Gray, W. S. ed. (1925). Report of the national committee on reading. 24h Yearbook of the National Society for the Study of Education. Bloomington, IL: Public School Publishing. Gray, W. S. ed. (1937). Teaching reading: A second report of the national committee on reading. 36h Yearbook of the National Society for the Study of Education. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Grunwald, M. (2006, October 1). Billons for an inside game on reading. Washington Post. B 01. Horn, E. ed. (1919). Fourth report of the committee on the economy of time in education. 18th Yearbook of the National Society for the Study of Education. Bloomington, IL: Public School Publishing. 31 Huey, E. B. (1908/1968). The psychology and pedagogy of reading. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. James, W. (1890). The Principles of Psychology. New York: Henry Holt. James, W. (1902). The varieties of religious experience. New York: New American. James, W. (1904). Does consciousness exist? Journal of Philosophy, Psychology, and Scientific Methods, 1, 477-491. Jerrolds, B. (1977). Reading reflections: A history of the international reading association. Newark: DE: International Reading Association. Judd, C. (1914/. Reading tests. Elementary School Journal, 14, 365-373. Kaestle, C. & Smith, M. (1983). The federal role in elementary and secondary education 1940-1980. Harvard Educational Review, 52, 384-408. Kennedy, T. (May 9, 2007). The chairman’s report on the conflict of interest found in the implementation of the reading first program at three regional tech centers. Washington DC: Government Printing Office. Keppel, F. (1965). Aid to elementary and secondary education: Hearings before the general subcommittee on education of the committee on education and labor. 89th Congress. 1st Session. Washington D C: Government Printing Office. Koch, S. (1992). Wundt’s creature at age zero – and as centenarian: Some aspects of the institutionalization of the new psychology. In S. Koch & D. Leary, eds. A Century of Psychology as Science. Washington D C: American Psychological Association. Kolers, P. (1968). Introduction. In Huey, E. B. (1908/1968). The psychology and pedagogy of reading. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. 32 Lagemann, E. C. (2000). An elusive science: The troubling history of education research. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Larson, J., ed. (2007). Literacy as snake oil: Beyond the quick fix. New York: Peter Lang. McCall, W. & Crabbs, L. M. (1925). Standard test lessons in reading. Five volumes. New York: Teachers College Press. McGill-Franzen, A. (1987). Failure to learn to read: Formulating a policy problem. Reading Research Quarterly, 22, 475-490. Metcalf, S. (2004). Reading between the lines. In A. Kohn & P. Shannon, eds. Education Inc.: Turning Learning into a Business. Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann. Mosier, R. (1965). Making the American mind: Social and moral ideas in the McGuffey readers. New York: Russell and Russell. National Commission on Excellence in Education. (1983). A nation at risk: The imperatives of educational reform. Washington D C: Government Printing Office. National Reading Panel (2000). Report of the national reading panel: Teaching children to read: An evidence-based assessment of the scientific research literature and its implications for reading instruction. Washington D C: National Institute of Child Health and Human Development. Pearson, K. (1896). The grammar of science. New York: MacMillan. Peet, R. (2007). Geographies of power. New York: Zed Books. Samuels, S. J. (2006, May 3). Statement made during the question and answer session of the Reading Hall of Fame presentation at International Reading Association Converntion, Chicago, IL. 33 Sears, L. (2007). Reaction, initiation, and promise: A historical study of the international reading association. An unpublished dissertation, University of Pittsburgh. Shannon, P. (1988). Broken promises: Reading instruction in 20th Century America. Westview, CT: Bergin & Garvey. Shannon, P. (2001). iSHOP/you shop: Raising questions about reading commodities. Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann. Shannon, P. (2007). Reading against democracy: The broken promises of reading instruction. Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann). Shaywitz, S. & Shaywitz, B. (2004). Reading disability and the brain. Educational Leadership, 6 (6), 6-1.1. Shore, M. (2001). Psychology ad memory in the midst of change: The social concerns of late 19th century North American psychologists. In C. Green, M. Shore, and T Tao, eds. The Transformation of Psychology: Influences of 19th Century Philosophy, Technology, and Natural Science. Washington D C: American Psychological Association. Smith, N. B. (2002). American reading instruction. 5th Edition. Newark, DE: International Reading Association. Snow, C., Burn, S. & Griffin, P. (1998). Preventing reading difficulties in young children. Washington D C: National Academy Press. Taylor, B., Anderson, R., Au, K., & Raphael, T. (2000). Discretion in the translation of research to policy: A case from beginning reading. Educational Researcher, 29, 16-26. 34 Taylor, F. W. (1912). The present state of the art of industrial management. The American Magazine, 71, 1-9. Thorndike, E. L. (1906). The principles of teaching based on psychology. New York: A. G. Seler. Thorndike, E. L. (1914). The measurement of ability in reading. Teachers College Record, 15, 1-17. Tolman, C. (2001). Philosophical doubts about psychology as a natural science. In C. Green, M. Shore, and T Tao, eds. The Transformation of Psychology: Influences of 19th Century Philosophy, Technology, and Natural Science. Washington D C: American Psychological Association. Toulmin, S. & Leary, D. (1992). The cult of empiricism in physchology and beyond. In S. Koch & D. Leary, eds. A Century of Psychology as Science. Washington D C: American Psychological Association. U.S. Department of Education’s Inspector General. (September 22, 2006). The Reading First Program’s Grant Application Process: Final Inspection Report. Washington D C: Government Printing Office. Venezky, R. (1984). The history of reading research. In P. D. Pearson ed. Handbook of Reading Research. New York: Longman. Ward, S. (2002). Modernizing the mind: Psychological knowledge and the remaking of society. Westport, CT: Praeger. Wilson, H. ed. (1915). Minimum essentials in elementary school subjects – Standards and current practices. 14th Yearbook of the National Society for the Study of Education. Bloomington , IL: Public School Publishing. 35 Wilson, H. ed. (1917). Second report of the committee on minimum essentials in elementary school subjects. 16th Yearbook of the National Society for the Study of Education. Bloomington, IL: Public School Publishing. Wilson, H. ed. (1918). Third report of the committee on economy of time in education. 17th Yearbook of the National Society for the Study of Education. Bloomington, IL: Public School Publishing. 36