Depressed Mood in Early Adolescent Relationships 1 Depressed Mood and the Quality of Parental and Peer Relationships in Early Adolescence Nicole K. Phillips Distinguished Majors Thesis University of Virginia May 2001 Advisor: Joseph P. Allen Second Reader: Gerald Clore Running Head: DEPRESSED MOOD IN EARLY ADOLESCENT RELATIONSHIPS Depressed Mood in Early Adolescent Relationships 2 Abstract Research has shown that teens with difficulties in relating to parents and peers are at greater risk for developing symptoms of depression and show later deficits in social functioning. The present study employed a multi-reporter, multi-method, longitudinal design to examine associations between depressed mood and personal relationships in a socioeconomically diverse community sample of 174 adolescents (83 males, 91 females). Results indicate that undermining autonomy and relatedness in disagreements with the mother predicts change in depressed mood over time. To a lesser degree, less conflict resolution with close friends and withdrawal from friends predict changes in depressed mood. In turn, depressed mood predicts changes in peerreported companionship and self-perceptions of friendship competence and social acceptance. Limitations and implications are discussed. Depressed Mood in Early Adolescent Relationships 3 Acknowledgements I would like to express my appreciation to my advisor, Dr. Joseph P. Allen, for his help and support through this process. I would also like to thank Maryfrances Porter for her tireless assistance with both the technical and theoretical details involved in the current project. This project would also not have been possible without the assistance of my second reader, Dr. Gerald Clore, and the many other graduate and undergraduate students who have collected data and provided valuable feedback. Depressed Mood in Early Adolescent Relationships 4 Depressed Mood and the Quality of Parental and Peer Relationships in Early Adolescence While a majority of children and adolescents experience some minor symptoms of depression from time to time, it is estimated that around 5% of children and 10 – 20% of adolescents experience levels of depressed mood requiring outside care (Lewinsohn, Hops, Roberts, Seeley, & Andrews, 1993; Reynolds & Johnston, 1994; Roberts, Attkisson, & Rosenblatt, 1998; Speir, Sherak, Hirsch, & Cantwell, 1995). It is also estimated that one out of every six children hospitalized for a mental illness has a diagnosis of major depressive disorder (Reynolds & Johnston, 1994). Throughout the literature, depression in adolescence has been associated with interpersonal problems (e.g., Reinherz, Giaconia, Hauf, Wasserman, & Silverman, 1999) and family environments (e.g., Blatt & Homann, 1992; Kaslow, Deering, & Racusin, 1994). However, the relationships among parents, peers, and depression have not been fully explored in early adolescence. Depression in Adolescence Depression has not always been recognized as a problem affecting children and adolescents. Traditionally, depression was thought to only affect older adolescents and adults because younger children did not have a sufficiently developed superego to be afflicted by the disorder. (Speier et al., 1995) Today, depression is being increasingly seen to be a major problem affecting children and adolescents (Reynolds & Johnston, 1994). In addition, early onset of depression has been identified as a significant risk factor for development of later affective disorder in adulthood. (Lewinsohn, Rohde, Klein, & Seeley, 1999; Rao, Hammen, & Daley, 1999; Reynolds & Johnston, 1994) Depression is associated with a significant impairment of normal functioning. Among children with a diagnosis of depression, one study showed that 87% exhibited functional Depressed Mood in Early Adolescent Relationships 5 impairment as a result of their disorder (Simonoff et al., 1997). This impairment of early occurrences of the disorder can have lasting effects later in life. Reinherz et al. (1999) found in a community sample of young adults that having received a diagnosis of depression in late adolescence was associated with significantly impaired functioning regardless of current diagnosis. Occurrence of depression during the transition to adulthood was also associated with greater interpersonal problems and an increased need for social support in this sample. Cognitive models of depression provide a working model to explain the functional impairments associated with depression. According to Beck (1967), depression is a result of negative patterns of thinking. In depressed persons, as negative schema are utilized more and more to explain events, they begin to be activated by a wider and wider range of stimuli leading to systematic errors in thinking. Thus, when a negative event occurs, depressed persons tend to attribute it to internal, global, and stable controls. Building on this idea, Seligman (1992) argued that these negative cognitive biases lead depressed persons to adopt a hopeless stance in life. Depressed persons do not believe they are capable of changing situations under their control and thus show patterns of responding in those instances similar to those of non-depressed persons facing uncontrollable situations. These cognitive models provide a good explanation for the mental processes at work in depression, however they do not provide an explanation of how these processes emerge. Depressed Mood in Early Adolescent Relationships 6 Parental Relationships and Depression Attachment theorists have postulated that the origin of negative cognitive biases and expectations in relationships is found in a child’s early relationship with his/her caregivers (Bowlby, 1969a). Insecure attachment relationships are believed to form in dyads in which the mothering response to the infant is inadequate to his or her needs (Ainsworth, Blehar, Waters, & Wall, 1978). The child internalizes beliefs about relationships and self-worth from the response of the mother to his/her signals (Bowlby, 1969a). Thus, negative experiences in early relationships can lead to internalized views of the self as bad or unworthy (Bowlby, 1969b). In the case of depression, Bowlby (1969b) argues that “in most forms of depressive disorder, including that of chronic mourning, the principal issue about which a person feels helpless is his ability to make and to maintain affectional relationships.” (Bowlby, 1969b, p. 247) Problematic attachment relationships with the parents, and especially the mother, have been empirically linked to internalizing disorders in childhood. For instance, psychopathology present at age six has been linked to attachment style at age one (Lewis, Feiring, McGuffog, & Jaskir, 1984). As the child moves from childhood to adolescence, attachment strategies continue to be associated with the occurrence of internalizing symptoms. Research with older adolescent hospital populations has shown that a majority of the adolescent hospitalized for mental disorders show insecure attachment patterns (Allen, Hauser, & Borman-Spurrell, 1996; Rosenstein & Horowitz, 1996). Additionally, adolescents with an ambivalent style were more likely to exhibit anxiety or dysthymia and were more likely to be withdrawn in social interactions (Rosenstein & Horowitz, 1996). The relationship between attachment style and pathology has also been reported in non-clinical populations such that insecure and preoccupied Depressed Mood in Early Adolescent Relationships 7 attachment strategies were associated with higher levels of depression (Allen, Moore, Kuperminc, & Bell, 1998; Kobak, Sudler, & Gamble, 1991). These attachment strategies become increasingly important through adolescence as the child begins to establish his/her own distinct identity. Being able to establish and maintain autonomy with the parents while keeping a sense of relatedness in the relationship has increasingly been identified as a major developmental task of adolescence (Allen, Hauser, Bell, & O’Connor, 1994; Allen & Land, 1999; Collins, 1990; Hill & Holmbeck, 1986). The security of the attachment relationship provides the basis for the child’s development of independence in conflicts (Allen & Hauser, 1996). If the child has a secure relationship with the mother, then he or she would tend to feel more comfortable asserting individual opinions and creating disagreement because he/she can be confident of the caregiver’s continued support. In less secure dyads mastering this task becomes more difficult. When the adolescent has no precedent for believing the parental figure will provide continued, unconditional support, the child becomes less likely to consistently assert his/her own opinion and less likely to attempt to maintain a rapport with the parent (Allen & Land, 1999). As an indicator of attachment cognitions and the quality of the relationship with a parent, autonomy and relatedness may be a useful predictor of later dysfunction (Garber & Kaminski, 1999). In a sample of high-risk adolescents, Allen, Hauser, Eickholt, Bell, & O’Connor (1994) found that adolescents who undermine their autonomy and relatedness with their parents at time 1 tend to show higher rates of negative affect at time 2. Among moderately at-risk adolescents, difficulties asserting autonomy in disagreements with the father have been shown to predict report of increased depressed mood over time (Leibman, 1998). However, previous research has Depressed Mood in Early Adolescent Relationships 8 not explored autonomy and relatedness as a predictor of change in depression in a normative sample of early adolescents to determine if the same trends hold. Peer Relationships and Depression While relationships with parents are important to overall development, relationships with peers gain increasing importance throughout adolescence (Buhrmester, 1996). Maintaining friendships is important to overall positive psychosocial functioning (Berndt, 1996). Depressed peers are more likely to be viewed as less likeable (Peterson, Mullins, & Ridley-Johnson, 1985) and less popular (Connolly, Geller, Marton, & Kutcher, 1992; Oldenburg & Kerns, 1997). Studies of psychiatric inpatient adolescents show that problems with peers are associated with persistent major depressive disorder (Goodyer, Herbert, Tamplin, Secher, & Pearson, 1997) and that depressive symptoms are uniquely predicted by quality of social relationships even after accounting for other disorders present (Barerra & Garrison-Jones, 1992). While much research has established a link between general friendships and depression, especially in clinical populations, relatively little research has focused on close friendship competence as both a predictor and outcome of depression in a normative, non-clinical sample of early adolescents. Much of the current literature on childhood and early adolescent peer relationships and predictions with depression in non-clinical samples focuses on the child’s sociometric status. With sociometric procedures, children are given the opportunity to list peers both that they like and that they do not like. Both neglected (seldom listed) and rejected (receiving primarily negative listings) kids have been shown to exhibit higher levels of internalizing disorders (Hecht, Inderbitzen, & Bukowski, 1998), and these classifications have also been shown to predict negative psychological outcomes including depression (Coie, Lochman, Terry, & Hyman, 1992; Coie, Terry, Lenox, & Lochman, 1995; Panak & Garber, 1992). Conversely, research has Depressed Mood in Early Adolescent Relationships 9 additionally supported the predictive nature of depression in sociometric status (Little & Garber, 1995). According to interpersonal theories of depression (Coyne, 1976), when depressed, a person begins to feel low self-worth and low effectiveness in social relationships. Often he/she will try to compensate by increasing social contact and excessive reassurance-seeking. This, in turn, creates more tension between the friend and the depressed person. As the depressed person senses the new tension in the relationship, they begin intensifying the reassurance-seeking, thus driving the friend farther away. These negative cognitive and relational cycles fuel the episode of depression. Findings from sociometric studies support the theory that depression has a bidirectional relationship with friendship throughout childhood and adolescence. As children progress from childhood to early adolescence, they begin to see friendships as more intimate and supportive (Berndt & Perry, 1990). Friends become more dependent on one another as intimacy in relationships increases during this stage of development (Collins & Repinski, 1994). Thus, given their increasing importance, it would seem that close friendships would especially be marked by the interpersonal difficulties that accompany depression, however little research has been done to compare mild to moderately depressed adolescents to normal adolescents. Overall, insecure attachments to both parents and peers have been associated with higher levels of depressed mood in early adolescence (Armsden, McCauley, Greenberg, Burke, & Mitchell, 1990). In parental relationships, expressions of attachment through strivings for autonomy and relatedness are thought to have an impact on development of negative affect. Difficulties with peers are also thought to have a predictive relationship with depression, and, in turn are predicted by the depressed mood. However, these models of depression have not been Depressed Mood in Early Adolescent Relationships 10 fully explored in normal, non-clinical samples of adolescents. Because of this, the current study examined the following questions and hypotheses. 1) What is the relationship between depressed mood and quality of interactions with parents in a community sample of early adolescents? Do difficulties in negotiating the autonomy relationship predict changes in depression over time? Based on past findings linking depressed mood to family environments, it was hypothesized that parenting would be related both cross-sectionally and longitudinally with depressed mood. Chiefly, it is believed that problems in asserting autonomy while maintaining relatedness with parents will predict changes in depressed mood over time. 2) Does quality of peer relationships in adolescence predict changes in depressed mood in a community sample of early adolescents? Given the increasing importance of peer relationships in adolescence, it is hypothesized that quality of peer relationships will be correlated with reported severity of depressed mood. Specifically, it is hypothesized that difficulties in relationships with peers fuel changes in depressed mood over time. 3) Does depression predict changes in quality of peer relationships over time in a community sample of early adolescents? Research and theories on depression suggest that the influence of peer relationships on depression operates in a cyclical fashion with depression both influencing and being influenced by quality of friendships. Thus, it also was hypothesized that depressed mood would predict changes in friendship quality over time. Method Participants A sample of 184 adolescents was recruited from three successive classes at a public middle school in the Southeastern United States. A smaller sample of 174 adolescents (83 males, 91 females) was selected from the total sample based on completion of at least one visit Depressed Mood in Early Adolescent Relationships 11 during the second of four waves of data collection. Data from the first wave were collected when adolescents were approximately 13 years old (M = 13.34, SD = 0.63). The second wave was collected one year later when the adolescents were about 14 years old (M = 14.17, SD = 1.28). The sample was ethnically and socioeconomically diverse with 103 adolescents identifying themselves as Caucasian, 49 as African American, and 22 as other/unspecified. Adolescents’ parents reported a mean total family income of $43,873 per year (range: $2,500 – $70,000). Data regarding adolescents’ friendship competence were additionally collected from one close friend nominated at each wave by the adolescent. Friends reported that they had known the adolescents for an average of 4.15 years (SD= 3.10) at the first wave and an average of 4.49 years (SD = 3.29) at the second wave. At the present time, the final sample does not include 10 adolescents who have not yet returned for visits with friends in wave two. These participants differ slightly from the population used in the analyses in age (M = 13.78 years; SD = 0.93) and total family income (M = $35,250, range: $7,500 - $70,000). The male to female ratio remained fairly constant with 4 males and 6 females not included. Adolescents of minority descent were more represented in this group than in the larger sample with 2 classifying themselves as African American and 4 identifying themselves as being of other or unspecified descent. Four classified themselves as being Caucasian. Procedure Nine hundred seventy-five adolescents and their families were first contacted by mail and asked to return a postcard if interested in being in a study on adolescent development. Families who indicated they were interested in the study were invited to complete a three-hour visit. Depressed Mood in Early Adolescent Relationships 12 Further follow-ups by phone were used to recruit a more diverse sample. Approximately 20% to 30% of those originally contacted agreed to participate. At the first wave, adolescents and their parents each received $20 for participating in the three-hour interview. Adolescents were invited to return for a two-hour follow-up visit one year later. They received $20 for participating in the second wave. All interviews took place in private rooms within a university clinic. At the initial visit for each wave of data collection, adolescents were asked to list the names of close friends whom they would be comfortable having in the study. Adolescents gave consent for the friends they nominated to be contacted and asked to participate. The researchers then contacted the adolescent’s closest friend and invited them to participate in a study on adolescent relationships with the adolescent. Peers who agreed to participate were invited to come in for a three-hour visit with the adolescent. Both the adolescent and friend each received $20 at the first wave and $20 at the second wave for participating. All participants gave active, informed consent before each interview session, and parents provided signed informed consent for participants under the age of 18. Confidentiality of information provided was guaranteed in the informed consent, in the introductions to each session, and during the sessions. A U.S. Department of Health and Human Services Confidentiality Certificate (MH-97-44) protected participants’ data from subpoena by local, state, and federal courts. It was noted that this would not apply if the adolescent were suicidal, homicidal, or being abused. Participants were taken to separate rooms to complete measures to further ensure privacy. All participants were told they should skip questions and decline any portion of the session with which they felt uncomfortable or to which they felt they could not respond honestly. Interviewers offered to read each measure so that literacy would not prevent participation. A list of referral agencies in the community was given at the end of each visit. Depressed Mood in Early Adolescent Relationships 13 Measures Depressed Mood. The Childhood Depression Inventory (CDI; Kovacs & Beck, 1977) is a 27-item questionnaire based on the Beck Depression Inventory (see Appendix A). Each item consists of three statements indicating varied levels of depressed mood. Participants are instructed to choose the statement that best describes how they have been feeling over the past two weeks. Responses on each item are then rated 0 to 2 with 0 being assigned for the statement indicating the least amount of depressed mood and 2 being assigned to the statements endorsing the highest level of depressed mood. Scores on all items are totaled to yield a summary score of depressed mood. The CDI has been shown to have high internal consistency and good discriminant validity (Kazdin, 1990). Cronbach’s alpha coefficients for the total depressed mood scale in this sample were .85 at wave 1 and .83 at wave 2. Autonomy and Relatedness. Levels of autonomy and relatedness in the mother-child dyad were assessed from the Family Interaction Task (FIT) coded using the Autonomy and Relatedness Observational Coding Scheme (AR; Allen, Hauser, Bell, Boykin, & Tate, 1996 v.2.14). Each pair participated in an 8-minute videotaped revealed differences task in which they discussed a family issue rated by both to be a topic of disagreement. Common topics selected included grades, money, problems with siblings or friends, and household rules. Two independent coders rated individual speeches in the task on one or more of 10 subscales. Subscales are grouped into four classifications. Behaviors promoting autonomy included stating reasons for disagreeing clearly and showing confidence in stating opinions. Behaviors coded as inhibiting autonomy included recanting one’s own position, overpersonalizing the disagreement/blurring the boundary between person and argument, and pressuring the other to agree. Relatedness-promoting behaviors were actions such as making queries, validating, Depressed Mood in Early Adolescent Relationships 14 agreeing, and being engaged in the interaction overall. Conversely, behaviors inhibiting relatedness included distracting, ignoring, cutting the other off, and making hostile or devaluing statements. See Appendix B for further information on individual scales. Each member of the dyad received total scores on each of these four scales which were combined to create overall negative autonomy/relatedness and overall positive autonomy/relatedness scales. Previous research has found this coding system to be a reliable predictor of adolescent functioning and overall family functioning (Allen, Hauser, Bell, & O’Connor, 1994). Interrater reliability was determined for the scales using intraclass correlations. Intraclass correlations for overall postive autonomy/relatedness were .86 , and intraclass correlations for overall negative autonomy/relatedness were .84. Attachment to Parents and Peers. The Inventory of Parent and Peer Attachment (IPPA; Armsden & Greenberg, 1989) was used to assess adolescents’ perceptions of their current parent and peer relationships. Questions relating to parents and peers were administered separately. See Appendix C for questions concerning relationships with parents and Appendix D for questions concerning relationships with peers. For both types of relationship, the adolescents rated 25 items on a 5-point Likert scale to determine levels of trust, communication, and alienation as well as to generate a composite score of the overall quality of their relationships. This composite measure has been shown to have good test-retest reliability and has been related to other measures of family environment and teen psychosocial functioning (Armsden & Greenberg, 1987). Cronbach’s alpha coefficients for the total parental attachment scales in this sample were .90 at wave 1 and .92 at wave 2. Cronbach’s alpha coefficients for the total peer attachment scales in this sample were .92 at both waves. Depressed Mood in Early Adolescent Relationships 15 Quality of Friendships. At both waves of data collection, adolescents and their friends completed the 40-item Friendship Quality Questionnaire (FQQ; Parker & Asher, 1993). Each statement is evaluated on a 5-point Likert scale with 1 = strongly disagree and 5 = strongly agree. Items were grouped and averaged to yield six scales assessing different aspects of friendship quality: validation and caring, help and guidance, companionship and recreation, conflict and betrayal, conflict resolution, and intimate exchange. Only scales for companionship and recreation and conflict resolution were used in the present study (see Appendix E). Items on the companionship and recreation scale assessed the level of interaction between the pair (e.g. “We do fun things together a lot.”). The conflict resolution scale measured the level of difficulty in settling disagreements within the dyad (e.g. “We make up easily when we have a fight”). Parker et al. (1993) found these scales to have good reliability and internal validity. Cronbach’s alpha coefficents for the scales used in this study at wave 1 are .72 for teen report of companionship and recreation, .65 for teen report of conflict resolution, .80 for peer report of companionship and recreation, and .60 for peer report of conflict resolution. At wave 2, Cronbach’s alpha coefficients were .81 for teen report of companionship and recreation, .72 for teen report of conflict resolution, .83 for peer report of companionship and recreation, and .65 for peer report of conflict resolution. Social Competence. Self-report measures of adolescent social competence were obtained at both waves using the Self-Perception Profile for Adolescents (Harter, 1988). Scales for close friendship competence and social acceptance from this measure were used in the present study (see Appendix F). The close friendship competence scale asks about the adolescents’ ability to form and maintain close, intimate friendships in which they could share personal thoughts and secrets (e.g., “Some people are able to make close friends - BUT – Other people find it hard to Depressed Mood in Early Adolescent Relationships 16 make really close friends”). The social acceptance scale measures the teen’s perception of their overall acceptance within their peer group and ability to make friends in general (e.g., “Some teens do have a lot of friends – BUT – Some teens don’t have a lot of friends”). For each item, adolescents first decided which side of the “BUT” was more descriptive of them. Next they rated whether the chosen statement was “really true” or “sort of true.” This procedure yielded a 4point rating scale (i.e., “really true” on the right side of the “BUT” was scored as 4, while “really true” on the left side of “BUT” was scored 1). Cronbach’s alpha coefficients for the close friendship scale were .66 at wave 1 and .72 at wave 2. Cronbach’s alpha coefficients for the social acceptance scale were .80 at wave 1 and .76 at wave 2. Withdrawal. Peer ratings of adolescents’ level of withdrawal were obtained using the Pupil Evaluation Inventory (PEIP; Pekarik, Prinz, Liebert, Weintraub, & Neale, 1976). The original inventory included scales for withdrawal, aggression, and likeability; however, only the scale for withdrawal was used in this project (see Appendix G). Close friends were asked to rate the teen’s behavior over the past six months. The measure consists of nine items rated on a 3 point Likert scale with 0 = not at all like him/her and 2 = a lot like him/her. Scores are averaged to create a total withdrawal score. Higher ratings indicate higher levels of withdrawal. The scale has been shown to be both reliable and valid in sociometric assessments of children’s social behavior (Weintraub, Prinz, & Neale, 1978). Cronbach’s alpha coefficient for the withdrawal scale in this sample was .73 at wave 1. Depressed Mood in Early Adolescent Relationships 17 Results Preliminary Analyses Sample Means. Means and standard deviations for depression, measures of autonomy and relatedness, and measures of friendship competence are presented in Table 1. Depressed mood in the overall sample showed a small decrease between wave 1 and wave 2 (t(174) = -3.34, p < .001). Most variables assessing friendship quality did not change significantly from wave 1 to wave 2. Only levels of perceived social acceptance increased from wave 1 to wave 2 (t(154) = 1.99, p < .05). Insert Table 1 about here Demographics. All outcome variables were examined for potential effects of gender, minority status, and total family income. Minority status for this sample was defined as selecting an option other than “Caucasian” on a measure of ethnic identity. Estimated total family income was obtained from the mother during the first wave of data collection. No significant effects of gender, minority status, or family income were found for overall depressed mood. Moderate effects of both minority status and family income were noted for behaviors promoting autonomy and relatedness, but only small effects of gender and minority status were found for behaviors undermining autonomy and relatedness. Gender was correlated with several of the friendship competence measures at both wave 1 and wave 2. Correlations are presented in appendix F for descriptive purposes. Demographic variables will be entered as covariates in the regression analyses below when they are related to the outcome variable of interest. Depressed Mood in Early Adolescent Relationships 18 Adolescents without Friends. Analyses were conducted for the current sample to examine differences between the adolescents with a friend participating at both waves of data collection and the adolescents who were unable to nominate a friend who could participate at the second wave (N = 11). Results for depression at wave 1 and wave 2 indicated that the teens without friends at wave 2 were not significantly different in terms of depression at either point of data collection. Similarly, the teens who had not had a friend return for the second wave visit were not seen as different in terms of withdrawal and had similar overall levels of friendship quality. Primary Analyses Correlations. Initial analyses examined zero-order correlations between depressed mood and the scales for autonomy and relatedness in the mother-child dyad and for close friendship competence. These correlations are presented for descriptive purposes in Table 2. Moderate correlations were found both across measures and across time. These correlations are examined further in the primary regression analyses below. Insert Table 2 about here Hypothesis 1. Depressed mood and overall negative autonomy and relatedness toward the mother. To test the hypothesis that adolescents who fail to establish autonomy and maintain relatedness may be at increased risk of developing symptoms of depression, an index score of negative autonomy and relatedness was used to predict depressed mood at wave 2 after accounting for the level of depressed mood at wave 1. Given the stability of tendencies toward depressed mood over time, Depressed Mood in Early Adolescent Relationships 19 accounting for initial levels of depressed mood allows us to examine the adolescents’ undermining if their own autonomy and relatedness as a predictor of relative change in depressed mood. Hierarchical regression analyses indicated that overall negative autonomy and relatedness accounted for approximately 7% of the variance in depression at time 2 (Table 3). Therefore, having an overall style of undermining autonomy while failing to establish a sense of relatedness predicted a change in number of reported symptoms of depressed mood over time ( = .26, p .01). Insert Table 3 about here A significant interaction between total negative autonomy and relatedness score and overall depressed mood at time 1 was also discovered. After accounting for the effects of depression at time 1 and total negative autonomy and relatedness, an interaction term was added to the hierarchical regression. Analyses indicated that the interaction between depression and negative autonomy and relatedness accounted for 3% of the variance above the effect of autonomy and relatedness alone (Table 3). A graphical representation of the interaction is provided in Figure 1. For adolescents who did not report high levels of depression at time 1, interaction style with the mother did not affect depression at time 2. However, for adolescents who reported higher levels of depression at wave 1, undermining autonomy and relatedness seemed to have a larger impact on depressed mood at wave 2 ( = .17, p .05). Insert Figure 1 about here Depressed Mood in Early Adolescent Relationships 20 Hypothesis 2. Peer reports of withdrawal and depressed mood. To examine the predictive relationship between social withdrawal and depressed mood, a hierarchical regression analysis was conducted to predict depressed mood at wave 2 from peer reports of withdrawal while accounting for depressed mood at wave 1. Overall, peer reports of withdrawal predicted approximately 2% of the variance in depressed mood at wave 2 (Table 4). Being seen by peers as being withdrawn predicts being more depressed over time ( = .14, p .05). Insert Table 4 about here Conflict resolution in close friendships and depressed mood. To test the relationship between ability to resolve conflicts with a close friend and overall depressed mood, a hierarchical regression analysis was used to predict depression at time 2 from the teen’s report of the level of conflict resolution with their best friend while controlling for the level of depression at wave 1. Conflict resolution scores uniquely predicted about 2% of the variance in depressed mood at time 2 (Table 5). Thus, lower levels of conflict resolution in close friendships may, to a small degree, predict changes in depressed mood over time ( = -.13, p .05). Insert Table 5 about here Hypothesis 3. Depressed mood and companionship with close friends. To examine the hypothesis that depression predicts changes in the level of recreation with close friends, close peer reported levels of companionship and recreation in the relationship were examined through hierarchical Depressed Mood in Early Adolescent Relationships 21 regression analyses. Peer report of companionship at wave 2 was predicted from teen reports of level of depressed mood at wave 1 while controlling for reports of companionship at wave 1 (Table 6). Overall, depressed mood predicted about 7% of the variance in companionship reported by the close friend at wave 2. Therefore, higher levels of depression predict less companionship felt by close peers over time ( = -.24, p .01). Insert Table 6 about here Depressed mood and self-report of close friendship competence. To test the hypothesis that depression leads adolescents to feel they are less able to have close friendships, a hierarchical regression analysis was used to predict beliefs about overall close friendship competence from depression at time 1 while controlling for minority status and perceived close friendship competence at time 1 (Table 7). Results indicate that depression at time 1 predicted 3% of the variance in close friendship competence at time 2. Thus, higher levels of depression may, to a small degree, predict changes in perceived close friendship competence over time ( = -.19, p .05). Insert Table 7 about here Depressed mood and self-report of social acceptance. To further explore the hypothesis that depression predicts change in beliefs about overall social competence, a hierarchical regression analysis was used to predict beliefs about overall social acceptance from depression at time 1 while controlling for perceived social acceptance at time 1 (Table 8). Results indicate that depression at time 1 predicted 4% of the variance in close friendship competence at time 2. Depressed Mood in Early Adolescent Relationships 22 Thus, higher levels of depression may, to a small degree, predict changes in perceived close friendship competence over time ( = -.20, p .01). Insert Table 8 about here In addition, an interaction between perceived social acceptance at time 1 and depressed mood also appeared to have a predictive relationship with social acceptance at time 2. Social acceptance at time 2 was predicted from the interaction of social acceptance at time 1 and depressed mood at time 1 after accounting for initial levels of social acceptance and depressed mood. The interaction accounted for an additional 2% of the change in perceived social acceptance over time. Thus, a total of 6% of the change in perceived social acceptance could be accounted for by depressed mood at time 1 and the interaction of depressed mood and perceived social acceptance at time 1 (Table 8). Adolescents with lower levels of perceived social acceptance and higher levels of depressed mood at time 1 exhibited lower levels of perceived social acceptance at time 2 compared to adolescents with lower levels of depressed mood. Adolescents with higher levels of perceived social acceptance at time 1 showed little variance in perceived social acceptance at time 2 regardless of depressed mood ( = .15, p .05; Figure 2). Insert Figure 2 about here Discussion In this study, associations between depressed mood and relationships with parents and peers were reported. Difficulties in establishing an autonomous stance in disagreements with the mother while maintaining a sense of relatedness during a disagreement was found to predict Depressed Mood in Early Adolescent Relationships 23 changes in depression over time. Having difficulties in both areas overall was associated with having even greater depressed mood in children with a prior history of depressed mood. Problems in resolving conflicts with friends and becoming more withdrawn from friends only slightly predicted changes in depression over time; however, depressed mood predicted changes in perceptions of close friendship competence and overall social acceptance as well as level of companionship reported by a close friend. Parental Relationships Based on previous work by Allen, Hauser, Eickholt, et al. (1996) with an at-risk sample of older adolescents, it was hypothesized that difficulties in negotiating conflicts with the mother would predict changes in depression over time. In the present sample, difficulties in both establishing autonomy and maintaining relatedness predicted changes in depression over time. For adolescents with higher levels of depressed mood at the beginning of the study, exhibiting negative autonomy and relatedness behaviors was associated with higher levels of depressed mood at the second point of data collection. Adolescents who reported fewer symptoms of depression at wave 1 demonstrated less change in depressed mood relative to undermining autonomy and relatedness with their mothers. Unsuccessfully negotiating the developmental task of establishing autonomy while maintaining relatedness appears to have an impact on psychosocial functioning even in early adolescence. One explanation for this relationship is that difficulties in establishing autonomy and relatedness arise from underlying beliefs about relationships gained from the mother during early stages of development. Difficulties in negotiating conflict are thought to be related to underlying views of relationships learned through the early attachment relationship with the mother (Allen & Land, 1999). In fact, research has shown that infant attachment strategies are strongly related Depressed Mood in Early Adolescent Relationships 24 to later expressions of autonomy and relatedness in interactions with the parents (Becker-Stoll & Fremmer-Bombik, 1997). These early attachment relationships provide the adolescent with a basis for expectations within relationships. Insecure attachments to the mother arise when the mother does not adequately respond to the infant’s needs. Both dismissing and preoccupied attachment strategies have been linked to problems with psychosocial functioning (cf. Allen & Land, 1999). Underlying preoccupations with attachment figures are thought to lead to failures to learn to regulate negative affect (Rosenstein & Horowitz, 1996). In adolescence, these preoccupied strategies may tend to lead to behaviors aimed at attracting attention from the caregiver while allowing expression of anger and resentment. Thus, adolescents who are acting on these preoccupations with attachment would be expected to show higher rates of depressive symptoms due to deficits in self-regulation. Peer Relationships Since relationships with peers gain increasing importance in adolescence (Buhrmester, 1996) and interpersonal difficulties have been previously associated with depression (Reinherz et al., 1999), it was also hypothesized that problems in peer relationships would also predict changes in depression. In the current study, teens who reported greater difficulties in negotiating conflicts with close friends tended to report more depression over time. Similarly, teens who were seen by their friends as more withdrawn subsequently reported more depression over time, although this relationship was small. Although these associations are relatively small, it seems that the adolescents who become more depressed are the adolescents who have more difficulties in relating to friends overall. This may occur because adolescents who have had problems relating with parents may carry those expectations and behaviors into relationships with friends. Depressed Mood in Early Adolescent Relationships 25 Alternately, through negative interactions with close friends, adolescents may gain further negative views of the self, leading to later increased feelings of depressed mood. The relationship between peer relationships and depressed mood was also examined in the opposite direction, using depressed mood to predict changes in quality of friendships over time. In the present sample of early adolescents, depression predicted changes in selfperceptions of close friendship competence and social acceptance. For adolescents who saw themselves as less socially accepted at wave 1, also showing high levels of depressed mood at wave 1 was associated with less social acceptance at wave 2 compared to those showing lower levels of depressed mood at time 1. One possible explanation for the predictive relationship between perceptions of friendship competence and depressed mood is that teens experiencing higher levels of depression also experience more negative beliefs and expectations about their environments. Alternately, these results may also reflect underlying difficulties in relating to others as a result of expressed symptoms of depressed mood. Depressed mood in early adolescents also predicted changes in their friends’ reports of companionship and recreation in the dyad. Adolescents reporting higher levels of depressed mood are seen by their close friends as spending less time together and engaging in fewer recreational activities. This could be a result of tendencies to draw away from relationships when depressed. Depression has been widely associated with greater withdrawal from social interaction (Reynolds & Johnston, 1994). Additionally, lower ratings of companionship by peers could also reflect, to some degree, a greater dissatisfaction with the relationship overall as a result of the depressive symptoms. Depressed Mood in Early Adolescent Relationships 26 Limitations While this study does provide support for the link between depressed mood and personal relationships in early adolescence, several limitations to this study should be addressed. The results suggest that parental and peer relationships have a predictive relationship with depression, however, personal relationships are only one aspect of the overall picture of depression. Additionally, even though depressed mood, quality of relationships with the mother, and quality of friendships were assessed over time, the predictive relationships found were correlational in nature. Thus, the direction of the effects cannot be assumed. It is also possible that a third variable could be, in part, responsible for the effects noted. Secondly, because the participants in the study were recruited from the community to participate in a project investigating relationships in adolescence, teens with higher levels of depressed mood, greater conflict with parents, or difficulties in relating to peers may have been less likely to volunteer for participation. Overall levels of depressed mood in the current sample either did not differ significantly or were slightly lower than levels reported for non-depressed control populations (e.g. Armsden, McCauley, Greenberg, Burke, & Mitchell, 1990; Sanders, Dadds, Johnston, & Cash, 1992). Levels of self-reported attachment to peers were higher than previously reported for typical adolescents (Armsden & Greenberg, 1987; Armsden et al., 1990) as were levels of companionship (Parker & Asher, 1993). These deficits could point to a greater recruitment rate for children with fewer problems relating to friends. However, given that the results found are for a sample with comparatively high social functioning, it would be expected that they would also hold for adolescents reporting lower quality relationships with friends. Further research is needed to explore the associations between depressed mood and personal relationships in broader samples of adolescents. Depressed Mood in Early Adolescent Relationships 27 Overall Implications Overall, the results from this study present an important view of depressed mood and psychosocial functioning in early adolescence. Teens who are having greater difficulties relating to both parents and peers seem to report more changes in depression over time. Depression, in turn, seems to have an effect on the adolescents’ perceptions of their own social abilities. Adolescents with higher rates of depression also seem to withdraw from close friendships as indicated by lower ratings of companionship by peers. Although gender has been widely associated with development of depression in adolescence (e.g., Hankin, et al., 1998; Nolen-Hoeksema & Girgus, 1994; Wichstrøm, 1999), no effects of gender were found for depressed mood in this study. Before puberty, rates of depression in children are almost equal for males and females with males experiencing slightly more depressed symptoms than females (Nolen-Hoeksema, Girgus, & Seligman, 1991). During early adolescence, girls begin to exhibit slightly more depression (Hankin, et al., 1998) and by late adolescence girls are almost twice as likely to be depressed (Nolen-Hoeksema & Girgus, 1994). Research has associated girls’ risk for developing depressive symptoms as compared to boys’ risk to poorer relationships with parents and increased interpersonal vulnerabilities (Leadbeater, Kuperminc, Blatt, & Hertzog, 1999). Thus, it would be expected that as adolescents move from early to middle adolescence, gender differences would become more important and the associations between psychosocial functioning and depressed mood would be different for males and females. Further research is needed to explore the relationships between gender, psychosocial functioning, and depressed mood in the transition from early to middle adolescence. Depressed Mood in Early Adolescent Relationships 28 Depression is also found to be highly comorbid with other internalizing and externalizing disorders in childhood and adolescence, especially with anxiety (cf. Reynolds & Johnston, 1994). Comorbidity of internalizing disorders is often indicative of more severe deficits in overall functioning, and it is theorized that anxiety may actually precede depression in many cases (Seligman & Ollendick, 1998). Research suggests that problems in psychosocial functioning may not be entirely specific to depression. Self-consciousness, self-esteem, and decreases in activities have all been related specifically to depression while other aspects of psychosocial functioning were only slightly more related to depression than other disorders (Lewinsohn, Gotlib, & Seeley, 1997). Further research is needed to determine whether the associations between relationships and depressed mood reported here are specific to depression or if they are also found for other forms of disorder. This study establishes the relationship between undermining autonomy and relatedness in disagreements with the mother and later depressed mood as well as the relationship between quality of friendships with peers and depressed mood. The relationships supported here provide important information on developmental and psychosocial processes associated with depressed mood in early adolescence. However, there are still many questions for further research to examine regarding the development and importance of psychosocial functioning in early adolescence. Future research is needed to determine at what age autonomy and relatedness first becomes a predictor of change in depressed mood and what developmental processes lead to its increasing importance. Future research is also needed to explore the relationship between peers and negative affect as children transition from late childhood to early adolescence. Depressed Mood in Early Adolescent Relationships 29 References Ainsworth, M. D. S., Blehar, M. C., Waters, E., & Wall, S. (1978). Patterns of attachment: A psychological study of the strange situation. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Allen, J. P., & Hauser, S. T. (1996). Autonomy and relatedness in adolescent-family interactions as predictors of young adults’ states of mind regarding attachment. Development and Psychopathology, 8, 793-809. Allen, J. P., Hauser, S. T., Bell, K. L., Boykin, K. A., & Tate, D. C. (1996). Autonomy and Relatedness Coding System Manual. Unpublished manuscript. University of Virginia, Charlottesville. Allen, J. P., Hauser, S. T., Bell, K. L., & O’Connor, T. G. (1994). Longitudinal assessment of autonomy and relatedness in adolescent-family interactions as predictors of adolescent ego development and self-esteem. Child Development, 65, 179-194. Allen, J. P., Hauser, S. T., & Borman-Spurrell, E. (1996). Attachment theory as a framework for understanding sequelae of severe adolescent psychopathology: An 11-year follow-up study. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 64, 254-263. Allen, J. P., Hauser, S. T., Eickholt, C., Bell, K. L., & O’Connor, T. G. (1994). Autonomy and relatedness in family interactions as predictors of expressions of negative adolescent affect. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 4, 535-552. Allen, J. P., & Land, D. (1999). Attachment in adolescence. In J. Cassidy & P. R. Shaver (Eds.). Handbook of attachment: Theory, research, and clinical applications (pp. 319335). New York: Guilford Press. Allen, J. P., Moore, C., Kuperminc, G., & Bell, K. (1998). Attachment and adolescent psychosocial functioning. Child Development, 69, 1406-1419. Depressed Mood in Early Adolescent Relationships 30 Armsden, G. C., & Greenberg, M. T. (1987). The inventory of parent and peer attachment: Individual differences and their relationship to psychological well-being in adolescence. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 16, 427-454. Armsden, G. C., & Greenberg, M. T. (1989). Inventory of parent and peer attachment: Revised manual. Unpublished revised version. University of Washington. Seattle, Washington. Armsden, G. C., McCauley, E., Greenberg, M. T., Burke, P. M., & Mitchell, J. R. (1990). Parent and peer attachment in early adolescent depression. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 18, 683-697. Barrera, M., & Garrison-Jones, C. (1992). Family and peer social support as specific correlates of adolescent depressive symptoms. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 20, 1-16. Beck, A. T. (1967). Depression: Clinical, Experimental, and Theoretical Aspects. New York: Harper & Row. Becker-Stoll, F., & Fremmer-Bombik, E. (1997, April). Adolescent-mother interaction and attachment: A longitudinal study. Paper presented at the biennial meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Washington, DC. Berndt, T. J. (1996). Exploring the effects of friendship quality on social development. In W. M. Bukowski, A. F. Newcomb, & W. W. Hartup (Eds.), The company they keep: Friendship in childhood and adolescence (pp. 346-365). Cambridge UK: Cambridge University Press. Berndt, T. J., & Perry, T. B. (1990). Distinctive features and effects of early adolescent friendships. In R. Montemayor, G. R. Adams, & T. P. Gullotta (Eds.) From childhood to adolescence: A transitional period? Advances in adolescent development (Vol. 2, pp. 269-287). Newbury Park: Sage. Depressed Mood in Early Adolescent Relationships 31 Blatt, S. J., & Homman E. (1992). Parent-child interaction in the etiology of dependent and self-critical depression. Clinical Psychology Review, 12, 47-91. Bowlby, J. (1969a). Attachment and loss: Vol. 1. Attachment. New York: Basic Books. Bowlby, J. (1969b). Attachment and loss: Vol. 3. Loss, sadness and depression. New York: Basic Books. Buhrmester, D. (1996). Need fulfillment, interpersonal competence, and the developmental contexts of early adolescent friendship. In W. M. Bukowski, A. F. Newcomb, & W. W. Hartup (Eds.), The company they keep: Friendship in childhood and adolescence (pp. 158-183). Cambridge UK: Cambridge University Press. Coie, J. D., Lochman, J. E., Terry, R., & Hyman, C. (1992). Predicting early adolescent disorder from childhood aggression and peer rejection. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 60, 783-792. Coie, J., Terry, R., Lenox, K., Lochman, J., & Hyman, C. (1995). Childhood peer rejection and aggression as predictors of stable patterns of adolescent disorder. Development and Psychopathology, 7, 697-713. Collins, W. A., & Repinski, D. J. (1994). Relationships during adolescence: Continuity and change in interpersonal perspective. In R. Montemayor, G. R. Adams, & T. P. Gulotta (Eds.) Personal relationships during adolescence. Advances in adolescent development (Vol. 2, pp.85-106). Newbury Park: Sage. Collins, W. A. (1990). Parent-child relationships in the transition to adolescence: Continuity and change in interaction, affect, and cognition. In R. Montemayor, G. R. Adams, & T. P. Gulotta (Eds.) From childhood to adolescence: A transitional period? Advances in adolescent development (Vol. 2, pp.85-106). Newbury Park: Sage. Depressed Mood in Early Adolescent Relationships 32 Connolly, J., Geller, S., Marton, P., & Kutcher, S. (1992). Peer responses to social interaction with depressed adolescents. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 21, 365-370. Coyne, J. C. (1976). Depression and the response of others. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 85, 186-193. Garber, J., & Kaminski, K. M. (2000). Laboratory and performance-based measures of depression in children and adolescents. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 4, 509-525. Goodyer, I. M., Herbert, J., Tamplin, A., Secher, S. M., & Pearson, J. (1997). Shortterm outcome of major depression: II. Life events, family dysfunction, and friendship difficulties as predictors of persistent disorder. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 36, 474-480. Hankin, B. L., Abramson, L. Y., Moffitt, T. E., Silva, P. A., McGee, R., & Angell, K. E. (1998). Development of depression from preadolescence to young adulthood: Emerging gender differences in a 10-year longitudinal study. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 107, 128-140. Harter, S. (1988). Manual for the self-perception profile for adolescents: Unpublished manuscript, University of Denver, Colorado. Hecht, D. B., Inderbitzen, H. M., & Bukowski, A. L. (1998). The relationship between peer status and depressive symptoms in children and adolescents. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 26, 153-160. Hill, J. P., & Holmbeck, G. N. (1986). Attachment and autonomy during adolescence. Annals of Child Development, 3, 145-189. Kaslow, N. J., Deering, C. G., & Racusin, G. R. (1994). Depressed children and their families. Clinical Psychology Review, 14, 39-59. Depressed Mood in Early Adolescent Relationships 33 Kazdin, Alan E. (1990). Childhood depression. Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry & Allied Disciplines, 31, 121-160. Kobak, R. R., Sudler, N., & Gamble, W. (1991). Attachment and depressive symptoms during adolescence: A developmental pathways analysis. Development and Psychopathology, 3, 461-474. Kovacs, R., & Beck, A. (1977). An empirical clinical approach toward a definition of childhood depression. In J.G. Schulterbrandt & A. Raskin (Eds.), Depression in children: Diagnosis, treatment and conceptual models (pp. 1-25). New York: Raven Press. Leadbeater, B. J., Kuperminc, G. P., Blatt, S. J., & Hertzog, C. (1999). A multivariate model of gender differences in adolescents’ internalizing and externalizing problems. Developmental Psychology, 35, 1268-1282. Leibman, S. E. (1998). Familial predictors of depressed mood in adolescence (Doctoral dissertation, University of Virginia, 1998). Dissertation Abstracts International, 58(12-B), 6814. Lewinsohn, P. M., Gotlib, I. H., & Seeley, J. R. (1997). Depression-related psychosocial variables: Are they specific to depression in adolescents? Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 106, 365-375. Lewinsohn, P. M., Hops, H., Roberts, R. E., Seeley, J. R., & Andrews, J. A. (1993). Adolescent Psychopathology: I. Prevalence and incidence of depression and other DSM-III-R Disorders in High School Students. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 102, 133-144. Lewinsohn, P. M., Rohde, P., Klein, D. N., & Seeley, J. R. (1999). Natural course of adolescent major depressive disorder: I. Continuity into young adulthood. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 38, 56-63. Depressed Mood in Early Adolescent Relationships 34 Lewis, M., Feiring, C., McGuffog, C., & Jaskir, J. (1984). Predicting psychopathology in six-year-olds from early social relations. Child Development, 55, 123-136. Little, S. A., & Garber, J. (1995). Aggression, depression, and stressful life events predicting peer rejection in children. Development and Psychopathology, 7, 845-856. Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Girgus, J. S. (1994). The emergence of gender differences in depression during adolescence. Psychological Bulletin, 115, 424-443. Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Girgus, J. S., & Seligman, M. E. P. (1991). Sex differences in depression and explanatory style in children. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 20, 233-245. Oldenberg, C. M., & Kerns, K. A. (1997). Associations between peer relationships and depressive symptoms: Testing moderator effects of gender and age. Journal of Early Adolescence, 17, 319-337. Panak, W. F., & Garber, J. (1992). Role of aggression, rejection, and attributions in the prediction of depression in children. Development and Psychopathology, 4, 145-165. Parker, J. G., & Asher, S. R. (1993). Friendship and friendship quality in middle childhood: Links with peer group acceptance and feelings of loneliness and social dissatisfaction. Developmental Psychology, 29, 611-621. Pekarik, E. G., Prinz, R. J., Liebert, D. E., Weintraub, S., & Neale, J. (1976). The Pupil Evaluation Inventory: A sociometric technique for assessing children’s social behavior. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 4, 83-97. Peterson, L., Mullins, L. L., & Ridley-Johnson, R. (1985). Childhood depression: Peer reactions to depression and life stress. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 13, 597-607. Depressed Mood in Early Adolescent Relationships 35 Rao, U., Hammen, C., & Daley, S. (1999). Continuity of depression during the transition to adulthood: A 5-year longitudinal study of young women. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 38, 908-915. Reinherz, H. Z., Giaconia, R. M., Hauf, A. M. C., Wasserman, M. S., & Silverman, A. B. (1999). Major depression in the transition to adulthood: Risks and impairments. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 108, 500-510. Reynolds, W. M., & Johnston, H. F. (1994). The nature and study of depression in children and adolescents. In Reynolds, W. M., & Johnston, H. F. (Eds.), Handbook of Depression in Children and Adolescents (pp. 3-17). New York: Plenum Publishers. Roberts, R. E., Attkisson, C. C., & Rosenblatt, A. (1998). Prevalence of psychopathology among children and adolescents. American Journal of Psychiatry, 155, 715725. Rosenstein, D. S., & Horowitz, H. A. (1996). Adolescent attachment and psychopathology. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 64, 244-253. Sanders, M. R., Dadds, M. R., Johnston, B. M., & Cash, R. (1992). Childhood depression and conduct disorder: I. Behavioral, affective, and cognitive aspects of family problem-solving interactions. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 101, 495-504. Seligman, L. D., & Ollendick, T. H. (1998). Comorbidity of anxiety and depression in children and adolescents: An integrative review. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 1, 125-144. Seligman, M. E. (1992). Helplessness: On Development, Depression, and Death. New York: W. H. Freeman & Company. Depressed Mood in Early Adolescent Relationships 36 Simonoff, E., Pickles, A., Meyer, J. M., Silberg, J. L., Maes, H. H., Loeber, R., Rutter, M., Hewitt, J. K., & Eaves, L. J. (1997). The Virginia twin study of adolescent behavioral development: Influences of age, sex, and impairment on rates of disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry, 54, 801-808. Speier, P. L., Sherak, D. L., Hirsch, S., & Cantwell, D. P. (1995). Depression in children and adolescents. In E. E. Beckham & W. R. Leber (Eds.), Handbook of Depression (2nd ed., pp. 467-493). New York, NY: Guilford Press. Weintraub, S., Prinz, R. J., & Neale, J. M. (1978). Peer evaluations of the competence of children vulnerable to psychopathology. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 6, 461-473. Wichstrøm, L. (1999). The emergence of gender difference in depressed mood during adolescence: The role of intensified gender socialization. Developmental Psychopathology, 35, 232-245. Depressed Mood in Early Adolescent Relationships 37 Appendix A Childhood Depression Inventory Directions: Kids sometimes have different feelings and ideas. From each of the group of three sentences pick one sentence that describes you best in the past two weeks. There is no right or wrong answer. Circle your choice. 1 0 1 2 I am sad once in a while. I am sad many times. I am sad all the time. 2 0 1 2 Nothing will ever work out for me. I am not sure if things will work out for me. Things will work out for me OK. 3 0 1 2 I do most things OK. I do many things wrong. I do everything wrong. 4 0 1 2 I have fun in many things. I have fun in some things. Nothing is fun at all. 5 0 1 2 I am bad all the time. I am bad many times I am bad once in a while. 6 0 1 2 I think about bad things happening to me once in a while. I worry that bad things will happen to me. I am sure that terrible things will happen to me. 7 0 1 2 I hate myself. I do not like myself. I like myself. 8 0 1 2 All bad things are my fault. Many bad things are my fault. Bad things are not usually my fault. 9 0 1 2 I do not think about killing myself. I think about killing myself but I would not do it. I want to kill myself. Depressed Mood in Early Adolescent Relationships 38 10 0 1 2 I feel like crying every day. I feel like crying many days. I feel like crying once in a while. 11 0 1 2 Things bother me all the time. Things bother me many times. Things bother me once in a while. 12 0 1 2 I like being with people. I do not like being with people many times. I do not want to be with people at all. 13 0 1 2 I cannot make up my mind about things. It is hard to make up my mind about things. I make up my mind about things easily. 14 0 1 2 I look OK. There are some bad things about my looks. I look ugly. 15 0 1 2 I have to push myself all the time to do my schoolwork. I have to push myself many times to do my schoolwork. Doing schoolwork is not a big problem. 16 0 1 2 I have trouble sleeping every night. I have trouble sleeping many nights. I sleep pretty well. 17 0 1 2 I am tired once in a while. I am tired many days. I am tired all the time. 18 0 1 2 Most days I do not feel like eating. Many days I do not feel like eating. I eat pretty well. 19 0 1 2 I do not worry about aches and pains. I worry about aches and pains many times. I worry about aches and pains all the time. 20 0 1 2 I do not feel alone. I feel alone many times. I feel alone all the time. Depressed Mood in Early Adolescent Relationships 39 21 0 1 2 I never have fun at school. I have fun at school only once in a while. I have fun in school many times. 22 0 1 2 I have plenty of friends. I have some friends but I wish that I had some more. I do not have any friends. 23 0 1 2 My schoolwork is alright. My schoolwork is not as good as before. I do very badly in subjects I used to be good in. 24 0 1 2 I can never be as good as other kids. I can be as good as other kids if I want to. I am just as good as other kids. 25 0 1 2 Nobody really loves me. I am not sure if anybody loves me. I am sure that somebody loves me. 26 0 1 2 I usually do what I am told. I do not do what I am told most times. I never do what I am told. 27 0 1 2 I get along with people. I get into fights many times. I get into fights all the time. Depressed Mood in Early Adolescent Relationships 40 Appendix B Summary of Autonomy and Relatedness Coding Guidelines Specific Scale Definitions and Guidelines for Coding Promoting Autonomy A. States reasons clearly for disagreeing Assesses use of reasoning by scoring the clarity, specificity, and thoroughness of points supporting the speaker’s position. C. Confidence in stating thoughts and opinions Assesses global level of confidence in the disagreement. Scores reflect factors such as speaking out forcefully and confidently, showing no signs of backing down, and asserting one’s position. Undermining Autonomy D. Recanting position Assesses inhibition of arguments through statements which the speaker doesn’t really mean and which serve to placate the other person or calm the disagreement. Codes reflect both the degree to which the speaker is convinced by the other side of the argument and the level of ambiguity in the speaker’s agreement. E. Blurs Assesses the degree to which the speaker attempts to blur the boundary between the person and the argument. Blurs include enlisting an outside person as supporting the position, characterizing the other person, forcing the other person to take on a specific role, attacking the other person directly, asserting personal limits by pleading helplessness or invoking guilt, and using the self or the other person as an example. F. Pressures other to agree Assesses the degree to which the speaker attempts to pressure the other person to change his/her mind by making it uncomfortable for him/her to maintain the original position. Exhibiting Relatedness G. Queries Assesses the degree to which the speaker asks questions and makes statements that show a genuine interest in what the other person is saying. H. Validates/agrees/positively reacts to other person Assesses the degree to which the speaker reacts positively to the other person and validates him/her. Examples of this include directly agreeing with the other person, stating/implying that his/her statements are interesting and insightful, laughing at jokes, and complimenting. Depressed Mood in Early Adolescent Relationships 41 I. Engaged interaction Assesses the overall level of engagement with the other person exhibited by both verbal and non-verbal communication of interest. Undermining Relatedness J. Distracting/ignoring/cutting off Assesses the degree to which the speaker communicates a lack of interest in what the other person is saying. K. Hostile or devaluing statements Assesses the degree to which the speaker makes statements that attack the other person and are intended to hurt or annoy . Depressed Mood in Early Adolescent Relationships 42 Appendix C Inventory of Peer and Parent Attachment (Parent Version) Directions: This section asks about your feelings about your relationship with your mother figure. Please read each of the following statements and circle the number for the response that best describes how you feel about your mother figure. Example: She picks me up from school every day. Not Sometimes Sometimes Usually True True Sometimes Not True True True Not True Sometimes True 1. I like to get her point of view on things I’m concerned about. 2. She can tell when I’m upset about something. 1 Almost Always True Usually True Almost Always True 2 Sometimes True, Sometimes Not True 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5 3. When we discuss things, she cares about my point of view. 4. Talking over my problems with her makes me feel ashamed or foolish. 5. I wish I had a different parent. 1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5 6. She understands me. 1 2 3 4 5 7. She helps me to talk about my difficulties. 1 2 3 4 5 8. She accepts me as I am. 1 2 3 4 5 9.I feel the need to be in touch with her more often. 1 2 3 4 5 10. She doesn’t understand what I’m going through these days. 11. I feel alone or apart when I’m with her. 1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5 12. She listens to what I have to say. 1 2 3 4 5 13. I feel she is a good parent. 1 2 3 4 5 14. She is fairly easy to talk to. 1 2 3 4 5 15. When I am angry about something she tries to listen. 16. She helps me to understand myself better. 1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5 17. She cares about how I am. 1 2 3 4 5 Depressed Mood in Early Adolescent Relationships 43 18. I feel angry with her. 1 2 3 4 5 19. I can count on her when I need to get something off my chest. 20. I trust her. 1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5 21. She respects my feelings. 1 2 3 4 5 22. I get upset a lot more than she knows about. 1 2 3 4 5 23. It seems as if she is irritated with me for no reason. 24. I can tell her about my problems and troubles. 1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5 25. If she knows something is bothering me, she asks me about it. 1 2 3 4 5 Depressed Mood in Early Adolescent Relationships 44 Appendix D Inventory of Peer and Parent Attachment (Peer Version) Directions: These questions ask about your feelings about your friendships in general. Please read each of the following statements and circle your choice. Not True Usually Not True Sometimes True, Sometimes Not Usually True Almost Always True 1. I like to get my friends’ point of view on things I’m concerned about. 1 2 3 4 5 2. My friends can tell when I’m upset about something. 1 2 3 4 5 3. When we discuss things, my friends care about my point of view. 1 2 3 4 5 4. Talking over my problems, my friends make me feel ashamed or foolish. 1 2 3 4 5 5. I wish I had different friends. 1 2 3 4 5 6. My friends understand me. 1 2 3 4 5 7. My friends help me to talk about my difficulties. 1 2 3 4 5 8. My friends accept me as I am. 1 2 3 4 5 9. I feel the need to be in touch with my friends more often. 1 2 3 4 5 10. My friends don’t understand what I’m going through these days. 1 2 3 4 5 11. I feel alone or apart when I’m with my friends. 1 2 3 4 5 12. My friends listen to what I have to say. 1 2 3 4 5 13. I feel my friends are good friends. 1 2 3 4 5 14. My friends are fairly easy to talk to. 1 2 3 4 5 15. When I am angry about something, my friends try to listen. 1 2 3 4 5 16. My friends help me to understand myself better. 1 2 3 4 5 Depressed Mood in Early Adolescent Relationships 45 17. My friends care about how I am. 1 2 3 4 5 18. I feel angry with my friends. 1 2 3 4 5 19. I can count on my friends when I need to get something off my chest. 1 2 3 4 5 20. I trust my friends. 1 2 3 4 5 21. My friends respect my feelings. 1 2 3 4 5 22. I get upset a lot more than my friends know about. 1 2 3 4 5 23. It seems as if my friends are irritated with me for no reason. 1 2 3 4 5 24. I can tell my friends about my problems and troubles. 1 2 3 4 5 25. If my friends know something is bothering me, he/she asks me about it. 1 2 3 4 5 Depressed Mood in Early Adolescent Relationships 46 Appendix E Friendship Quality Questionnaire Scales Companionship and Recreation: Directions: For each item, decide how true the statement is for your friendship with your friend here with you today. Circle your choice. Not At All True A Little True Somewhat True Pretty True Really True 1. We always spend free time at school together. 1 2 3 4 5 6. We always pick each other as partners for things. 1 2 3 4 5 18. We do fun things together a lot. 1 2 3 4 5 21. We go to each others’ houses. 1 2 3 4 5 22. We always play together or hang out together. 1 2 3 4 5 Conflict Resolution: Directions: For each item, decide how true the statement is for your friendship with your friend here with you today. Circle your choice. Not At All True A Little True Somewhat True Pretty True Really True 10. We talk about how to get over being mad at each other. 1 2 3 4 5 25. We make up easily when we have a fight. 1 2 3 4 5 34. We get over our arguments really quickly. 1 2 3 4 5 Depressed Mood in Early Adolescent Relationships 47 Appendix F Harter Self-Perception Profile Scales Social Acceptance: Directions: For each question, read the two statements. Decide which statement you think is most like you. Then decide if the statement is really true for you or sort of true for you, and put an X on the line for that choice. You should only mark one X for each numbered item. If you have any questions, please ask the interviewer. Really True for Me Sort of True For Me Sort of True for Me Really true for Me 3. _____ _____ Some teens find it hard to make friends BUT for other teens it’s pretty easy. _____ ______ 16. ____ _____ Some teens have a lot of friends BUT Other teens don’t have very many friends. _____ _______ 41. ____ _____ Some teens are popular with other kids their age BUT Other teens are not very popular with kids their age. _____ _______ 51. ____ _____ Some teens feel that they are accepted by other kids their age BUT Other teens wish that more kids their age accepted them. _____ _______ Close Friendship Competence: Directions: For each question, read the two statements. Decide which statement you think is most like you. Then decide if the statement is really true for you or sort of true for you, and put an X on the line for that choice. You should only mark one X for each numbered item. If you have any questions, please ask the interviewer. Really True for Me Sort of True For Me Sort of True for Me Really true for Me 9.___ ____ Some teens are able to make close friends BUT Other teens find it hard to make close friends. _____ _______ 22.____ ____ Some teens do have a close friend they share secrets with BUT Other teens don’t have a close friend they can share secrets with. _____ _______ 34. ____ _____ Some teens wish they had a really close friend to share things with BUT Other teens do have a really close friend to share things with. _____ _______ 53. ____ _____ Some teens don’t have a friend that is close enough to share really personal thoughts with Other teens do have a close friend that they can share personal thoughts and feelings with. _____ ______ BUT Depressed Mood in Early Adolescent Relationships 48 Appendix G Pupil Evaluation Inventory Directions: Below is a list of items that describe kids. For each item that describes your friend here with you today, now or within the past 6 months. Please answer all of the items as well as you can, even if some do not seem to apply to your friend. Circle your choice. Not True Somewhat or Sometimes True Very Often or Often True 1. She is too shy to make friends easily. 0 1 2 2. Her feelings are too easily hurt. 0 1 2 3. She never seems to be having a good time. 0 1 2 4. She is upset when called on to answer questions in class. 0 1 2 5. She is usually chosen last in group activities. 0 1 2 6. She has very few friends. 0 1 2 7. She is unhappy or sad. 0 1 2 8. She often doesn’t want to hang out (or do things with other kids). 0 1 2 9. She isn’t noticed much. 0 1 2 Depressed Mood in Early Adolescent Relationships 49 Appendix H Correlations of Demographic Variables to Variables of Interest Variable Depressed mood Wave 1 Wave 2 Autonomy and relatedness Teen’s negative A/R Teen’s positive A/R Quality of relationship with mother Wave 1 Wave 2 Friendship quality at wave 1 (teen report) Companionship and recreation Conflict resolution Close friendship Social acceptance Attachment to peers Friendship quality at wave 2 (teen report) Companionship and recreation Conflict resolution Close friendship Social acceptance Attachment to peers Friendship quality at wave 1 (peer report) Withdrawal Companionship and recreation Conflict Resolution Friendship quality at wave 2 (peer report) Companionship and recreation Conflict resolution *** p<.001 **p<.01 *p<.05 +p<.10 Gender r -.01 0 Minority Status r -.05 -.05 Total Income r -.10 -.03 .20* -.04 .15+ .34*** .03 .36*** .03 -.13 .03 0 .04 .03 -.05 .10 .03 -.02 .19** .19* .14+ .14+ 0 .15* .09 .28*** .05 .11 .21** .25** .22** .11 -.09 .28*** .22** .16* -.16* -.04 .12 .01 .10 -.10 0 .03 .08 -.01 .16* -.02 .11 .06 -.09 .10 .14+ .05 .15+ .12 .10 .02 .02 Depressed Mood in Early Adolescent Relationships 50 Table 1 Means and Standard Deviations Time 1 Time 2 Variable M SD M SD 6.64 5.53 5.61 5.25 Negative autonomy and relatedness 0.60 0.47 -- -- Positive autonomy and relatedness 1.80 0.62 -- -- 77.71 11.31 73.88 10.84 102.51 13.87 103.28 13.01 Companionship and recreation 19.76 3.87 20.16 4.14 Conflict resolution 11.36 2.87 11.42 2.82 Close friendship competence 14.05 2.35 13.93 2.72 Social acceptance 13.07 2.90 13.42 2.60 Companionship and recreation 19.55 4.49 19.56 4.38 Conflict resolution 11.46 2.73 11.26 2.80 0.23 0.27 -- Depressed mood Relationship with mother Quality of relationship Relationship with peers (teen report) Total attachment to peers Relationship with peers (peer report) Withdrawal Note: Dashes indicate data were not available at wave 2. -- Depressed Mood in Early Adolescent Relationships 51 Table 2. Zero Order Correlations of Variables Used 1 2 3 Depressed Mood 1. Wave 1 2. Wave 2 .54*** Autonomy & Relatedness 3. Teen Neg. A/R .09 .30** 4. Teen Pos. A/R -.14 .05 0 Relationship with Mother 5. Wave 1 -.47*** -.35*** -.17+ 6. Wave 2 -.31*** -.38*** -.22* Wave 1 Friendship (Tn Rpt) 7. Attachment to -.40*** -.23** 0 Peers 8. Conflict -.19* -.23** -.07 Resolution 9. Companionship -.23** -.12 -.11 10. Close -.25*** -.24** -.07 Friendship 11. Social -.34*** -.29*** -.16 Acceptance Wave 2 Friendship (Tn Rpt) 12. Attachment to -.37*** -.28*** -.04 Peers 13. Conflict -.19* -.17* .01 Resolution 14. Companionship -.23** -.27*** -.08 15. Close -.27*** -.26*** -.02 Friendship 16. Social -.37*** -.43*** -.11 Acceptance Wave 1 Friendship (Peer Rpt) 17. Withdrawal .10 .19* .25** 18. Conflict .06 .10 .07 Resolution 19. Companionship .02 -.02 -.12 Wave 2 Friendship (Peer Rpt) 20. Conflict -.10 .001 .01 Resolution 21. Companionship -.23** -.07 -.10 *** p < .001 ** p < .01 * p < .05 + p < .10 4 .11 -.14 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 .60*** .24* .37*** .15+ .32*** .27*** .19* .45*** .22* .23* .14+ .12 .16+ -.01 .41*** .32*** .51*** .17+ .26** .04 .12 .07 .22** .03 .34*** .38*** .14 .32*** .33*** .55*** .28*** .29*** .25** .23** .16+ .15+ .09 .41*** .38*** .39*** .21** .08 .54*** .25** .02 .17* .08 .06 .21* .35*** .27*** .32*** .19* .54*** .19* .17* .33*** .29*** .18* .44*** .48*** .54*** .34*** .36*** -.02 .17* .31*** .21** .18* .25** .31*** .59*** .43*** .24** .27*** .01 -.02 .01 -.16* .03 -.16+ 0 -.05 -.06 .16* .25** .02 -.04 -.03 .03 .09 .13 .08 .13 -.12 .10 -.11 .20** -.19* .16* -.10 .06 -.13+ -.08 -.09 .09 -.09 .21** .55*** .06 .09 -.03 .12 0 .02 .06 .09 .11 .14+ .36*** .06 .28*** .23** -.15+ .05 .45*** -.01 .05 -.18* .01 -.14+ .29*** .05 .08 -.16* .11 .20* .17* .06 .20* .30*** .62*** .26** .20* -.01 .40*** .11 .10 -.03 .27** .46*** Depressed Mood in Early Adolescent Relationships 52 Table 3. Predicting Depressed Mood at Wave 2 from Negative Autonomy and Relatedness Hierarchical Regression Results R2 R2 1. Depressed mood at wave 1 .42*** .18 2. Negative autonomy and relatedness .26** .25 .07 3. Depressed mood X negative autonomy/relatedness .17* .28 .03 Note: ’s are from entry into the model. N = 117. ***p < .001 **p < .01 *p < .05 Depressed Mood in Early Adolescent Relationships 53 Table 4. Predicting Depressed Mood at Wave 2 from Peer Report of Withdrawal Hierarchical Regression Results R2 1. Depressed mood at wave 1 .54*** .30 2. Peer report of withdrawal .14* .32 Note: ’s are from entry into the model. N = 172. ***p < .001 **p < .01 *p < .05 R2 .02 Depressed Mood in Early Adolescent Relationships 54 Table 5. Predicting Depressed Mood at Wave 2 from Conflict Resolution Hierarchical Regression Results 1. Depressed mood at wave 1 2. Conflict resolution with close friend Note: ’s are from entry into the model. N = 173. ***p < .001 **p < .01 *p < .05 R2 .55*** .30 -.13* .32 R2 .02 Depressed Mood in Early Adolescent Relationships 55 Table 6. Predicting Peer Report of Companionship and Recreation at Wave 2 Hierarchical Regression Results 1. Companionship and recreation at wave 1 2. Depressed mood at wave 1 Note: ’s are from entry into the model. N = 136. ***p < .001 **p < .01 *p < .05 R2 .26** .07 -.24** .13 R2 .06 Depressed Mood in Early Adolescent Relationships 56 Table 7. Predicting Teen Report of Close Friendship Competence at Wave 2 Hierarchical Regression Results R2 1. Close Friendship Competence at Wave 1 .33*** .11 2. Minority Status .26 .13 .02 -.19* .16 .03 3. CDI Score at Wave 1 Note: ’s are from entry into the model. N = 154. ***p < .001 **p < .01 *p < .05 R2 Depressed Mood in Early Adolescent Relationships 57 Table 8. Predicting Teen Report of Social Acceptance at Wave 2 Hierarchical Regression Results 1. Social acceptance at wave 1 2. Depressed mood at wave 1 3. Social acceptance X depressed mood Note: ’s are from entry into the model. N = 154. ***p < .001 **p < .01 *p < .05 R2 .57*** .34 R2 -.20** .38 .04 .15* .40 .02 Depressed Mood in Early Adolescent Relationships 58 Figure 1. Interaction between depressed mood at wave 1 and level of undermining autonomy and relatedness with the mother in predicting depressed mood at wave 2. Wave 2 Depressed Mood 1 High Wave 1 Depressed Mood 0.8 0.6 0.4 0.2 0 -0.2 Low Wave 1 Depressed Mood -0.4 -0.6 Low High Undermining Autonomy and Relatedness Depressed Mood in Early Adolescent Relationships 59 Figure 2. Interaction between perceived social acceptance at wave 1 and overall depressed mood at wave 1 in predicting perceived social acceptance at wave 2. Social Acceptance Wave 2 0.6 0.4 High Social Acceptance Wave 1 0.2 0 -0.2 -0.4 -0.6 Low Social Acceptance Wave 1 -0.8 -1 Low High Depressed Mood Wave 2