6. Historical Linguistics I) The nature and causes of language change: Language change is both obvious and rather mysterious. Historical linguistics is concerned with both the description & explanation of language change. All languages undergo changes over time. English has undergone continuous & dramatic change throughout its 3 major periods: Old English (roughly from 450 to 1100), Middle English (1100-1500), and Modern English (from 1500 to the present). All components of the grammar from meaning (semantics) to individual sounds (phonology) are subject to change. A striking fact about language change in general is its regularity and systematicity. (E.g.: the development of a fixed SVO basic word order in Eng. didn’t affect just a few verbs; all Vs in ModE appear before rather than after the O d.) The inevitability of language change is guaranteed by the way in which lg. is handed down from one generation to the next. Children don’t begin with an intact grammar of he lg. being acquired but rather must construct a grammar on the basis of the available data. In such a situation it’s hardly surprising that differences will arise, even if only subtle ones, from 1 generation to the next. Principal cause of language change: articulatory simplification spelling pronunciation, analogy and reanalysis, language contact. II) Sound change: Sound changes have a physiological basis. Since such changes typically result in articulatory simplification, they have traditionally been related to the idea of ‘ease of articulation’. 1.) Articulatory simplification occurs in people’s everyday life: deletion of a consonant, insertion of a vowel to break up a complex cluster (e.g.: [fifθs][fifs]; [æθlijt][æθəlijt]). 2.) A minor, but important source of sound change in Eng. + other lg.-es is spelling pronunciation. Since the written form of a word can differ significantly from the way it is pronounced, a new pronunciation can arise which seems to reflect more closely the spelling of the word. E.g.: “often” was pronounced with a [t] in earlier Eng., it was subsequently lost: [ɔfən]. Since spelling tends to remain stable even though sound changes have occurred, spelling pronunciation can reintroduce a pronunciation that was earlier altered through spelling pron. 3.) Cognitive factors also play a role in change. 2 sources of such change are: analogy + re-analysis. Analogy reflects the preference of speakers for regular patterns over irregular ones. Both phonological & semantic characteristics can serve as basis for analogy (e.g.: on the basis of similarity with such Vs as sting/stung, swing/swung, in some dialects “bring” has developed a form “brung”). Reanalysis is particularly common in morphological change. Morphological reanalysis often involves an attempt to attribute a compound or root + affix structure to a word that formerly wasn’t broken into component morphemes (e.g.: hamburgeroriginally: type of meat patty deriving its name from the city of Hamburgreanalysed as: ham + burger “burger” has since appeared in several new forms, e.g.: fishburger, chickenburger, etc.). The resulting reanalysis need not necessarily be correct. 4.) Language contact: (=borrowing) all components of the grammar, esp. the lexicon, is affected. (Eng. has borrowed many Am. Words, e.g.: Canada, moccasin, totem, tomahawk, moose, etc.) Effects of borrowing: introduction of new phonemes or allophones. If there are a number of borrowings from another lg., the borrowed foreign segment can eventually become a phoneme. (MidE London dialect had [f] but not [v] in word initial position, it was later introduced file—vile). Language contact also results in hypercorrection, which occurs when a speaker who is attempting to speak another dialect or lg. overgeneralizes particular rules. (E.g.: Am. dialect no distinction btw. [t] & [d] latter—ladder both pronounced with an intervocalic flap [D].) III) Phonetic vs. phonological change: Several types of sound change can be distinguished. Most of them begin as subtle alterations in the sound pattern of a lg. in particular environments. The ling’c processes underlying such phonetically conditioned sound change are identical to the ones found in the phonology of currently spoken languages. The application of such processes usually brings about an articulatory simplification. All aspects of phonology (tone, stress, syllable structure, etc.) are subject to change over time. Most sound changes involve sequences of segments, this is called sequential change. There’s also a type of change called segmental change. The most common type of phonetically conditioned change is assimilation, which has the effect of increasing efficiency of articulation through a simplification of articulatory movements. Partial assimilation, involving place or manner of articulation is a very common change, which can result in total assimilation. Types of assimilation: voicing ~ (slǣpdeslǣpte = ‘slept); ~ of nasality (stefnstemn = ‘stem (of a tree)); palatalization, which is often the 1st step in affrication (e.g.: Lat.: centum [k] Old French: cent [ts], It.: ciento [ʧ]. Nasalization refers to the nasalising effect that a nasal consonant can have on an adjacent vowel. Umlaut: the effect a vowel or sometimes a glide in one syllable can have on the vowel of another syllable, usually a preceding one. Umlaut played an important role in OE & is the source of irregular plurals such as goose/geese, mouse/mice in ModE. Dissimilation: the process whereby 1 segment is made less like another segment in its environment. This occurs when it would be difficult to articulate or perceive 2 similar sounds in close proximity (e.g.: Lat.: anma ‘soul’ Spanish alma) ~ can also operate at a distance to affect non-adjacent segments. Epenthesis: sound change that involves the insertion of a consonant or vowel into a particular environment (e.g.: OE earlier form: ganra—later form: gandra “gander”). The epenthetic segment serves as a bridge for the transition btw. the segments on either side. In other cases, the vowel e serves to break up a sequence of sounds which would otherwise be difficult to pronounce or even inconsistent with the phonotactic patterns of the lg. (e.g.: ath[ə]lete; Lat.: schola [sk] Sp.: escuela [esk] => prothesis). 1 Metathesis: A change in the relative positioning of segments. This also can affect adjacent segments or segments at a distance (e.g.: OE: earlier form: wæpslater form: wæsp “wasp”). Both vowels and consonants are also susceptible to outright deletion as well as to various weakening processes. Vowel deletion may involve a word-final vowel (apocope) or a word-internal vowel (syncope). A vowel in an unstressed syllable is particularly susceptible to deletion, esp. when a nearby neighbouring syllable is stressed. The effects of syncope are apparent in the loss of the medial vowel in ModE words such as vegetable, interest, family, which are frequently pronounced as [vɛ́ʤtəbl], [intrɛst], [fǽmlij]. Vowel deletion is commonly preceded by vowel reduction, in which a full vowel is reduced to a short central vowel [ə]. Vowel reduction typically affects short vowels in unstressed syllables. (E.g.: OE: stanas [a] MidE: stones [ə] {=> vowel reduction}Early ModE: stones ø {=> syncope}; OE: nama [a] ME: name [ə] {=> vowel reduction}EModE: name ø {=> apocope}.) Consonant deletion is also a very common sound change. F.e., the word-initial cluster [kn] was found in O + MidE as the spelling of such words as knight, knit, and knee, but k was subsequently lost. Consonant weakening, just like vowel red., can also be identified. Geminates weaken to non-geminates (degemination), stops weaken to fricatives (frication), voiceless stops or voiceless fricatives weaken to voiced stops or voiced fricatives respectively (voicing). Weakening can ultimately result in the deletion of the consonant. (Degen.: Lat.: mittere Sp.: meter; Voicing: Lat.: mātūrusOldSp.: maduro; Fricaion: OSp.: maduroSp.: maduro [ð]; Deletion: OFr.: [mađyr]Fr.: mûr). Rhotacism is a relatively common type of weakening that typically involves the change of [z] to [r]. Often, ~ is preceded by a stage involving the voicing of [s] to [z]. (E.g.: ModE: rhotacism is the source of alteration btw. [z] & [r] in was & were.) Consonantal strengthening: just as Cs weaken, they can also strengthen. Glide strengthening: strengthening of a glide to an affricate particularly common in wordinitial position. (E.g.: Lat.: iuvenis [j]->It.: giovane [ʤ] ‘young’.) Auditory factors also play a role in sound change. Substitution: a type of audition-based change involving the replacement of one segment with another similar sounding segment (e.g.: MidE: laugh [x]ModE: laugh [f]). Phonetic sound change: 1st stage of a sound change results in the creation of a new allophone of an already existing phoneme. E.g.: the laxing of short high vowels that has developed in Canadian Fr. Sometimes sound changes can lead to changes in a lg.’s phonological system by adding, eliminating, or rearranging phonemes. Such phonological change can involve splits, mergers, or shifts. In a phonological split, allophones of the same phoneme come to contrast with each other due to the loss of the conditioning environment, with the result that 1 or more phonemes are created. Eng. phoneme [η]: result of a phonological split: the original phoneme split into 2 phonemes: /n/ (i) /n/, (ii) /η/. Mergers: 2 phonemes collapse into a single one, thereby reducing the number of phonemes in the lg. (Cockney Eng.: /θ//f/ => /θ/ & /f/ merged into one /f/ thin + fin pronounced as /fin/. /θ/ + /f/ /f/; /ð/ + /v/ /v/.) Shifts: a change in which a series of phonemes is systematically modified so that their organization with respect to each other is altered. E.g.: Great Eng. Vowel Shift. Lg.-es with 7 or more vowels often undergo diphthongisation. This can be the seen as a reaction to the overcrowding of the phonological space. Its effect is to reduce the 7 vowel system to a 5 vowel system. IV) Morphological change: Borrowing has been a very important source of affixes in English (e.g.: -ment from Fr., -able). However, not all new affixes are the result of borrowing. Words can develop into affixes in a process called fusion. If 2 words are frequently adjacent, over time they can become fused together to form a single unit consisting of a stem & an affix. Fusion can result in either prefixes or suffixes. A number of ModE suffixes are derived from earlier words by means of fusion(e.g.: -hoodOE word: hād ‘state, condition, rank’; -domOE word: dōm ‘condition, power’, etc). Just as affixes can be added to the grammar, they can also be lost. Sometimes affixes simply fall into disuse for no apparent reason. F.e., a number of OE derivational affixes, including –bǣre & -bora, are no longer used (e.g.: lustbǣre ‘pleasant, agreeable’; mundbora ‘protector’). It’s also very common for affixes to be lost through sound change (e.g.: OE case & gender markings got lost). By the 15th century, Eng. case endings had changed radically. Consonant deletion resulted in the loss of the earlier [m] of the dative plural suffix & through vowel reduction all the unstressed vowels of the case endings were reduced to the central vowel [ə], which was later lost through vowel deletion. Consequently, many of the earlier case and gender distinctions were obliterated. (E.g.: ModE gen. sing. hund’s, plural: hunds, gen.: hunds’.) OE has 5 distinct affixes for cases; MidE only 2 (-e, -es); ModE: -s for possessive & plural. Since lg.-es vary in the complexity of their morphology, linguists often make a distinction btw analytic and synthetic lg.-es. Analytic lg.-es have very few affixes (like ModE), synthetic lg.-es have many (e.g. Lat., OE). Sound change and fusion ensure that there is an endless transition in the morphology of a lg over time. Fusion also ensures the rise of new synthetic forms; through fusion a lg. with analytic morphology can become much more synthetic over time. (E.g.: ModE coulda represents the fusion of could + have.) The drastic effect that sound change can have on the morphology of a lg. are often alleviated through analogy. E.g. OE hand plural was handa vowel reduction + apocope applying to handa would have yielded a ModE plral form identical with the singular form, namely hand; obviously, then the ModE plural form hands cannot be the direct consequence of sound change. Rather, it’s a result of earlier analogy with words such as MidE hund ‘hound’ which did form the plural with the suffix –a other plural forms besides hands that were created on the basis of analogy including eyes (eyen in MidE) & shoes (shoen). Reanalysis can also result in a new morphological structure for a word. BUT the reanalysis exemplified by hamburger is rather called folk etymology, ‘cos it commonly involves changes in pronunciation reflecting the new morphological analysis & isn’t 2 based on correct analysis of the word by the speaker. Examples of folk etymology: belfry from belfrey; bridegroom from bridegome; muskat from musquash. Affixes can also be affected by reanalysis; e.g.: -ly (ModE adverbial suffix) developing from OE –lic(e). (N+-lic = Adj; Adjlic + -e = Adv) it was reanalysed as an adverbial suffix, and ten used by analogy to derive Adv.-s from Adj.-s in forms where it was not used earlier (e.g.: deeply, etc). V) Syntactic change: VI) Lexical and semantic change: An obvious type of lg change involves modifications to the lexicon. 2 types: addition & loss, which often reflect cultural changes that include novel objects & notions & eliminate outdated ones. Addition is often the result of technological innovations or contact with other cultures. Such developments result in lexical gaps, which can be filled by adding new words to the lexicon either by borrowing or by word formation processes, e.g.: - Compounding & derivation (sunbeam, childish) (BUT conversion was not available in OE) Depending on the cultural relationship holding btw. lg.-es, 3 types of influence of 1 lg. on the other are traditionally identified: substratum, adstratum and superstratum. Substratum: influence or effect of a politically or culturally nondominant lg. on a dominant one. (E.g. Amerindian on N-Am. & Canadian Fr.). In the history of Eng., the influence of Celtic substratum is also evident. (E.g.: place names: Thames, London, Dover). Borrowed words are often restricted to place names & unfamiliar items or concepts. Superstratum: effect of a politically or culturally dominant lg. on another lg. E.g.: the Athapaskan lg. Gwich has borrowed a number of governmental terms from Eng. In the case of Eng., Norman Fr had a superstratum influence (after Norman Conquest, 1066) Fr. terms for political, judicial & cultural notions (+ science, warfare, religion). (E.g.: tax, revenue, government, prayer, sermon, fashion, army, navy, art). Adstratum: situation where 2 lg.-es are in contact & neither is (clearly) dominant. E.g.: Scandinavian influence on Eng. => ~ usually results in the borrowing of everyday words (Sc. words e.g.: anger, cake, egg, root, score, etc). Just as words can be added to the lexicon, they can be lost as a result of changes in society esp. where the object or notion a word refers to becomes obsolete (E.g.: OE flÿtme – ‘a blood letting instrument). Words rarely jump from one meaning to an unrelated one. The change are step by step and involve either semantic broadening, ~ narrowing, amelioration, pejoration, weakening or semantic shift & metaphor. Semantic broadening is the process in which the meaning of a word becomes more general or more inclusive than its historically earlier form (e.g.: aunt old meaning: ‘father’s sister’, new: ‘father’s or mother’s sister’). Semantic narrowing is the process in which the meaning of a word becomes less general or less inclusive than its historically earlier meaning (e.g.: hound old m.: ‘any dog’, new: ‘a hunting breed’). Amelioration: In ~, the meaning of a word becomes more positive of favourable. The opposite change, pejoration, also occurs (e.g.: wench = ‘girl’, new: ‘wanton woman, prostitute’) Weakening of meaning also frequently occurs (e.g.: soon old m.: ‘immediately’, new: ‘in the near future’). Semantic shift is a process in which a word loses its former meaning, taking on a new, but often related meaning (e.g.: bead old m.: ‘prayer’, new: ‘prayer bead, bead’). One of the most striking types of semantic meaning is triggered by metaphor, a figure of speech based on a perceived similarity btw. distinct objects or actions. Metaphorical change usually involves a word with a concrete meaning, taking on a more abstract sense, although the word’s original meaning is not lost. (E.g.: grasp ‘understand’; high ‘on drugs’; dull ‘stupid’.) 3