Journal of Counseling Psychology April 2001 Vol. 48, No. 2, 223-232 © 2001 by the American Psychological Association For personal use only--not for distribution. Family Caregiver Social Problem-Solving Abilities and Adjustment During the Initial Year of the Caregiving Role Timothy R. Elliott Department of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, University of Alabama at Birmingham Richard M. Shewchuk Department of Health Services Administration, University of Alabama at Birmingham J. Scott Richards Department of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, University of Alabama at Birmingham ABSTRACT The authors examined the relation of social problem-solving abilities to trajectories of adjustment of family caregivers in the initial year of their caregiving role. Persons who recently assumed the caregiver role for a family member with a recent-onset spinal cord injury completed measures of problem solving, depression, anxiety, and health during the inpatient rehabilitation program and at 3 other times throughout the year. Hierarchical linear modeling showed that negative problem orientation explained significant variation in the rates of change in caregiver depressive behavior, anxiety, and health complaints. Caregivers with a greater negative orientation were at risk to develop psychological and health problems at a significantly higher rate over the year. Implications for psychological interventions and health policy concerning the needs of family caregivers and their care recipients are discussed. Once regarded in a patronizing fashion as “informal” extensions of health care programs (Kane & Kane, 1987), family caregivers are now responsible for a wide variety of services and activities that were formerly and formally provided by some traditional health service professions. As the number of chronic conditions escalate in our society and health care programs continue to limit services to persons with these conditions, more family members will assume caregiver roles (Hoffman, Rice, & Sung, 1996). Dramatic cutbacks in formal health care services have intensified the role of the family caregiver. Likewise, the emergence of managed care as the predominant health care paradigm has also resulted in many negative effects for families with a member who has a chronic disease or disability (Council on Scientific Affairs, 1993). With these changes in health care allocation—and with increases in the incidence of chronic disease and disability—more individuals will be compelled to assume the role of primary caregiver for a family member who incurs a severe physical disability. Family members assume caregiving roles under different circumstances. In some situations, caregivers may gradually evolve into their role in the face of chronic conditions that slowly erode the capacities of a family member or loved one (e.g., Alzheimer's dementia, AIDS). In other cases, caregivers may be involved with care recipients who may have considerable life expectancies (e.g., developmental disabilities, multiple sclerosis). Sometimes caregivers are thrust into their role as nonnormative, “off-time” life events (Neugarten, 1979)—such as a sudden-onset disability (e.g., spinal cord injury, head trauma)—compete with and preclude the pursuit of more normative personal, vocational, and social roles (Moen, Robison, & Dempster-McClain, 1995). With the increasing incidence of chronic health problems in American society and corresponding increases in the number of family caregivers, greater emphasis is being placed on the needs of caregivers across the life span (Schulz & Quittner, 1998). Essentially, individuals who begin caregiving assume a role defined by the tasks in which they engage and the relationship they have with the care recipient (Aneshensel, Pearlin, Mullan, Zarit, & Whitlatch, 1995). Empirical research indicates that family caregivers are at an increased risk for problems with distress and illness (for reviews, see Schulz et al., 1997; Vitaliano, 1997). Caregivers of persons with physical disability have higher levels of distress than noncaregivers (e.g., Weitzenkamp, Gerhart, Charlifue, Whiteneck, & Savic, 1997). More elaborate research designs reveal that over time the health of the caregiver may be compromised by disruptions in cardiovascular and immune functioning (see Vitaliano, 1997). These psychological and physical problems may be exacerbated as caregivers neglect their own health and care (Burton, Newsom, Schulz, Hirsch, & German, 1997). Psychological characteristics and related resources have been found to have a greater impact on caregiver adjustment than objectively defined aspects of the caregiver role or care recipient condition (Schulz & Quittner, 1998). Caregivers' distress may be due in part to their subjective appraisal of caregiving as onerous and personally distressing (Chwalisz, 1996; Haley et al., 1996). Further, some caregivers appear to be predisposed to experiencing higher levels of emotional strain regardless of how caregiving duties are objectively defined (Hooker, Monahan, Shifren, & Hutchinson, 1992). Caregiving is often marked by a lack of available time for personal pursuits (Quittner, Opipari, Regoli, Jacobsen, & Eigen, 1992) and a slow erosion of social support with corresponding social isolation (Quittner, Glueckauf, & Jackson, 1990). Caregiver coping strategies may be a better predictor of caregiver adjustment than social support (Chwalisz, 1996). In our ongoing, prospective study of caregiver adjustment and adaptation, we have learned that the first year of caregiving is a dynamic process, characterized by fluctuations in social support that can have pronounced effects on caregiver distress and health (Shewchuk, Richards, & Elliott, 1998). However, simply modeling or describing the caregiver distress process is insufficient. We must identify caregivers' characteristics that predispose them and their care recipients to less than optimal outcomes, so that we can develop strategic interventions for these persons. Furthermore, we must consider theoretical models that delineate clear directives for effective, low-cost interventions for family caregivers and their care recipients who often have limited access, resources, and interest in formal psychological services (Shewchuk & Elliott, 2000). Several theorists believe that persons who provide care to those with chronic disease and disability could benefit from cognitive–behavioral interventions that equip them with the skills necessary for emotional regulation and task management (Elliott & Shewchuk, 2000; Grant, 1999; Houts, Nezu, Nezu, & Bucher, 1996). Contemporary models of social problem-solving abilities stipulate that individuals differ in the cognitive–behavioral skills that influence the processing of information about their problems, in their ability to regulate emotional experiences when problem solving, and in their ability to implement effective strategies for resolving problems (D'Zurilla & Nezu, 1999). Social problem-solving abilities have been conceptualized as two components, problem-orientation and problem-solving skills (D'Zurilla & Nezu, 1999). The problem-orientation component encompasses beliefs and attitudes a person has about their overall abilities, their level of confidence in problem solving, and their abilities in regulating their emotions so that the individual is motivated to handle minor problems efficiently and work diligently on more time-consuming problems. The problem-solving skills component entails both effective (e.g., rational, logical) and ineffective (e.g., impulsive, careless, avoidant) tendencies. This model is particularly appealing to clinical researchers as evidence indicates that training in social problemsolving abilities has been effective in treating depression and distress, and in improving self-management skills (Arean et al., 1993; Nezu & Perri, 1989; Richards & Perri, 1978). Programmatic research has demonstrated that the problem-orientation component is a consistent predictor of emotional states and distress under routine and stressful conditions (Elliott, Sherwin, Harkins, & Marmarosh, 1995) and among persons with physical disability and other health conditions (Elliott, Godshall, Herrick, Witty, & Spruell, 1991; Elliott, Shewchuk, Richeson, Pickelman, & Franklin, 1996). The problem-orientation component has also been related to health complaints (Elliott & Marmarosh, 1994; Godshall & Elliott, 1997), acceptance of disability among persons with recent-onset spinal cord injuries (Elliott, 1999), and depressive behavior among undergraduates under duress (Priester & Clum, 1993). Cross-sectional studies have found the problem-orientation component predictive of distress experienced by mothers of children with physical disabilities (Noojin & Wallander, 1997) and of depression among family caregivers of persons recently discharged from a stroke rehabilitation program (Grant, Elliott, Giger, & Bartolucci, 2001). Although a negative problem orientation was associated with caregiver distress in these studies, the lack of prospective analyses hinders our ability to understand the relation of social problem solving to caregiver adjustment over time. The social problem-solving abilities of family caregivers may also affect the adjustment of care recipients. Caregiver impulsive and careless problem-solving styles are significantly associated with low acceptance of disability scores of care recipients being discharged from medical rehabilitation (Elliott, Shewchuk, & Richards, 1999). These tendencies also predicted the occurrence of a preventable skin ulcer a year later among persons who returned for an annual medical examination (with 88% accuracy). Unfortunately, cross-sectional designs reveal little about ongoing processes that characterize caregiver adjustment over time, and we know that caregiver adjustment in the initial year of this role can be particularly complex (Shewchuk et al., 1998). Matthews (1993) astutely observed that we ultimately “know comparatively little about the trajectory of caregiving” (p. 115). Statistical techniques such as growth curve analyses and hierarchical linear modeling (HLM) are ideally suited for studying trajectories of adaptation among families who have a member with a chronic health condition (Drotar, 1997; Frank et al., 1998; Shewchuk & Elliott, 2000). These techniques are well-suited for identifying specific characteristics of individuals who may be at risk for adjustment problems over time. In the present study, we prospectively examined the relationship between social problem-solving abilities and the adjustment of caregivers throughout the first year of the caregiving role. We measured three related but distinct aspects of caregiver adjustment throughout the year (depressive behavior, anxiety, and physical health). We expected the problem-orientation component to be significantly associated with caregiver anxiety, depressive behavior, and health complaints throughout the course of the initial year. We used data-analytic techniques that would permit examination of different developmental trajectories on each of these three dimensions of adjustment. Method Participants Participants included 11 men (5 Black, 6 Caucasian; M age = 42.91 years, SD = 15.74 years, range = 26–73 years; M years of education = 11.77, SD = 3.16, range = 6–16 years of formal education) and 55 women (21 Black, 34 Caucasian; M age = 41.76 years, SD = 13.09 years, range = 18–74 years; M years of education = 12.32, SD = 2.47, range = 8–22 years of formal education). The preponderance of women identified as caregivers in this sample is consonant with previous research. Twenty of the caregivers were mothers of the care recipient, 12 were wives, 7 were daughters, and 4 were sisters. Four husbands, 4 fathers, and 1 brother were also in caregiver roles. Five caregivers were categorized as having other kinds of preinjury relationships (e.g., aunts, close family friends, former in-laws). Forty-four caregivers were married at the time they assumed their role, 15 had been previously divorced, and 7 had never married. Thirty-two caregivers were not employed outside the home prior to the injury incurred by the care recipient, 27 were employed full time, and 6 worked part time. Following each consecutive admission to the inpatient rehabilitation program, a trained research assistant reviewed each case to determine potential eligibility for inclusion in the study. Individuals were eligible for the study if a family member would be involved in providing some level of care and assistance in ongoing caregiving activities to the person with spinal cord injury upon return to the community (e.g., assistance with activities of daily living, adherence to self-care regimens, and other supportive care). The research assistant then approached prospective family caregivers and informed them of the study. Informed consent was obtained from those who agreed to participate. The initial assessment was conducted during the hospitalization. Subsequent assessments were conducted by mail 1 month, 6 months, and 1 year postdischarge. Questionnaires were mailed in envelopes in no particular order, and participants completed the instruments in no particular sequence. The research assistant maintained telephone contact with participants to discuss receipt of materials and to address any questions or comments about the study. Although we did not have information about the number of individuals who were approached and declined to participate in this study over the course of this 5-year project, our present research indicates that the number of eligible caregivers who have refused to participate has been 16%. Moreover, of those who were approached, we have noted that approximately 20% were unable to participate because they did not meet our inclusionary criteria or were ineligible for a variety of other reasons (e.g., they were relocating to another state, there were multiple caregivers, or they had cognitive impairments). Measures Social Problem Solving Abilities—Revised (SPSI–R; D'Zurilla, Nezu, & Maydeu-Olivares, in press). The SPSI–R is a 52-item, self-report measure of social problem-solving abilities (Maydeu-Olivares & D'Zurilla, 1996). Each item is rated on a 5-point Likert-type scale ranging from 0 (not very true of me) to 4 (extremely true of me). Higher scores on each scale indicate a greater propensity in that facet of problem solving. The SPSI– R is based on a five dimensional model of problem solving and provides five scales. Two of the scales measure the problem-orientation dimensions: Positive Problem Orientation (PO) and Negative Problem Orientation (NO). The remaining three scales are considered problem-solving skills scales. These include Rational Problem Solving (RPS), Impulsivity and Carelessness Style (IC), and Avoidance Style (AV). The PO scale assesses a general cognitive set, which includes the tendency to view problems in a positive light, to see them as challenges rather than threats, and to be optimistic about one's ability to detect and implement effective solutions. Sample items from the PO scale include “Whenever I have a problem, I believe that it can be solved,” “When I have a problem, I try to see it as a challenge or opportunity to benefit in some positive way from having a problem.” The NO scale assesses a cognitive–emotional set indicative of a greater pessimism, a lack of motivation toward problem solving, and a proclivity for negative moods that hinders effective problem solving. Sample items on the NO scale include “I hate having to solve the problems that occur in my life,” “When I am trying to solve a problem I get so upset that I cannot think clearly,” and “When I am faced with a difficult problem, I doubt that I will be able to solve it on my own no matter how hard I try.” The RPS scale assesses the tendency to systematically and deliberately use effective problem-solving techniques by defining the problem, generating alternatives, evaluating alternatives, and implementing solutions and evaluating outcomes. Sample items on the RPS scale include “Before I try to solve a problem, I set a specific goal so that I know exactly what I want to accomplish” and “When I have a decision to make, I weigh the consequences of each option and compare them to each other.” The IC scale measures the tendency to solve problems in an impulsive, incomplete, and haphazard manner. The IC scale has items such as “When making decisions, I do not evaluate all my options carefully enough” and “When I am trying to solve a problem, I go with the first good idea that comes to mind.” The AV scale assesses dysfunctional patterns of problem solving characterized by putting the problem off and waiting for problems to solve themselves. Sample items on this scale include “I go out of my way to avoid having to deal with problems in my life” and “I spend more time avoiding my problems than solving them.” Internal consistency estimates for the scales with college students range from alphas of .76 for PO to .92 for RPS, and test–retest (3 weeks) reliability ranges from .72 for PO to .88 for NO for the same sample (D'Zurilla et al., in press). Significant correlations between the SPSI–R scales and similar constructs on the Problem Solving Inventory (Heppner, 1988) and with other theoretically related constructs as stress, somatic symptoms, anxiety, depression, hopelessness, and suicidality, provide evidence of construct validity (Chang & D'Zurilla, 1996; D'Zurilla et al., in press). The SPSI–R scales have been predictably associated with self-esteem, life satisfaction, extraversion, social adjustment, and social skills (D'Zurilla et al., in press; Sadowski, Moore, & Kelley, 1994). The SPSI–R was administered at the initial assessment. Depressive behavior. The Center for Epidemiological Studies Depression Scale (CES–D; Radloff, 1977) was used to obtain an index of depressive behavior at each assessment. The CES–D was developed to assess symptoms of depression in the general population (Radloff, 1977). This instrument contains 20 items that assess current levels of depressive behavior, with a particular emphasis on the impact of depressed mood (e.g., “I felt sad,” “I thought my life had been a failure,” “I had crying spells”). Items are scored on a 4-point scale ranging from 0 (rarely or none of the time) to 3 (most or all of the time) to indicate how often symptoms are experienced in the preceding week. Scores range from 0 to 60. Higher scores indicate higher levels of depressive behavior; scores greater than 16 have been found to differentiate depressed from nondepressed community-residing adults (Craig & Van Natta, 1978). CES–D scores have been associated across many studies with low levels of social support and social integration, low perceptions of control over one's life, poor health, and social discomfort (Elliott & Umlauf, 1995). The CES–D correlates well with other measures of depression (e.g., r = .81; Weissman, Prusoff, & Newberry, 1975), and evidence indicates that CES–D scores do not appear to be biased by somatic complaints (N = 1,060 persons aged 55 years and older; Foelker & Shewchuk, 1992). Alpha coefficients have ranged from .84 to .90 in several field studies (Radloff, 1977). Test– retest coefficients for this measure in the present study ranged from .73 to .89. Anxiety. The trait version of the State–Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI; Spielberger, Gorsuch, Lushene, Vagg, & Jacobs, 1983) was used to assess caregiver anxiety. The STAI contains 20 items that assess how a person generally feels. Internal consistency coefficients for the scale have ranged from .86 to .95; test–retest reliability coefficients have ranged from .65 to .75 over a 2-month interval (Spielberger et al., 1983). Test–retest coefficients for this measure in the present study ranged from .69 to .89. Considerable evidence attests to the construct and concurrent validity of the scale (Spielberger, 1989). Higher scores indicate greater anxiety. This measure was selected to assess any ongoing problems with anxiety over the four assessment periods, rather than to measure state-specific, and possibly transitory, periods of anxiety at each time. Health complaints. The general form of the Pennebaker Inventory of Limbic Languidness scale (PILL; Pennebaker, 1982) was used to assess caregiver health complaints. The PILL contains 54 symptoms (e.g., back pains, headaches, indigestion, severe itching) that are rated in a yes–no format. The PILL measures health complaints experienced by the individual over the preceding 3 weeks. A total score is acquired by summing the reported symptoms; higher scores reflect more health complaints. The PILL general form has adequate internal consistency (.88), and test–retest reliabilities over a 2-month period have ranged from .79 to .83 (Pennebaker, 1982). Test–retest coefficients for this measure in the present study ranged from .51 to .84. Evidence of convergent and predictive validity has been demonstrated in significant associations between higher PILL scores and frequent physician visits, increased aspirin use within the past month, more days of restricted activities because of illness, poor exercise habits, greater drug and caffeine use, disturbed sleep and eating patterns, and scores on other symptom inventories (Pennebaker, 1982). Statistical Analysis The principal research questions in this study were addressed by using the multilevel modeling features of LISREL 8 (Jöreskog, Sörbom, du Toit, & du Toit, 1999) to examine time-ordered measures of caregiver depression, anxiety, and health complaints reported by family caregivers over the first year of caregiving. The multilevel approach was first used to estimate the parameters of curves that reflected time-ordered changes in caregiver-specific adjustment (i.e., depression, anxiety, and health complaints), and then to examine the between-caregiver variation in the parameters defining the individual change curves. Finally, the multilevel modeling approach was used to determine the extent to which such variation could be predicted by hypothesized individual differences in social problem-solving abilities. Conceptually, the repeated observations of our longitudinal study provided a hierarchical or nested (i.e., a multilevel) data arrangement where the Level 1 data units consisted of the time-ordered measures of adjustment that are nested within Level 2 data units consisting of individual caregivers. In the Level 1 model, the repeated measures of caregiver adjustment were used to estimate caregiver-specific change trajectories as a linear function of time. These trajectories are defined by a unique set of caregiver-specific intercept and slope parameters that describe the intraindividual, or within-person, relationship between adjustment and time. The Level 2 model examined the between-caregiver variability in the parameters defining the performance curve for each participant. Conceptually, we hypothesized that the variation in the within-caregiver growth curves (i.e., the variability of Level 1 change parameter π1j) could be modeled at Level 2 as a function of fixed and random effects that included a set of social problem-solving abilities that were hypothesized to vary between caregivers. In other words, the coefficients describing the Level 1 trajectories become the outcome variables in the Level 2 model. In using this approach we assumed that observed levels of each adjustment measure reflected ongoing change processes that could be represented by continuous time-dependent curves at the individual-caregiver level. We also assumed that there would be sufficient variation in these curves that could be modeled as a function of systematic individual differences in measures that describe caregivers' problem-solving abilities. A principal benefit of the multilevel modeling approach used in this study is the ability to use information from both levels of the data structure to examine the relationships that occur both within and between caregivers. By linking both levels of a data structure, it is possible to identify systematic patterns of change and correlates of change that would be difficult to discern when examining mean change levels associated with aggregate level data. Another advantage of using an HLM, or multilevel modeling approach, as opposed to an approach that uses a repeated measures design in the analysis of variance (ANOVA) or multivariate analysis of variance tradition—especially with longitudinal data—is that it is possible to obtain statistically efficient solutions for data that include missing or nonsynchronous observations (Bryk & Raudenbush, 1987; Tate & Hokanson, 1993). The statistical and conceptual bases for HLM modeling of longitudinal data have been discussed in considerable detail (Bryk, Raudenbush, & Congdon, 1996; De Leeuw & Kreft, 1986; Francis, Fletcher, Stuebing, Davidson, & Thompson, 1991; Goldstein, 1986; Goldstein, Healy, & Rasbash, 1994; Willett, 1988).1 Results Table 1 displays the number of observations for each measure at each measurement occasion. A comprehensive analysis of the missing values did not reveal any pattern that would bias our analyses in any way. Moreover, the statistical algorithm used to estimate the parameters of our models outweighs the cases with more reliable data (Bryk & Raudenbush, 1992). Consequently, neither the missing values nor the pattern of missing values compromised the integrity of our analyses. Although we found no systematic biases in our pattern of missing values, we do not know why some participants did not return all materials at each assessment. Means and standard deviations for the self-report variables and their correlations are presented in Table 2. Unconditional Models As the first step in our analyses, we estimated a fully unconditional or random intercept model for each caregiver adjustment measure. The fully unconditional model is actually the functional equivalent of a one-way random effects ANOVA and, therefore, serves as a way to partition the total observed variability in caregiver adjustment into within-caregiver and between-caregiver components (Bryk & Raudenbush, 1992). By decomposing the overall variance in this manner, it is possible to obtain a baseline measure for assessing the relative explanatory contributions of models that include theory-relevant variables. The equations defining the random intercept model at both Level 1 and Level 2 include only intercepts and residual terms. At Level 1, each adjustment measure is modeled as a function of a caregiver intercept parameter (i.e., π0j) and a random error term (i.e., rij). Because this equation does not include covariates or predictors, each Level 1 intercept is an estimate of the average level of adjustment (i.e., average level of depression, anxiety, and health complaints) for a particular caregiver over the four measurement occasions. In the Level 2 model, the estimates of the Level 1 intercepts were estimated as a function of the grand mean of each adjustment measure (i.e., β00 = 19.61 for depression, β00 = 44.94 for anxiety, and β00 = 13.30 for physical health) for all caregivers over all observation occasions, and a random residual term (i.e., u0j). The variance of the Level 1 residuals for each adjustment measure (i.e., ς2 = 38.93 for depression, ς2 = 42.23 for anxiety, and ς2 = 30.30 for physical health) provides an estimate of the within-caregiver variation. The variance of the Level 2 residuals, denoted as τ00, represents the level of between-caregiver variation. The values of τ00 were 113.15, 152.65, and 91.66 for depression, anxiety, and health complaints, respectively. Random Coefficient Regression Model Before addressing our hypotheses concerning the problem-solving variables, we also estimated a simple linear growth model for each measure of caregiver adjustment. Because this model included only the measurement occasion variable (i.e., time) as a Level 1 covariate and was specified without any Level 2 predictors, it also can be considered an unconditional model (see Table 3). Generally, as the most basic linear growth model specification, this unconditional model estimates the Level 1 initial status parameter (intercepts) at Level 2 as a function of the average intercept over all participants, β00, and a residual, u0j. The estimates for β00 indicate that caregivers had average scores of 19.48 and 45.90 for the depression and anxiety measures, and an average of score of 11.74 for health complaints at the first measurement occasion. Similarly, the estimates for the Level 1 rate of change parameter (i.e., slope) for each adjustment measure were modeled at Level 2 in terms of the overall average slope across all caregivers, β10, and a residual, uij. The estimates obtained for the β10 parameters indicate that caregiver health complaints increased an average of 1.04 on each successive measurement occasion (see Table 3). This was not a statistically significant change. Caregivers on average also had lower depression (−.08) and anxiety (−.66) scores from one measurement occasion to the next; these changes were also not significant. The absence of statistical significance associated with the β10 parameter for each adjustment measure can likely be attributed to the fact that the parameter estimates represent the average rate of change for caregivers in the aggregate. In addition, the absence of significant changes for the group over time may be attributed to the substantial and statistically significant variation that existed across caregivers for these change parameters. However, the information provided by the statistically significant estimates of variance (i.e., the random effects associated with each rate of change parameter; see Table 3) indicated that the data provided sufficient variation to warrant modeling of these parameters as a function of caregiver problem-solving abilities. Given our primary focus on the rate of change parameter, the random effect for the β00 parameter for each adjustment measure was not estimated for this model. Final Model A final model was formulated to examine the variation in the Level 1 rate of change parameters (π11) as a function of problem-solving abilities that were measured at the between-caregiver level (see Table 4 for estimates obtained for this model). The high correlations between the rate of growth and the initial status parameters for each measure of adjustment in this model indicated a near linear dependency. As such, a second model was estimated wherein only the variability in the rate of change parameter (π1j) across participants was examined as a function of caregiver level problem-solving measures at Level 2. Although fixed effects estimates of the initial level or intercept parameter (i.e., β00) were obtained for each adjustment measure, the random effects associated with these parameters were not estimated for the final model. The results of the final model indicated that of the five problem-solving variables, only negative orientation had a statistically significant effect on rate of change parameters for depression (β11 = .31, p < .001), anxiety (β11 = .48, p < .001), and health complaints (β11 = .33, p < .001). The positive coefficients associated with these parameters indicate that caregivers with a higher negative orientation were likely to develop increasing levels of distress at a more rapid rate over the first year of providing care than caregivers with a lower negative orientation. The effects of negative orientation explained approximately 34%, 50%, and 21% of the variance in the rate of change parameters of depression, anxiety, and health complaints, respectively. Because we measured the problem-solving variables at only one occasion, they were specified as between-subjects (i.e., person-level) time-invariant covariates (i.e., problem-solving abilities were treated as if they did not vary over time). Given the linkage between the Level 1 and Level 2 models, and the relationship between the error terms of these models, the covariation between the outcome and the Level 2 covariates is implicit. Thus, by simple substitution it is possible to express each outcome measure as a function of the combined Level 1 and Level 2 models (Singer, 1998). In effect, the linkage between the two models takes into consideration the level of covariation between the individual difference variables (i.e., the Level 2 problemsolving predictors) and each outcome measure. Discussion Consistent with our predictions and with contemporary models of social problem solving, an NO was predictive of the variability in the rates of change in caregiver psychological and physical adjustment during the inaugural year of caregiving. As a group, caregivers did not evince a change over time for the three indices of adjustment. However, caregivers with a higher negative orientation were more likely to experience more distress during the year at a greater rate than caregivers with a lower negative orientation. Thus, caregiver negative orientation assessed during the inpatient rehabilitation program was an important indicator of subsequent caregiver adjustment over the first year of caregiving. Individuals with a higher negative orientation toward problem solving may experience greater distress for a variety of reasons. These persons are more likely to use palliative, emotion-focused coping strategies and have ruminative, irrational thoughts in time of stress that do not remedy problematic situations (D'Zurilla & Chang, 1995; MacNair & Elliott, 1992). A higher negative orientation is also associated with a more negative mood, and under duress the inability to regulate these moods may make a person susceptible to pronounced experiences of depressive symptomatology (Elliott et al., 1996). The lack of confidence and enduring pessimism that characterize a negative orientation may also render a person vulnerable to depression (Priester & Clum, 1993). Heppner and Krauskopf (1987) believe persons who have a negative appraisal of their problem-solving abilities are likely to encounter several difficulties processing problem-related information. Pessimistic ruminations and negative moods can distort available and pertinent information, interfere with retrieval and storage of information, impair the implementation of solutions, and decrease cognitive flexibility. Consistent with this position, we have found negative orientation impairs cognitive problem-solving performance independently of negative affect (Shewchuk, Johnson, & Elliott, 2000). Caregivers with a higher negative orientation might experience an unfortunate yet circular situation. They may have difficulties managing their own emotions and creatively meeting caregiving demands. When these problems are unresolved over time, pessimistic beliefs about the self may be reinforced, exacerbating their distress levels (Nezu, 1987; Nezu & D'Zurilla, 1989). This cycle could contribute to the development of more emotional and health problems as the caregiving role unfolds. The present study converges with other research indicating that caregivers who possess more cognitive–behavioral resources exhibit fewer emotional difficulties adjusting to their role (Chwalisz, 1996; Haley et al., 1996). Moreover, it appears that caregiver problem-solving abilities predict the emotional and physical health of the caregiver and care recipient (Elliott et al., 1999), as observed over the course of a year. Problem-solving interventions have demonstrated efficacy in the treatment of a variety of psychological concerns, and this approach is well suited for programs designed to help family caregivers adjust to their role and accompanying demands (D'Zurilla & Nezu, 1999). Research investigating the effects of problem-solving training with family caregivers is warranted. There is a pressing need for interventions that help family caregivers address the routines and tasks “essential to maintaining family functioning” (Altman, Cooper, & Cunningham, 1999, p. 67). Psychologists can have an immense impact in developing, evaluating, and delivering interventions that address the everyday needs of caregivers and their care recipients. Intervention research in family health psychology indicates that psychoeducational strategies are consistently more effective than other modalities, presumably because these approaches address the specific needs of family members and often actively involve family members (Burman & Margolin, 1992; Campbell & Patterson, 1995). Programs that address the specific needs of families may be more likely to succeed (Burman & Margolin, 1992). Likewise, effective interventions are those that address the problems as experienced by families (Elliott & Shewchuk, 2000). These interventions should help families to become more active and expert in their self-management and to operate competently as extensions of the formal health care system (Wagner, Austin, & von Korff, 1996). Counseling psychologists can develop and implement the delivery of problem-solving training programs for caregivers that can be provided in traditional and innovative formats that promote adjustment for the caregiver and care recipient (cf. Kurylo, Elliott, & Shewchuk, in press). This study concerned a segment of family members who assume caregiver roles for persons with spinal cord injury. Our results, therefore, may not apply to other family caregivers of persons with other health conditions. In the future, a replication with a larger sample of family caregivers—who provide care to a family member with a condition other than spinal cord injury—would help us understand more about the dynamic processes of caregiving over time. We did not examine the influence of other possible mediating characteristics such as gender, ethnicity, or income; these are issues that could also be addressed in studies with larger samples. Participant attrition over time is a common problem in longitudinal field research; it may be advisable to design research protocols that are more attentive to the special considerations and demands that family caregivers are likely to experience that can hinder participation. A longer time frame that includes more observations would permit examination of complex patterns of adjustment that might emerge over the trajectories of the caregiving role. References Altman, B. A., Cooper, P. F.,& Cunningham, P. J. (1999). The case of disability in the family: Impact on health care utilization and expenditures for nondisabled members. The Milbank Quarterly, 77, 39-73. Aneshensel, C. S., Pearlin, L. I., Mullan, J. T., Zarit, S. H.,& Whitlatch, C. J. (1995). Profiles in caregiving: The unexpected career. (San Diego, CA: Academic Press.) Arean, P. A., Perri, M., Nezu, A., Schein, R., Christopher, F.,& Joseph, T. (1993). Comparativ e effectiveness of social problem-solving therapy as treatments of depression in older adults. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 61, 1003-1010. Bryk, A.,& Raudenbush, S. W. (1987). Application of hierarchical linear models to assessing change. Psychological Bulletin, 101, 147-158. Bryk, A.,& Raudenbush, S. W. (1992). Hierarchical linear models. (Newbury Park, CA: Sage.) Bryk, A., Raudenbush, S. W.,& Congdon, R. (1996). HLM: Hierarchical linear and nonlinear modeling with the HLM/2L and HLM/3L programs. (Chicago: Scientific Software International.) Burman, B.,& Margolin, G. (1992). Analysis of the association between marital relationships and health problems: An interactional perspective. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 39-63. Burton, L. C., Newsom, J. T., Schulz, R., Hirsch, C.,& German, P. S. (1997). Preventive health behaviors among spousal caregivers. Preventive Medicine, 26, 162-169. Campbell, T. L.,& Patterson, J. M. (1995). The effectiveness of family interventions in the treatment of physical illness. Journal of Marital and Family Therapy, 21, 545583. Chang, E. C.,& D'Zurilla, T. J. (1996). Relations between problem orientation and optimism, pessimism, and trait affectivity: A construct validation study. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 34, 185-194. Chwalisz, K. (1996). The perceived stress model of caregiver burden: Evidence from the spouses of persons with brain injuries. Rehabilitation Psychology, 41, 91-114. Council on Scientific Affairs, American Medical Association. (1993). Physicians and family caregivers; A model for partnership. JAMA: Journal of the American Medical Association, 269, 1282-1284. Craig, T.,& Van Natta, P. A. (1978). Current medication use in symptoms of depression in a general population. American Journal of Psychiatry, 135, 1036-1039. de Leeuw, J.,& Kreft, I. (1986). Random coefficient models for multilevel analysis. Journal of Educational Statistics, 11, 57-85. Drotar, D. (1997). Relating parent and family functioning to the psychological adjustment of children with chronic health conditions: What have we learned? What do we need to know? Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 22, 149-165. D'Zurilla, T. J.,& Chang, E. (1995). The relations between social problem solving and coping. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 19, 547-562. D'Zurilla, T. J.,& Nezu, A. M. (1999). Problem-solving therapy ((2nd ed.). New York: Springer.) D'Zurilla, T. J., Nezu, A. M.,& Maydeu-Olivares, A. (). Manual for the Social Problem Solving Inventory–Revised (SPSI–R). (in pressNorth Tonawanda, NY: Multi-Health Systems.) Elliott, T. (1999). Social problem-solving abilities and adjustment to recent-onset spinal cord injury. Rehabilitation Psychology, 44, 315-332. Elliott, T., Godshall, F., Herrick, S., Witty, T.,& Spruell, M. (1991). Problemsolving appraisal and psychological adjustment following spinal cord injury. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 15, 387-398. Elliott, T.,& Marmarosh, C. (1994). Problem-solving appraisal, health complaints, and health-related expectancies. Journal of Counseling & Development, 72, 531-537. Elliott, T., Sherwin, E., Harkins, S.,& Marmarosh, C. (1995). Self-appraised problem-solving ability, affective states, and psychological distress. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 42, 105-115. Elliott, T.,& Shewchuk, R. M. (2000). Problem-solving therapy for family caregivers of persons with severe physical disabilities. (In C. Radnitz (Ed.), Cognitive– behavioral interventions for persons with disabilities (pp. 309–327). Northvale, NJ: Jason Aronson.) Elliott, T., Shewchuk, R. M.,& Richards, J. S. (1999). Caregiver social problemsolving abilities and family member adjustment to recent-onset physical disability. Rehabilitation Psychology, 44, 104-123. Elliott, T., Shewchuk, R., Richeson, C., Pickelman, H.,& Franklin, K. W. (1996). Problem-solving appraisal and the prediction of depression during pregnancy and in the postpartum period. Journal of Counseling & Development, 74, 645-651. Elliott, T.,& Umlauf, R. (1995). Measurement of personality and psychopathology in acquired disability. (In L. Cushman & M. Scherer (Eds.), Psychological assessment in medical rehabilitation settings (pp. 325–358). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.) Foelker, G. A.,& Shewchuk, R. (1992). Somatic complaints and the CES– D. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 40, 259-262. Francis, D. J., Fletcher, J. M., Stuebing, K. K., Davidson, K. C.,& Thompson, N. M. (1991). Analysis of change: Modeling individual growth. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 59, 27-37. Frank, R. G., Thayer, J., Hagglund, K., Vieth, A., Schopp, L., Beck, N., Kashani, J., Goldstein, D., Cassidy, J. T., Clay, D., Chaney, J., Hewett, J.,& Johnson, J. (1998). Trajectories of adaptation in pediatric chronic illness: The importance of the individual. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 66, 521-532. Godshall, F.,& Elliott, T. (1997). Behavioral correlates of self-appraised problemsolving ability: Problem-solving skills and health-compromising behaviors. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 27, 929-944. Goldstein, H. (1986). Multilevel mixed linear model analysis using iterative generalized least squares. Biometrika, 73, 43-56. Goldstein, H., Healy, M.,& Rasbash, J. (1994). Multilevel time series models with applications to repeated measures data. Statistics in Medicine, 13, 1643-1655. Grant, J. S. (1999). Social problem-solving partnerships with family caregivers. Rehabilitation Nursing, 24, 256-262. Grant, J. S., Elliott, T., Giger, J. N.,& Bartolucci, A. (2001). Social problem-solving abilities, social support, and adjustment among family caregivers of individuals with a stroke. Rehabilitation Psychology, 46, 44-57. Haley, W. E., Waff, D., Coleton, M., Ford, G., West, C., Collins, R.,& Isobe, T. (1996). Apprai sal, coping, and social support as mediators of well-being in Black and White family caregivers of patients with Alzheimer's disease. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 64, 121-129. Heppner, P. P. (1988). A manual for the Problem Solving Inventory. (Columbia: University of Missouri.) Heppner, P. P.,& Krauskopf, C. J. (1987). An information processing approach to personal problem solving. The Counseling Psychologist, 15, 371-447. Hoffman, C., Rice, D.,& Sung, H. (1996). Persons with chronic conditions: Their prevalence and costs. JAMA: Journal of the American Medical Association, 276, 1473-1479. Hooker, K., Monahan, D., Shifren, K.,& Hutchinson, C. (1992). Mental and physical health of spouse caregivers: The role of personality. Psychology and Aging, 7, 367375. Houts, P. S., Nezu, A. M., Nezu, C. M.,& Bucher, J. A. (1996). The prepared family caregiver: A problem-solving approach to family caregiver education. Patient Education and Counseling, 27, 63-73. Jöreskog, K., Sörbom, D., du Toit, S.,& du Toit, M. (1999). LISREL 8: New statistical features. (Chicago: Scientific Software International.) Kane, R. A.,& Kane, R. L. (1987). Long-term care: Principles, programs and policies. (New York: Springer.) Kurylo, M., Elliott, T.,& Shewchuk, R. (). FOCUS on the family caregiver: A problem-solving training intervention. Journal of Counseling & Development, , . (in press) MacNair, R. R.,& Elliott, T. (1992). Self-perceived problem-solving ability, stress appraisal, and coping over time. Journal of Research in Personality, 26, 150-164. Matthews, A. M. (1993). Issues in the examination of the caregiving relationship. (In S. H. Zarit, L. I. Pearlin, & K. W. Schaie (Eds.), Caregiving systems: Informal and formal helpers (pp. 107–118). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.) Maydeu-Olivares, A.,& D'Zurilla, T. J. (1996). A factor analytic study of the social problem solving inventory: An integration of theory and data. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 20, 115-133. Moen, P., Robison, J.,& Dempster-McClain, D. (1995). Caregiving and women's well-being: A life course approach. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 36, 259273. Neugarten, B. L. (1979). Time, age, and the life cycle. American Journal of Psychiatry, 36, 887-894. Nezu, A. M. (1987). A problem-solving formulation for depression: A literature review and proposal of a pluralistic model. Clinical Psychology Review, 7, 121-144. Nezu, A. M.,& D'Zurilla, T. J. (1989). Social problem solving and negative affect. (In P. Kendall & D. Watson (Eds.), Anxiety and depression: Distinctive and overlapping features (pp. 285–315). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.) Nezu, A. M.,& Perri, M. G. (1989). Social problem-solving therapy for unipolar depression: An initial dismantling investigation. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 57, 408-413. Noojin, A. B.,& Wallender, J. L. (1997). Perceived problem-solving ability, stress, and coping in mothers of children with physical disabilities: Potential cognitive influences on adjustment. International Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 4, 415-432. Pennebaker, J. W. (1982). The psychology of physical symptoms. (New York: Springer-Verlag.) Priester, M. J.,& Clum, G. (1993). Perceived problem-solving ability as a predictor of depression, hopelessness, and suicide ideation in a college population. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 40, 79-85. Quittner, A. L., Glueckauf, R.,& Jackson, D. (1990). Chronic parenting stress: Moderating versus mediating effects of social support. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 1266-1278. Quittner, A. L., Opipari, L., Regoli, M., Jacobsen, J.,& Eigen, H. (1992). The impact of caregiving and role strain on family life: Comparisons between mothers of children with cystic fibrosis and matched controls. Rehabilitation Psychology, 37, 275-290. Radloff, L. S. (1977). The CES–D scale: A self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Applied Psychological Measurement, 1, 385-401. Richards, C. S.,& Perri, M. G. (1978). Do self-control treatments last? An evaluation of behavioral problem solving and faded counselor contact as treatment maintenance strategies. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 25, 376-383. Sadowski, C., Moore, L. A.,& Kelly, M. L. (1994). Psychometric properties of the social problem-solving inventory (SPSI) with normal and emotionally-disturbed adolescents. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 22, 487-500. Schulz, R., Newsom, J., Mittelmark, M., Burton, L., Hirsch, C.,& Jackson, S. (1997). Health effects of caregiving: The caregiver health effects study: An ancillary study of the cardiovascular health study. Annals of Behavior Medicine, 19, 110-116. Schulz, R.,& Quittner, A. L. (1998). Caregiving for children and adults with chronic conditions: Introduction to the special issue. Health Psychology, 17, 107-111. Shewchuk, R.,& Elliott, T. (2000). Family caregiving in chronic disease and disability: Implications for rehabilitation psychology. (In R. G. Frank & T. Elliott (Eds.), Handbook of rehabilitation psychology (pp. 553–563). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.) Shewchuk, R., Johnson, M.,& Elliott, T. (2000). Self-appraised social problem- solving abilities, emotional reactions, and actual problem-solving performance. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 38, 727-740. Shewchuk, R., Richards, J. S.,& Elliott, T. (1998). Dynamic processes in health outcomes among caregivers of patients with spinal cord injuries. Health Psychology, 17, 125-129. Singer, J. D. (1998). Using SAS PROC MIXED to fit multilevel models, hierarchical models, and individual growth models. Journal of Educational and Behavioral Statistics, 24, 323-355. Spielberger, C. D. (1989). State-Trait Anxiety Inventory: Bibliography ((2nd ed.). Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.) Spielberger, C. D., Gorsuch, R. L., Lushene, R., Vagg, P. R.,& Jacobs, G. A. (1983). Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory. (Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.) Tate, R. L.,& Hokanson, J. E. (1993). Analyzing individual status and change with hierarchical linear models: Illustration with depression in college students. Journal of Personality, 61, 181-206. Vitaliano, P. P. (1997). Physiological and physical concomitants of caregiving: Introduction to the special issue [Special issue]. (Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 19(2).) Wagner, E. H., Austin, B. T.,& von Korff, M. (1996). Organizing care for patients with chronic illness. The Milbank Quarterly, 74, 511-544. Weissman, M. M., Prusoff, B.,& Newberry, P. B. (1975). Comparison of CES–D, Zung, and Beck self-report depression scales. ((Technical Report No. ADM 42-4783). Rockville, MD: National Institute of Mental Health, Center for Epidemiologic Studies.) Weitzenkamp, D. A., Gerhart, K. A., Charlifue, S. W., Whiteneck, G. G.,& Savic, G. (1997). Spouses of spinal cord injury survivors: The added impact of caregiving. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, 78, 822-827. Willett, J. B. (1988). Questions and answers in the measurement of change. (In E. Rothkopf (Ed.), Review of research and education (pp. 345–422). Washington, DC: American Educational Research Association.) 1 A thorough description of the statistical procedures used in the current study is available from Timothy R. Elliott. Results of alternate models in which attempts were made to specify the three outcome measures as observed indicants of a single latent adaptation construct are also available on request. This study was supported by Grants H133B30025 and H133N5009 from the National Institute on Disability and Rehabilitation Research and Grant R49/CCR412718-01 from the National Center for Injury Prevention and Control and the Disabilities Prevention Program, National Center for Environmental Health. Its contents are solely the responsibility of the authors and do not necessarily represent the official views of the funding agencies. This study is one in a series investigating the adjustment of family caregiver and care recipients following the onset of spinal cord injury. Correspondence may be addressed to Timothy R. Elliott, Spain Rehabilitation Center 530, 619 19th Street South, Birmingham, Alabama 35249-7330. Electronic mail may be sent to telliott@uab.edu Received: February 2, 2000 Revised: July 27, 2000 Accepted: October 24, 2000 Table 1. Number of Observations for Outcome Measures Table 2. Means, Standard Deviations, and Correlations Observed for Social Problem-Solving Abilities and the Outcome Variables Table 3. Unconditional Baseline Model of Estimated Caregiver Adjustment Curve Parameters and Variance Components Table 4. Final Model: Social Problem-Solving Abilities As Predictors of Rates of Change in Caregiver Depressive Behavior, Anxiety, and Health Complaints Over the Initial Year of Caregiving