LD When Toddlers Turn on the TV and Actually Learn By: Lisa Guernsey, Adapted from: New York Times, September 5, 2006 http://www.nytimes.com/2006/09/05/health/psychology/05tots.html 1. Sit down with a 3-year-old to watch “Blue’s Clues” or “Dora the Explorer,” and see the shouting erupt. Whenever a character faces the camera and asks a question, children out there in TV land are usually answering it. Active engagement with television has been an answer to criticism that TV creates zombies. “Blue’s Clues,” which celebrated its 10th anniversary last month, has been credited with helping young children learn from the screen. Indeed, academic research has shown that viewers ages three to five score better on tests of problem solving than those who haven’t watched the show. 2. But what happens with children younger than three years old? Should babies and toddlers be exposed to television at all? Is there any chance that they could actually learn from the screen? While debates continue among parents, pediatricians, and critics of baby videos, developmental psychologists are conducting research to find answers to these questions. Questions 1. Why does the writer mention the example of “Blue’s Clues”? The writer mentions “Blue’s Clues” because it is an example of a TV show that: a. has been around for the past 10 years b. interactively involves young viewers c. creates passive zombies d. causes young children to shout 2. Which children were able to solve problems better? ___________________________________________________________ _______________________________________________________________________ When Toddlers Turn on the TV and Actually Learn/1 3. Read paragraph 2. Based on this paragraph, how do you expect the writer to answer the questions? a. by explaining the opinions of various educators b. by comparing different educational TV shows c. by presenting evidence from research d. by giving examples of children who learned from TV ______________________________________________________________ 3. Experiments conducted at Vanderbilt University, described in the May/June issue of Child Development, provide some information about toddlers. Georgene L. Troseth and Megan M. Saylor, psychologists at Vanderbilt, and Allison H. Archer, an undergraduate student there, designed the study to find out if toddlers would learn from video if they considered the onscreen actors to be, as they put it, “social partners.” 4. The experiments compared two video experiences. One was based on a videotape. Watching it was similar to watching “Blue’s Clues”; the actor onscreen paused to simulate a conversation, but back-and-forth interaction with the viewer was impossible. A different group of children experienced twoway live video. It worked like a Web cam, with each side responding in real time. 5. The test involved a hiding game. First the two-year-olds watched the video — either the tape or the live version. The screen showed a person hiding a stuffed animal, Piglet, in a nearby room, often under a table or behind a couch. When the video ended, the children were asked to retrieve Piglet. Those who saw the recorded video had some trouble. They found him only 35 percent of the time. Children in the other group succeeded about 69 percent of the time, a rate similar to face-to-face interaction. 6. This experiment shows that 24-month-olds are more likely to use information conveyed by video if they consider the person on the screen to be someone they can talk to. Without that, the children seemed unable to act on what they had seen and heard. _______________________________________________________________________ When Toddlers Turn on the TV and Actually Learn/2 Questions 4. Read paragraphs 3-6 to fill in the table below about the study at Vanderbilt University. I. Research Question II. Methods A. Participants Group 1: __________________________________________ _________________________________________________ Group 2: __________________________________________ __________________________________________________ B. Procedure Step 1: The children had a video experience. Step 2: The children _________________________________ III. Results Group 1 was BETTER / WORSE than group 2. IV. Conclusion Children learn better from videos when __________________ _________________________________________________ _________________________________________________ ______________________________________________________________ 7. Does this mean that TV programs that simulate interaction are doing nothing for young children? Not necessarily, the researchers say. A few of the children in the recorded video group were especially responsive to the games and pauses, and they were the few children in that group who retrieved the toy. “We found that if children gave evidence of treating the video as a social partner,” Dr. Troseth said, “they will use the information.” 8. Developmental psychologists say the Vanderbilt research offers an intriguing clue to a phenomenon called the “video deficit.” Toddlers who have no trouble understanding a task demonstrated in real life often have problems when the same task is shown onscreen. They need repeated viewings to figure it out. This deficit got its name in a 2005 article by Daniel R. Anderson and Tiffany A. Pempek, psychologists at the University of Massachusetts, who _______________________________________________________________________ When Toddlers Turn on the TV and Actually Learn/3 reviewed literature on young children and television. Child-development experts say the deficit confirms the age-old wisdom that real-life interactions are best for babies. Parents can be assured, they say, that their presence is more beneficial than TV. 9. But psychologists still want to get to the bottom of what might explain the difference. Is it the two-dimensionality of the screen? Do young children have some innate difficulty in remembering information transmitted as symbols? “It’s definitely still a puzzle, and we’re trying to figure out the different components to it,” said Rachel Barr, a psychologist at Georgetown University who specializes in infant memory. 10. The Vanderbilt research offers the possibility that the more socially engaging a video is, the more likely the deficit will disappear. But Dr. Troseth and other psychologists stress that in-person connections with parents are by far a child’s best teacher. However, this probably doesn’t include those moments when busy and tired parents are so distracted that TV characters are more responsive than they are. Questions 5. (par. 7) According to the article, can TV programs that simulate interaction be useful? YES / NO Explain your answer. ___________________________________________________________ 6. a. What is the “video deficit”? ___________________________________________________________ b. Do researchers have a clear explanation for this phenomenon? YES / NO Justify your answer by quoting from the text. ___________________________________________________________ _______________________________________________________________________ When Toddlers Turn on the TV and Actually Learn/4 7. In paragraph 9 the author says that “psychologists still want to get to the bottom of what might explain the difference.” What difference is the author referring to? (Fill in the blanks to answer.) The difference between _________________ and _________________. 8. Under what condition might the video deficit disappear? (2 WORDS) When the video is ___________________________________. ______________________________________________________________ _______________________________________________________________________ When Toddlers Turn on the TV and Actually Learn/5