INTRAPERSONAL CONSEQUENCES OF ANOTHER’S JEALOUSY EXPRESSION: TOWARD A REACTION MODEL OF JEALOUSY IN CLOSE RELATIONSHIPS by JENNIFER LEIGH BEVAN (Under the Direction of Jerold L. Hale) ABSTRACT Jealousy research primarily involves identifying and studying antecedents and correlates of jealousy experience and expression. To learn more about the consequences of jealousy expression, this project proposes and tests four potential cognitive and emotional reactions to another’s jealousy expression. Specifically, general partner uncertainty, relational uncertainty, jealousy-related emotion, and rumination are predicted to arise when one’s close relational partner expresses jealousy. These reactions are examined in relation to relational context (i.e., dating partnership, sibling relationship, and cross-sex friendship) and jealousy expression type (i.e., integrative communication, distributive communication, and negative affect expression). Uncertainty findings revealed that cross-sex friends were more uncertain about the partner and the relationship than either siblings or daters after another expresses jealousy. Siblings experienced greater relational uncertainty than did dating partners when reacting to another’s jealousy expression. In addition, another’s use of negative affect expression was related to greater partner and relational uncertainty compared with another’s use of integrative communication. For jealousy-related emotion, siblings and dating partners experienced stronger jealousy-related emotion compared with cross-sex friends, though jealousyrelated emotion levels did not differ according to jealousy expression type. In terms of rumination, when one’s partner used distributive communication or negative affect expression to communicate jealousy, that individual ruminated more than if integrative communication was used. Rumination levels did not differ according to relational context. In addition, a reaction model of jealousy is presented that specifies the order in which these reactions occur after one’s close partner expresses jealousy. After one’s partner expresses jealousy, that individual first experiences general partner uncertainty, followed by relational uncertainty. Next, the individual ruminates about the jealousy expression and then finally experiences jealousy-related emotion. These results broaden jealousy expression research to include consequences of jealousy expression. INDEX WORDS: Jealousy expression, General partner uncertainty, Relational uncertainty, Jealousy emotion, Rumination, Sibling relationships, Cross-sex friendships, Dating relationships INTRAPERSONAL CONSEQUENCES OF ANOTHER’S JEALOUSY EXPRESSION: TOWARD A REACTION MODEL OF JEALOUSY IN CLOSE RELATIONSHIPS by JENNIFER LEIGH BEVAN B.A., University of Delaware, 1997 M.A., University of Delaware, 2000 A Dissertation Submitted to the Graduate Faculty of The University of Georgia in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY ATHENS, GEORGIA 2003 © 2003 Jennifer Leigh Bevan All Rights Reserved INTRAPERSONAL CONSEQUENCES OF ANOTHER’S JEALOUSY EXPRESSION: TOWARD A REACTIONAL MODEL OF JEALOUSY IN CLOSE RELATIONSHIPS by JENNIFER LEIGH BEVAN Electronic Version Approved: Maureen Grasso Dean of the Graduate School The University of Georgia May 2003 Major Professor: Jerold Hale Committee: Kenzie Cameron Tina Harris Michael Kernis Jennifer Monahan iv DEDICATION This dissertation is dedicated to my maternal grandmother, Margaret Virginia Snyder Bull, who passed away before this project was completed but who was always proud and supportive of my obtaining my Ph.D. v ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS There are many people who have been a part of my journey toward finishing my dissertation and my Ph.D. I am grateful overall to my committee members, my friends, and my family for the important and essential ways that they guided me through this long and sometimes frustrating process. First, I would like to thank Jerry Hale, my major professor. His suggestions always helped to improve this dissertation and his calm demeanor always helped to counteract my “occasional” overexcitement and anxiety about the project. I am also grateful to Jennifer Monahan and Kenzie Cameron, who I always felt comfortable approaching with any sort of question about interpersonal communication. Tina Harris and Mike Kernis also contributed unique and useful suggestions to improve this project. Many friends have been instrumental to my success as a graduate student. Without Sandy Williams, I simply never would have gotten through my first two years at Georgia and I will eternally be grateful to her for our distracting notes in class, our coffee shop “work” sessions, and the many projects we have worked on together (including data collection for her master’s thesis). Without Kristy Maddux, I would not have been able to finish up my final two years at Georgia. Our gossip sessions, her willingness to baby-sit her “nieces” Daphne and Oreo, and our shopping trips will make me miss being in Athens for years to come. In addition, I am grateful for the friendships that I share with John Lynch, Rich Nabring, Shawn Powers, Kristan Poirot, Tasha Dubriwny, Pam Lannutti, Eric Taubel, and John Roberts. My experience at Georgia has been greatly vi enhanced by knowing all of you. Finally, and most notably, I want to acknowledge the influence and importance of my master’s advisor, Wendy Samter. Without you, I simply would not be where I am today. My family has also been so instrumental in this journey. My mother, Jennie Bull, and my father, Ron Bevan, have always supported my desire to earn a Ph.D., even if they weren’t exactly sure what I was studying! Their pride in their only child was always vast and unwavering. Also, I am excited knowing that I have made my maternal grandmother, Margaret Bull (who passed away in February 2003 and to whom this dissertation is dedicated), and my paternal grandparents, Jerry and Bob Bevan, proud by completing my dissertation and earning my Ph.D. Finally, I am most grateful to my immediate “family”: Gordon, Daphne, and Oreo. Gordon is the most amazing fiancée that a girl could have. There is no one else that knows and understands and loves (and tolerates!) me better. I am thrilled to be spending my life with you and so thankful that you choose to do the same with me. Our sweet pets also deserve recognition, as they have been with me first-hand throughout the dissertation process and thus should be awarded medals of honor for being on the front lines! I love you all. vii TABLE OF CONTENTS Page ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS .................................................................................................v LIST OF TABLES ........................................................................................................... viii LIST OF FIGURES ........................................................................................................... ix CHAPTER 1 INTRODUCTION, BACKGROUND, AND RATIONALE.............................1 The Nature of Relational Jealousy ................................................................3 Proposed Consequences of Another’s Jealousy Expression .......................17 2 METHODOLOGY ..........................................................................................33 Pilot Studies .................................................................................................33 Main Investigation .......................................................................................49 3 RESULTS ........................................................................................................64 4 DISCUSSIONS, LIMITATIONS, AND CONCLUSIONS ............................80 Consequences of Another’s Jealousy Expression .......................................81 The Reaction Model of Jealousy in Close Relationships ............................91 Limitations and Conclusions .......................................................................95 REFERENCES ................................................................................................................100 APPENDICES A EXAMPLE OF PILOT STUDY I INSTRUMENT (CROSS-SEX FRIEND CONDITION) ...........................................................................................116 viii B ORIGINAL JEALOUSY EXPRESSION DEFINITIONS AND EXAMPLES FROM GUERRERO ET AL. (1995) AND JEALOUSY EXPRESSION MESSAGES TESTED IN PILOT STUDY I ............................................125 C EXAMPLE OF PILOT STUDY II INSTRUMENT (DATING PARTNER CONDITION) ...........................................................................................127 D EXAMPLE OF MAIN INVESTIGATION INSTRUMENT (SIBLINGNEGATIVE AFFECT EXPRESSION CONDITION) .............................130 ix LIST OF TABLES Page Table 1: Realism, Frequency of Occurrence, and Ever Occurred Variable Means and Standard Deviations for Relational Context Scenarios Examined in Pilot I and Pilot II ..38 Table 2: Valence and Realism Variable Means and Standard Deviations for Jealousy Expression Types Examined in Pilot I ...............................................................................42 Table 3: Demographic Variables Overall and by Relational Context ...............................53 Table 4: Relational Variables Overall and by Relational Context .....................................54 Table 5: Means, Standard Deviations, and Correlations for Main Investigation Dependent Variables ............................................................................................................................61 Table 6: Realism, Frequency of Occurrence, and Ever Occurred Variable Means and Standard Deviations for Jealousy Scenarios Examined in Main Investigation .................67 Table 7: Valence and Realism Variable Means and Standard Deviations for Jealousy Expression Types Examined in Main Investigation ..........................................................69 Table 8: Means and Standard Deviations for the Relationship between Relational Context and the Dependent Variables .............................................................................................74 Table 9: Means and Standard Deviations for the Relationship between Jealousy Expression Type and the Dependent Variables .................................................................75 x LIST OF FIGURES Page Figure 1: Proposed Path-Analytic Reaction Model of Jealousy ........................................32 Figure 2: Final Path-Analytic Reaction Model of Jealousy with Path Coefficients ..........79 1 CHAPTER 1 INTRODUCTION, BACKGROUND, AND RATIONALE Jealousy has commonly been conceptualized as an interpersonal event that necessarily involves an individual, his or her relational partner, and a potentially threatening rival (e.g., White & Mullen, 1989). Specifically, because relational jealousy occurs only as a result of one’s attachment to another, it is viewed as an “inherently relational emotion” (Staske, 1999, p. 214). However, the study of jealousy within close relationships typically has focused solely on individual and relational antecedents and correlates of jealousy. For example, recent research has explored jealousy experience and expression in relation to individuals’ sex and gender (Aylor & Dainton, 2001), sexual orientation (Bevan & Lannutti, 2002), and loneliness (Rotenberg, Shewchuk, & Kimberley, 2001) and in association with one partner’s assessment of relationship commitment (Marelich, Gaines, & Banzet, in press), length (Aune & Comstock, 1997), and intimacy (Knobloch, Solomon, & Cruz, 2001). Although these studies are informative, one area is still lacking in jealousy expression research: information about an individual’s reactions to his or her relational partner’s direct communication of jealousy. Specifically, the intrapersonal reaction of an individual to another’s expression of jealousy has escaped extensive scrutiny but is important for two reasons. First, examining jealousy from this perspective can provide crucial insight into the reactional nature of jealousy expression within close relationships and the future relational consequences that could result. Scholars (e.g., Constantine, 2 1976; Ellis & Weinstein, 1986) suggest that jealousy exists in all close partnerships, meaning that its presence and impact is likely to be negotiated and dealt with by relational partners during the course of a relationship. Second, the study of jealousy is lacking in useful theoretical frameworks created specifically to explain the process of jealousy in close partnerships. Two theoretical trends are currently evident in jealousy research. First, recent research that tested the viability of Bryson’s (1991) dual motivation model of jealousy (Guerrero & Afifi, 1998) and Bringle’s (1991) transactional model of jealousy (Marelich et al., in press) failed to support the tenets set forth by these frameworks. Thus, a number of the models believed to explain jealousy processes have not received empirical support. Second, many scholars have imported general psychology and communication theories to explain the jealousy process, including attachment theory (Guerrero, 1998) and evolution theory (Buss, 2000; Buunk & Dijkstra, 2001). However, merely including jealousy as part of the scope of these theories does not provide a comprehensive theoretical framework of jealousy processes. Thus, current jealousy research should strive to create and refine empirically supported and useful jealousy theories and models. To examine both of these issues, the current project intends to study the associations between relational context (i.e., siblings, cross-sex friends, and dating partners), types of jealousy expression, and subsequent cognitive and emotional reactions to another’s jealousy expression. More specifically, the present examination proposes that jealousy-related emotions, general partner and relational uncertainty, and rumination within an individual will arise in reaction to another’s jealousy expression. Further, these variables are examined in concert with one another to create a reaction model of jealousy 3 useful in explaining how one partner reacts cognitively and emotionally to the other partner’s jealousy expression within their relationship. The Nature of Relational Jealousy Jealousy is commonly defined as a perceived or actual threat to the exclusive nature of one’s romantic relationship (e.g., Afifi & Reichert, 1996; Bringle & Buunk, 1986; White, 1981; White & Mullen, 1989). However, conceptualizing jealousy in this manner is restrictive because it does not acknowledge the presence and importance of nonromantic jealousy across different relational contexts (Bevan & Samter, 2001). As such, jealousy is broadly defined as “a protective reaction to a perceived threat to a valued relationship, arising from a situation in which the partner’s involvement with an activity and/or another person is contrary to the jealous person’s definition of their relationship” (Hansen, 1991, p. 213). This characterization, an expanded version of Clanton’s (1981) definition, is useful for two reasons: (1) it is applicable to a range of relational contexts; and (2) it suggests that the relationship is valuable to the jealous individual. When jealousy does occur, it triggers a multidimensional experience involving cognition, emotion, and communication (Guerrero & Andersen, 1998b; Pfeiffer & Wong, 1989). According to Aylor and Dainton (2001), communication research has extended the psychological study of jealousy in two ways: (1) by emphasizing the interactive nature of jealousy by distinguishing between the cognitive and emotional experience of jealousy and its communicative expression; and (2) by acknowledging the presence of prosocial jealousy responses, which are frequently overlooked but important in terms of relational development and satisfaction. In addition, communication scholars have also 4 developed the most comprehensive typology of jealousy expression. Specifically, Guerrero, Andersen, Jorgensen, Spitzberg, and Eloy (1995) developed 14 communicative responses to jealousy, defined as behavioral reactions to jealousy that possess communicative value and have the potential to satisfy individual and relational goals. Guerrero et al.’s (1995) jealousy responses can be divided into two general classes of jealousy expression: interactive responses, which are efforts to either engage in or avoid direct communication with one’s partner, and general behavioral responses, which may be aimed at one’s partner, but do not necessarily involve direct communication between the partners. Generally, the communication of jealousy and how it arises can help relational partners manage and negotiate it (Planalp, 1999). In order to further knowledge regarding how jealousy is expressed by one partner and the other partner’s reaction to that expression, three interactive responses will be focused upon: integrative communication, negative affect expression, and distributive communication. Guerrero and Andersen (1998b) define integrative communication as an expression of jealousy that is direct and non-aggressive, such as disclosures and reassurances. Negative affect expression is defined as nonverbal jealousy expression that one’s partner can observe, including appearing hurt, crying, or acting anxious. Finally, distributive communication involves direct and aggressive jealousy expression such as accusations, sarcasm, or arguing. For the purposes of the current study, these jealousy responses were selected from Guerrero et al.’s typology (1995) for three reasons: (1) each is specifically directed toward the partner; (2) each has been found to occur with relative frequency across a variety of samples (Bevan & Lannutti, 2002; Bevan & Samter, 2001; Guerrero et al., 5 1995), meaning that participants should be able to either recall a time when each did occur or easily imagine an instance where each could happen in their own relationships; and (3) each are likely to elicit qualitatively different reactions from one’s partner. Namely, distributive communication is a negative method of jealousy expression, with Guerrero and Andersen (1998b, p. 179) calling it “one of the most deleterious communication strategies.” Negative affect expression could be viewed as a neutral strategy because it can be positively or negatively construed depending upon the other methods of jealousy expression it accompanies (Andersen, Eloy, Guerrero, & Spitzberg, 1995; Guerrero et al., 1995). Finally, integrative communication is a positive method for communicating jealousy because it can have constructive relational consequences (Guerrero & Andersen, 1998b). The Reaction Perspective in Jealousy Research The bulk of jealousy research has centered on the individual who is jealous and how he or she experiences and communicates jealousy. In other words, although communication implies that the partner is somehow involved in the jealousy process, most scholars only assess the way one’s partner would express jealousy and fall short of measuring an individual’s reaction. However, jealousy should be considered from a reaction perspective where the behavior of one partner is examined in terms of the intrapersonal and interpersonal reactions from the other partner, and vice versa. In other words, much like Dance’s (1967) helical spiral as a representation of interpersonal communication, the interaction between partners’ emotions, cognitions, and behaviors should be acknowledged according to the feedback received by both individuals during the exchange. 6 This reaction perspective is important, as it allows for a more comprehensive and realistic conceptualization of jealousy in close relationships. For example, three important models of romantic jealousy (Guerrero & Andersen, 1998b; Pfeiffer & Wong, 1989; White, 1981) fail to specify how an individual might react to or evaluate various types of communicative or coping strategies used by another. Instead, these jealousy models merely allude to the effect of jealousy expression or coping by labeling them as “outcomes” or “relational consequences.” According to Guerrero and Andersen (1998b, p. 182), “how the partner and rival respond to the jealous individual’s communication, and how their communication affects the jealous person’s goals, decisions, and behaviors are interesting and important questions that have yet to be addressed.” Further, Yoshimura, Guerrero, and Trost (1999, p. 3) point out that solely examining the jealous individual’s perspective prevents scholars from “seeing the more global context in which ‘jealousy’ occurs.” In response to these calls for studying jealousy as a more reactional process, the current project focuses upon an individual’s cognitive and emotional reactions to another’s direct communication of jealousy. In other words, what intrapersonal processes do individuals go through after another expresses jealousy to them? Only a handful of jealousy researchers have suggested possible reactions to another’s jealousy. For example, Ellis and Weinstein (1986) note that expressing jealousy to another can carry risks such as the jealousy object viewing the jealous individual as overly demanding or suspicious or feeling as if the jealous individual views him or her as dishonest or deceiving. Conversely, Planalp (1999) states that jealousy expression can be flattering for the jealousy object. Further, Yoshimura et al. (1999) suggested that an individual who 7 another is jealous of can respond using a number of communication strategies, including denial of the jealousy-provoking behavior, assurances to the jealous individual of continued love and commitment, requests to the jealous individual for increased trust, confirmation that the rival relationship exists and either is or is not threatening, and use of partner jealousy expression as an excuse to end the relationship. When considering empirical research on the subject, Mullen and Martin (1994) found that almost 20% of a sample of members of the general population believed that their partner’s jealousy caused a great deal of problems in the relationship. However, not all jealousy interactions are negative. In a case study of jealousy between cross-sex friends, a “shared understanding” of the male’s relational jealousy was reached and resulted in the friends’ understanding of one’s romantic attachment to the other (Staske, 1999). From this, Staske (1999, p. 233) concluded that positive discussions of jealousy between cross-sex friends can “provide a particularly useful resource in the ongoing construction of romantic relational identities and the romantic bond.” In the most comprehensive known examination of an individual’s reaction to another’s jealousy expression, Sheets, Fredendall, and Claypool (1997) asked 178 pilotstudy participants to imagine that their partners became jealous following specific jealousy-evoking situations and then describe how they would likely react to such jealousy expressions. Forty six percent of the responses were reassurances such as restating commitment to the relationship, ignoring/making light of the jealousy comprised 20% of the reactions, 12% of the responses were exacerbations, where the partner accented his or her attraction to the other, and 10% were explanations of actions to the partner. In a subsequent study, Sheets et al. (1997) found that reassuring and 8 exacerbating comments from one’s partner were positively associated with the partner’s expected relationship stability (as perceived by the participant). Further, reassuring comments from the other person were positively associated with an individual’s actual relationship stability. From these results, the authors concluded that reassurances from the partner appear to strengthen the romantic bond. Further, jealousy can be seen as immediately negative but can actually contribute to relational stability when countered with partner reassurances. The above research findings provide encouraging preliminary evidence that the reaction perspective in jealousy research will be fruitful in learning more about how relationships are affected by jealousy situations. The first step in this process is to determine what an individual’s cognitive and emotional reactions might be when another expresses jealousy and how these reactions might vary according to relational context and the actual method used by the other to communicate jealousy. A discussion of potentially relevant relational contexts is provided below. Jealousy in Non-Romantic and Romantic Relational Contexts Jealousy is believed to be present in nearly all partnerships (Constantine, 1976). Despite this idea that jealousy is almost universal, it is primarily studied in relation to romantic relationships, possibly because romantic jealousy is viewed as a particularly potent threat between romantic partners (e.g., Bringle, 1995; Ellis & Weinstein, 1986). Numerous researchers (e.g., Aune & Comstock, 1997; Bringle & Buunk, 1991) have suggested that those in romantic relationships can experience jealousy about partners’ involvement with friends, family, hobbies, or work. Indeed, Hansen (1982, 1985) substantiates these claims with his findings that involvement with family members and 9 spending extra time at work can elicit moderate to high jealousy responses between spouses. Jealousy has also been suggested within multiple relational contexts. Specifically, jealousy has occurred between business associates, romantic partners, family members, or friends (Buunk & Bringle, 1986; Guerrero & Andersen, 1998a; Hill & Davis, 2000; White & Mullen, 1989). Buunk and Bringle (1987, p. 125) note that jealousy across relationships is plausible because each relationship type can be characterized by “strong, frequent, and diverse interdependency that lasts over a considerable period of time.” Further, Guerrero and Andersen (1998a) contend that jealousy across non-romantic relational contexts consistently involves a threat to something that is valued and is in jeopardy of being altered or taken away. Relational characteristics are important to the understanding of jealousy experience. Indeed, Bringle and Buunk (1986) note that relationship characteristics accounted for more variance than did personality variables in terms of jealousy experience. Further, previous research indicates that dating relationships (Aylor & Dainton, 2001; Guerrero, Eloy, Jorgensen, & Andersen, 1993), sibling relationships (Masiuch & Kienapple, 1993; Miller, Volling, & McElwain, 2000) and cross-sex friendships (Bevan, 1999; Bevan & Samter, 2001; Werking, 1997b) are three relational contexts that seem especially prone to jealousy. In fact, Bringle (1991) reported that significantly more people had experienced friend jealousy, romantic jealousy, or sibling jealousy than had not. As such, these three types of relationships are of particular interest to the present examination and are developed in more detail below. 10 Jealousy in Dating Relationships When two individuals become dating partners, they agree (explicitly or implicitly) to engage in a romantic relationship that can vary in definition from casual and nonexclusive to exclusive, committed, and directed toward a more serious relational commitment. Compared with marital partners, daters experience significantly more romantic jealousy (Aylor & Dainton, 2001; Guerrero et al., 1993). Aylor and Dainton (2001) found that casual daters experienced cognitive jealousy (i.e., suspicion or worrying about a rival) to a greater degree than did serious daters, but no differences for emotional jealousy experience were observed. Further, the amount of jealousy dating partners experienced did not significantly vary according to whether dating partners viewed the relationship as casual or exclusive (Yareb, Allgeier, & Sensibaugh, 1999). As such, dating partners seem to believe that an expectation of exclusivity is present from the relationship’s onset, regardless of whether or not exclusivity is explicit (Yareb et al., 1999). Romantic jealousy within dating relationships can have both positive and negative implications. Romantic jealousy is positive when it facilitates communication and produces an increase of relational commitment (Bringle, 1995). Conversely, romantic jealousy is negative in that it can lead to violence and homicide in extreme circumstances (Bringle, 1995; Guerrero & Andersen, 1998a). Pines and Aronson (1983) found that individuals believe that there are more negative consequences to romantic jealousy than positive ones. Overall, negative aspects of the relationship typically accompany romantic jealousy between daters. 11 In terms of the communicative expression of romantic jealousy by daters, Aune and Comstock (1991) found that dating partners were more likely to express jealousy to one another than were friends. Further, Guerrero et al. (1993) found that dating partners tended to use negative and avoidant jealousy responses, especially when compared with spouses. More recently, Aylor and Dainton (2001) found that negative affect expression, integrative communication, avoidance/denial, and compensatory restoration (i.e., attempting to make the primary relationship attractive to one’s partner) were the dominant forms of jealousy expression across dating relationships. Bevan and Lanutti (2002) also reported the prevalence of these jealousy responses in romantic relationships (including dating partnerships). However, although much is known about how dating partners experience and express romantic jealousy, how the individual whose dating partner is jealous reacts to that expression remains unclear. Sibling Rivalry and Jealousy Cicirelli and Nussbaum (1989, pp. 283-4) define sibling relationships as “the total of the interactions (actions, verbal, and nonverbal communication) of two or more individuals who share common biological parents, as well as their knowledge, perceptions, attitudes, beliefs, and feelings regarding each other.” The sibling relationship is unique from other close relationships for a number of reasons. Siblings share a common biological heritage, are often committed to maintaining their relationship throughout the life span because of common parentage, and have a long shared history resulting in intimate and common experiences (Cicirelli & Nussbaum, 1989). Compared with cross-sex friendships and dating relationships, sibling relationships are nonvoluntary in nature, often provide unique roles and functions such as role models or socialization 12 agents, and cannot be completely terminated, even in the absence of frequent interaction (Myers et al., 2001). Moreover, siblings have longer and more egalitarian relationships (Cicirelli & Nussbaum, 1989) and provide less psychological, social, and instrumental support (Bedford, 1989) than do cross-sex friends or daters. In terms of similarities, each of these three relational contexts provides feelings of positive affect, association, attachment, and well being (Myers et al., 2001). Further, sibling relationships can have initiation, maintenance, and dissolution phases similar to non-familial relationships; however, these phases develop over the entire life span (Cicirelli, 1985). According to Miller et al. (2000, p. 434), sibling interactions are salient because they “lay the groundwork for how a child responds in future relationships.” The significance of sibling relationships in future relational development is echoed by other family researchers (e.g., Brody, 1998; Fitzpatrick & Badzinski, 1994; Ponzetti & James, 1997). However, despite the significant impact sibling interactions can have in an individual’s relational development, very few family and personal relationship scholars study the sibling relationship (Fingerman & Hay, 2002). To remedy this and acknowledge the importance of siblings in individual’s lives, the current study includes sibling relationships in its examination of jealousy. One sibling dynamic that can play an instrumental role in the type of adult one becomes is sibling rivalry, defined as “competition between siblings for the love, affection, and attention of one or both parents or for other recognition or gain” (Leung & Rosen, 1991, p. 314). Specifically, White and Mullen (1989) note that sibling rivalry could play a role in one’s jealousy in adult romantic relationships and can also be a major factor in the formation of one’s personality. Thus, sibling rivalry has received heavy 13 focus from sibling researchers (Bank & Kahn, 1997), and research on this topic has established rivalry as one of the primary dimensions of sibling interaction. For example, both Furman & Buhrmeister (1985) and Stocker and McHale (1992) determined that rivalry represented one of the primary dimensions or qualities of sibling relationships between the ages of six and 11 years. Further, Gold (1989) considered the extent to which jealousy was present between siblings as an important aspect of sibling relationships when creating five types of elderly sibling relationships. Although rivalry is widely believed to be one of the more dominant aspects of young sibling relationships, researchers do not agree on the extent to which rivalry persists after childhood. Some scholars (Allan, 1977; Mander, 1991) believe that rivalry dissipates as the siblings grow older and move to different geographic locations. Other researchers (Bank & Kahn, 1997; Brody, 1998; Cicirelli, 1982, 1985; Cicirelli & Nussbaum, 1989; Dunn, 1985) contend that rivalry can continue into adulthood, although it is more latent than childhood competition. In fact, Ross and Milgram (1982) found that 71% of adult group interview participants still experienced sibling rivalry, which was most commonly attributed to continued parent favoritism and competitive sibling behavior. In addition, Gold (1989) observed jealousy between elderly siblings that ranged from constructive to aggressive. In terms of the communication of sibling jealousy and rivalry, Furman and Burhmeister (1985) reported a moderate positive correlation between conflict and rivalry. Further, Hetherington (1988) found that one sibling’s rivalry was positively correlated with the other sibling’s avoidance, rivalry, and aggression or coercion. Toddlers and their older siblings tended to use negative affect when expressing jealousy (Miller et al., 2000). 14 Though valuable, the above research findings all involve children younger than 11 years of age. Whether these same communication patterns would extend to adolescent and young adult siblings is unknown. However, researchers hypothesize that adolescent and adulthood rivalry is difficult to communicate and is either avoided (Cicirelli, 1985; Ross & Milgram, 1982) or communicated in a verbally aggressive or violent manner (Raffaelli, 1992). Obviously, inconsistency exists with regard to the extent to which sibling rivalry continues and is communicated by adult siblings. Further, the manner in which sibling rivalry is communicated has escaped empirical scrutiny. To help clarify these issues and to fill what many researchers (e.g., Cicirelli, 1982; Dunn, 1985; Stocker & McHale, 1992) consider to be a void in this research area, the current study intends to examine sibling jealousy in late adolescence and early adulthood. Rivalry and jealousy are very similar concepts, with Bank and Kahn (1997, p. xv) referring to them as “emotional cousins,” and a number of researchers using the terms interchangeably (e.g., Leung & Robson, 1991; Mander, 1991). Indeed, a recent study on sibling jealousy (Miller et al., 2000) found that such an experience could be managed and regulated much like previous research findings on rivalry (Dunn & Kendrick, 1982; Masiuch & Kienapple, 1993). Thus, to be consistent throughout the current project and to allow for comparisons with general jealousy research and the specific study of jealousy in cross-sex friendships and dating relationships, the term sibling jealousy (rather than rivalry) will be utilized exclusively. 15 Jealousy between Cross-Sex Friends A cross-sex friendship is “a specific type of friendship – a nonromantic, nonfamilial, personal relationship between a man and a woman” (O’Meara, 1989, p. 526). However, being nonromantic does not exclude sexuality or passion entirely; instead, the relationship is seen as separate from romantic courtship rites (O’Meara, 1989). Fundamentally, a cross-sex friendship shares many of the same characteristics that all friendships have, including spontaneity, support, acceptance, and understanding (Monsour, 1997). Overall, examining cross-sex friendship as an influential relational context is important for two reasons: (a) adult friendship represents one of the most salient relationships for individuals (Bruess and Pearson, 2000); and (b) very few personal relationship scholars examine friendships (Fingerman & Hay, 2002). Although 8th graders reported that their sibling relationships and cross-sex friendships did not differ in the amount of companionship present (Buhrmeister & Furman, 1987), cross-sex friendships have been found to be closer, more important, and more reciprocal compared with siblings (Pulakos, 1988). Further, friends discuss more topics and share more activities than do siblings (Pulakos, 1988). In addition, unlike sibling relationships (but similar to dating relationships), friendships between men and women introduce the potential for sexual attraction (Afifi & Faulkner, 2000; O’Meara, 1989). Compared to romantic relationships, friendships (both same- and cross-sex) involve less exclusiveness, lower demands on loyalty, and less willingness to help the partner (Davis & Todd, 1985). Same- and cross-sex friends are also less romantically intense, less intimate, and have less sharing, caring, and supportiveness of the partner compared with romantic relationships (Davis & Todd, 1982). Cross-sex friends also 16 disclose less intimate information with one another compared with romantic partners (Patford, 2000). However, friends and romantic partners both are voluntary relationships that involve affection, love, and emotional commitment (Werking, 1997a). Further, both contexts possess similar levels of trust, respect, acceptance, spontaneity, and stability (Davis & Todd, 1982). In general, considerable research (e.g., Afifi & Burgoon, 1998; O’Meara, 1989; Rawlins, 1982) describes cross-sex friendships as being more difficult to maintain and containing greater obstacles to overcome compared with other relational contexts. Specifically, Arnold (1995) notes that cross-sex friends must often deal with issues of relational definition, sexuality, emotional intensity, jealousy, and the way third parties perceive the relationship. These difficult issues can be explained by the scant societal guidelines or role-defined expectations guiding cross-sex friend interactions (O’Meara, 1989). The presence and management of sexuality between cross-sex friends has constituted a large portion of research in this area. However, Kaplan and Keys (1997) suggest that the nature of cross-sex friends’ emotional bond should also be considered. One emotional experience that is relevant to the study of cross-sex platonic relationships is jealousy (Arnold, 1995). Recent research suggests that various types of jealousy are experienced at moderate levels of intensity (Bevan & Samter, 2001) and frequency (Bevan, 1999) in cross-sex friendships. Such jealousy is troublesome for cross-sex friends: Females reported that being jealous of other people was the most costly aspect of their relationships, with males indicating that jealousy was second most costly to them (Beske & Buss, 2000). 17 Jealousy expression is also problematic for cross-sex friends. For example, Aune and Comstock (1991) reported that friends (same- and cross-sex) communicated jealousy to a significantly lesser degree than romantic partners did. Moreover, although integrative communication was most frequently used across the entire sample, Bevan (1999) found that participants with romantic interest in their cross-sex friends were more likely to avoid communicating romantic jealousy to the friends than those who were unsure of or without romantic interest. Thus, there seems to be difficulty in expressing jealousy between cross-sex friends. To learn more about this issue, and about the general management of jealousy in both romantic and non-romantic associations, cross-sex friendships, dating relationships, and sibling relationships will serve as frameworks from which to examine how individuals react to others’ direct communication of jealousy to them. Proposed Consequences of Another’s Jealousy Expression Four consequences seem possible when another expresses jealousy to an individual: general partner uncertainty, relational uncertainty, jealousy-related emotion, and rumination. Descriptions of these consequences and their relation to specific jealousy expression types and relational contexts are presented below. General Partner and Relational Uncertainty The first proposed consequence of partner jealousy expression is the presence of uncertainty. The concept of uncertainty has arisen from Berger and Calabrese’s (1975) Uncertainty Reduction Theory (URT), which has been useful to communication scholars in focusing attention to “the role of uncertainty in various communication situations and to practical concerns with how individuals manage uncertainty in problematic situations” 18 (Goldsmith, 2001, p. 514). Scholars agree that uncertainty is a basic human experience that must be managed in a variety of situations and relational contexts (Baxter & Montgomery, 1996; Bradac, 2001; Brashers, 2001; Goldsmith, 2001). Though the construct of uncertainty is most frequently associated with new relationships, uncertainty levels remain salient in established relationships as well (Berger, 1987; Knobloch & Solomon, 2002b). Indeed, Prisbell (1999) reported that friends and romantic partners experienced greater amounts of uncertainty compared with casual acquaintances and business relationships. Further, because the negotiation of uncertainty within close relational contexts is an important aspect of relational health (Knobloch & Solomon, 2002b), it is an area worthy of study. Goldsmith (2001) criticizes URT’s failure to address the role of communication quality in uncertainty processes. To remedy this theoretical oversight, the current project examines the quality of another’s jealousy expression (i.e., whether it is seen as positive, negative, or neutral) as a predictor or antecedent of an individual’s experience of uncertainty. Generally, uncertainty is present when “when details of situations are ambiguous, complex, unpredictable, or probabilistic; when information is unavailable or inconsistent; and when people feel insecure in their own state of knowledge or the state of knowledge in general” (Brashers, 2001, p. 478). Uncertainty can be divided into two broad categories: general and relational. General uncertainty, which exists when there is little knowledge about one’s attitudes, beliefs, feelings, and behavior, can further be divided into self and partner uncertainty (Berger & Bradac, 1982). Knobloch and Solomon (1999, p. 262) define self uncertainty as occurring when individuals are unable “to describe, predict, or explain their own attitudes or behavior” and partner uncertainty as being 19 present when there is “an inability to predict the other person’s attitudes and behaviors within interaction.” On the other hand, relational uncertainty is defined as “doubt about the status of the relationship apart from either self or partner” (Knobloch & Solomon, 1999, p. 262). Thus, the fundamental difference between general uncertainty and relational uncertainty is that the latter focuses on the relationship as a unit, meaning that relational uncertainty exists at a higher order of abstraction than general uncertainty (Berger & Bradac, 1982). General partner uncertainty and relational uncertainty will be focused upon and compared in the present investigation. Although Knobloch and Solomon (1999) criticized uncertainty research’s focus on general partner uncertainty, this type of uncertainty seems germane to the situation of interest. Specifically, general partner uncertainty should be primary to an individual when considering his or her partner’s direct expression of jealousy. Further, uncertainty about the relationship should also be considered after another communicates his or her jealousy to an individual because such a situation may call the quality of the relationship into question. This focus on a situation where one’s partner expresses jealousy is consistent with recent research (e.g., Afifi & Metts, 1998; Knobloch & Solomon, 2002a) that has examined specific uncertainty-evoking events within close relationships. The relationship between uncertainty and jealousy has received robust support. Smith and Clanton (1983) describe this association by stating that jealousy can lead to the inability to predict one’s own behavior, or one’s relationship, including the relationship’s future, how the partner will generally behave, and the partner’s specific reaction to jealousy expression. In terms of empirical research findings, Afifi and Reichert (1996) 20 reported that a positive relationship between jealousy experience and relational state uncertainty existed for dating partners. Further, according to Emmers and Canary (1996), jealousy represented a situation that elicited moderate amounts of uncertainty in romantic relationships. Relatedly, Planalp and Honeycutt (Planalp & Honeycutt, 1985; Planalp, Rutherford, & Honeycutt, 1988) observed that competing relationships, including romantic infidelity and new friendships, were uncertainty-increasing events for friends and romantic partners. Although Prisbell (1999) found that friends and intimates did not differ from one another in their levels of general partner uncertainty, differences in relational uncertainty levels across relational contexts have not yet been assessed. In the current investigation where jealousy is the specific uncertainty-increasing event, differences in general partner and relational uncertainty between the three relational contexts are expected. Specifically, cross-sex friendships should logically possess the greatest uncertainty, followed by dating relationships, with sibling relationships associated with the lowest uncertainty levels. Cross-sex friendships are often defined by their ambiguity (Rawlins, 1982), and the voluntary nature of this relational context suggests that friends – in response to another’s jealousy expression – may be more uncertain about the other and insecure about the continuation of the relationship compared with siblings and daters. For example, Afifi and Burgoon (1998) observed greater levels of general partner and relational state uncertainty in cross-sex friendships than in dating relationships. Because dating relationships are characterized by high-to-moderate levels of relational and partner uncertainty (e.g., Knobloch & Solomon, 2002a; Knobloch et al., 2001; Parks & Adelman, 1983), and are by no means certain or permanent, they should be associated with greater 21 partner and relational uncertainty compared with siblings. Further, siblings will likely have dealt with their own and their sibling’s jealousy in their youth (Hill & Watts, 2000; Leung & Robson, 1991) and discovering one’s sibling is jealous of them should not be an uncertainty-arousing event. Similar predictions for both general partner and relational uncertainty are forwarded because previous research found no significant differences between the two uncertainty types in their influence on jealousy experience and expression (Afifi & Reichert, 1996). As such, the first two hypotheses predict significant differences between each of the three relational contexts in the following order: H1: Individuals will experience the greatest levels of general partner uncertainty when jealousy is expressed within relational contexts in the following order: (a) cross-sex friend, (b) dating partner, (c) sibling. H2: Individuals will experience the greatest levels of relational uncertainty when jealousy is expressed within relational contexts in the following order: (a) cross-sex friend, (b) dating partner, (c) sibling. The method of jealousy expression chosen by another should also be associated with an individual’s level of general partner and relational uncertainty. From the three modes of jealousy expression examined in the present study, the other’s use of distributive communication seems more likely to be associated with higher levels of general partner and relational uncertainty than either negative affect expression or integrative communication. Distributive communication has been classified as negative because it is typically destructive and damaging to one’s relationship (Guerrero & Andersen, 1998b), and such a negative form of jealousy expression from another is likely 22 to make an individual question aspects of that person and of the relationship. Moreover, previous research has found that the presence of general partner and relational state uncertainty were both negatively associated with the use of integrative communication in romantic relationships (Afifi & Reichert, 1996). In a similar vein, the goal of reducing uncertainty about one’s primary romantic relationship was positively associated with his or her use of integrative communication and negative affect expression (Guerrero & Afifi, 1999). In terms of the current project, greater levels of an individual’s general partner and relational uncertainty should result from another’s use of distributive communication to express jealousy compared with uncertainty levels resulting from another’s use of integrative communication or negative affect expression. Because the meaning of negative affect expression could change depending upon the jealousy interaction (Andersen et al., 1995; Guerrero et al., 1995), such jealousy expression from another person should be related to individuals’ uncertainty levels that fall between those associated with distributive and integrative communication. Finally, integrative communication from another should be associated with the lowest levels of uncertainty within an individual, as constructive, open communication from the other is likely to abate any general partner and relational uncertainty one might feel after the interaction. Hypotheses three and four formally predict significant differences between each of the jealousy expression strategies in the following order: 23 H3: Individuals will experience the greatest levels of general partner uncertainty according to jealousy expression type in the following order: (a) distributive communication, (b) negative affect expression, (c) integrative communication. H4: Individuals will experience the greatest levels of relational uncertainty according to jealousy expression type in the following order: (a) distributive communication, (b) negative affect expression, (c) integrative communication. Jealousy-Related Emotions The second proposed consequence of another’s jealousy expression is the elicitation of an individual’s jealousy-related emotions. Jealousy is a compound emotion or affect blend, involving an “aggregate of primary emotions” (Hupka, 1984, p. 142). These emotions, which are primarily negative, can include anxiety, fear, insecurity, anger, sadness, envy, guilt, sexual arousal, and frustration (Bringle, 1991; White & Mullen, 1989; Zammuner & Fischer, 1995). Pfeiffer and Wong’s (1989) widely used measure of emotional jealousy includes the emotions of envy, anxiety, discomfort, anger, jealousy, fear, insecurity, worry, and upset. These eight emotions consistently combine to operationalize jealousy emotion. Emotional reaction is thus one of the primary aspects of one’s own experience of jealousy (e.g., Pfeiffer & Wong, 1989), and it is assumed that similar emotions could arise in an individual when another expresses jealousy to him or her. For the purposes of this study, logical anecdotal evidence suggests that seven emotions are relevant and particularly likely as one’s emotional reaction to another’s jealousy expression: sadness, 24 anger, frustration, guilt, fear, insecurity, and surprise. Further, these emotions will be assessed as a single jealousy-related emotion construct because previous research has determined that they share certain cognitive appraisals used to interpret specific situations. Namely, Ellsworth and Smith (1988) found that the emotional experiences of anger, sadness, fear, guilt, and surprise did not differ according to the cognitive appraisals of certainty (which should be used to appraise the current jealousy expression situation) and attentional activity. Further, the unpleasant emotions of anger, fear, guilt, and sadness combined with at least one other unpleasant emotion more than 25% of the time that they were experienced (Ellsworth & Smith, 1988). In terms of comparisons between relational contexts, research has not provided a clear-cut prediction as to whether cross-sex friends, dating partners, or siblings will experience more intense emotions in relation to jealousy expression from another. Crosssex friends may have intense emotional reactions because they are not used to direct jealousy expression within their relationships (e.g., Afifi & Burgoon, 1998). Dating partners may possess strong emotions because a rival threatens the relationship that the partners may have agreed was exclusive in nature (e.g., Ellis & Weinstein, 1986). Conversely, siblings may experience strong jealous affect because they may have had to deal with unresolved jealousy within their relationship throughout their lives (Leung & Robson, 1991). As such, hypothesis five predicts that one’s intensity of jealousy-related emotion will differ according to the relational context in which jealousy expression occurs: 25 H5: An individual’s intensity of jealousy-related emotion will differ according to relational context (i.e., whether one’s cross-sex friend, dating partner or sibling expresses jealousy). Unlike relational context and jealousy-related emotions, the relationship between jealousy-related emotion and jealousy expression is clearer. Guerrero et al. (1995) found that distributive communication and negative affect expression are both strongly and positively related to emotional jealousy. Integrative communication also shared a weak, but significant, positive relationship with emotional jealousy. Further, negative affect expression, which inherently involves the expression of emotion, served as a strong predictor of emotional jealousy (Guerrero et al., 1995). As such, the sixth hypothesis predicts that another’s use of negative affect expression and distributive communication will both be more strongly related to an individual’s experience of jealousy-related emotions than will integrative communication: H6: Another’s use of distributive communication or negative affect expression will lead to more intense jealousy-related emotions than will another’s use of integrative communication. Rumination The final proposed consequence of jealousy expression from another is an individual’s rumination, or mulling, about the interaction. Thoughts become ruminative when they automatically and unexpectedly control our awareness to a noticeable or bothersome extent (Gold & Wegner, 1995). Specifically, ruminations are “conscious thinking directed toward a given object for an extended period of time” and are said to occur as the result of a traumatic or unpleasant situation (Gold & Wegner, 1995, p 1246). 26 Rumination may sometimes be useful in coping with stress and achieving goals, but more often takes up attentional capacity and interferes with something an individual seeks to achieve (Koole, Smeets, van Knippenberg, & Dijksterhuis, 1999). Martin and Tesser (1989) highlight three important features of rumination: (1) it occurs over relatively long time periods, potentially involving years and a great deal of energy; (2) it is counterproductive because it often happens after the individual’s best chance to discover or work out a solution has passed; and (3) it involves both automatic and controlled processes (such as adapting to a changed situation, considering alternatives, and implementing problem-solving behaviors) and unwanted, intrusive thoughts that occur without consciousness and are difficult to suppress. Though generally negative, ruminations may be a part of humans’ need for meaning within their lives, specifically with relation to the event causing the rumination. In other words, ruminations can serve as “a way to attempt to find answers to life’s difficult questions” (Gold & Wegner, 1995, p. 1250). In general, ruminations are often caused by distressing events, the frustration of one’s goals or plans, the choice to not share an event or information with others, and attempting to not think about the issue (Gold & Wegner, 1995). Overall, rumination is an important area of study because such cognitive activity is unintentional, difficult to eliminate, potentially long lasting and dominant to an individual’s thinking, and is related to other cognitions, emotions, and behaviors (Martin & Tesser, 1996a). Research has examined rumination in relation to negative experiences and events. For example, Johnson and Roloff (1998) report that mulling about a serial argument occurred at a moderate frequency and was negatively related to the belief that the 27 argument could be resolved. Further, Cloven and Roloff (1991) found that mulling about a major problem in one’s relationship increased perceptions of the seriousness of the issue. In addition, McCullough and his colleagues found that rumination was positively associated with feelings of vengefulness, negative affectivity, and revenge (McCullough, Bellah, Kilpatrick, & Johnson, 2001; McCullough et al., 1998). To provide a cohesive theoretical explanation for rumination processes, Martin and Tesser (1989) proposed a theory of goal-driven rumination. This theory assumes that thoughts and actions are goal-directed and one’s inability to attain a certain goal will stimulate rumination. Such rumination will be present until the individual either reaches his or her frustrated goal or disengages from it (Martin & Tesser, 1989). The first two assumptions of the theory are that people’s thoughts and actions are goal-directed, and that these goals are hierarchically structured. The third assumption is that individuals proceed through a relatively specific behavior sequence after an important goal is frustrated. This sequence includes repetition, problem solving, end-state thinking, negotiation, and learned helplessness. The third assumption also states that rumination involves either attempting to discover alternatives to attaining frustrated goals or realizing that the attempt to attain these goals has failed. Ideally, however, rumination ceases when the frustrated goal is reached (Koole et al., 1999; Martin & Tesser, 1989). Regarding the specific link between jealousy and rumination, Carson and Cupach (2000) note that believing that there is a rival for one’s partner can be a perceived threat to the ongoing goal of relationship continuation. As such, jealousy about a rival can stimulate ruminative thoughts. Once jealousy occurs, “rumination reflects uncomfortable mulling about the security of the relationship” and is distressing, aversive, and self- 28 perpetuating (Carson & Cupach, 2000, p. 322). Such a relationship between jealousy and rumination is consistent with Martin and Tesser’s (1989) theory of goal-driven rumination because it implicates jealousy as an example of a frustrated goal within romantic relationships. Extending the above reasoning to the current project and in accordance with the theory of goal-driven rumination, it also seems viable that situations where one’s relational partner expresses that he or she is jealous can be an unpleasant event that threatens that individual’s goal of maintaining a satisfying relationship. More specifically, it is likely that a cross-sex friend’s expression of jealousy will be related to greater rumination compared with a sibling’s or dating partner’s communication of jealous feelings. Because discussion of issues such as jealousy are often avoided by cross-sex friends (Afifi & Burgoon, 1998; Aune & Comstock, 1991; Bevan, 1999), when one’s cross-sex friend does directly express jealousy, it will likely be an uncomfortable and distressing event that the individual will ruminate about. In contrast, siblings and dating partners will likely be more acquainted with jealousy expression within their relationship and thus may not need to mull about the event to the same degree as crosssex friends. Thus, it is assumed that siblings and dating partners will not significantly differ in their amount of rumination, but that rumination levels for both contexts will be significantly lower than will those for cross-sex friends. The seventh hypothesis considers this relationship: H7: Expressions of jealousy from an individual’s cross-sex friend will lead to more rumination than expressions of jealousy from an individual’s sibling or dating partner. 29 In addition to the negative psychological and relational variables discussed above, a number of communication tactics are also related to rumination. For example, Cloven and Roloff (1991) found that distributive communication served to exacerbate the repercussions of mulling about a major issue, whereas integrative communication alleviated perceptions of problem seriousness. Further, Carson and Cupach (2000) reported that ruminating about jealousy strongly predicted the usage of a variety of negative techniques for expressing jealousy, including distributive communication, avoidance/denial, possessiveness, and manipulation. Rumination also strongly predicted one’s usage of negative affect expression to communicate jealousy (Carson & Cupach, 2000). Combining this finding with McCullough et al.’s (2001) observation that rumination is positively associated with negative affectivity, it appears as if rumination and negative affect expression should share a relationship as strong as the association between distributive communication and rumination. In contrast, no relationship was observed between jealousy rumination and integrative communication (Carson & Cupach, 2000). The above findings thus suggest that another’s use of either distributive communication or negative affect expression will be associated with more rumination within an individual compared with another who expresses jealousy by way of integrative communication. As such, distributive communication and negative affect expression are not predicted to significantly differ from one another, but both are expected to be associated with higher rumination levels compared with integrative communication. Hypothesis eight thus posits: 30 H8: An individual will ruminate more if another expresses jealousy using distributive communication or negative affect expression compared to integrative communication. The Proposed Reaction Model of Jealousy One final consideration is the relationships between each of the proposed cognitive and emotional consequences of jealousy expression. Examining these associations allows for the creation of a model detailing the specific relationships among each of these jealousy expression consequences that is both theoretically driven and empirically supported. Testing and refining a reaction jealousy expression model will hopefully provide greater theoretical insight into the consequences of jealousy expression in close relationships. General partner uncertainty is presumed to be the first reaction to another’s expression of jealousy, followed by relational uncertainty. This prediction is based upon Knobloch et al. (2001) and Knobloch and Solomon (2000), who determined that partner uncertainty preceded relational uncertainty in four separate structural equation models. Relational uncertainty is then proposed to lead to both jealousy-related emotions and rumination about the situation. Knobloch and Solomon (2000) reported that relational uncertainty was directly and positively related to the emotions of anger, sadness, and fear. Further, relational uncertainty mediated the relationship between partner uncertainty and each of these three emotions (Knobloch & Solomon, 2000). Knobloch et al. (2001) also determined that emotional jealousy was related to relational uncertainty (though this relationship was mediated by cognitive jealousy). 31 In terms of rumination, Carson and Cupach (2000) predicted a positive association between uncertainty and rumination. Specifically, they stated that an individual will “worry excessively about potential treats to the relationship,” implying that uncertainty will precede ruminative thoughts (Carson & Cupach, 2000, p. 314). Further, although inconsistency exists as to whether or not negative affect (i.e., jealousyrelated emotion) and rumination covary (e.g., Klinger, 1996; Martin & Tesser, 1996a, 1996b) or if rumination leads to the experience of negative affect (e.g., McIntosh & Martin, 1992), the bulk of this research suggests that these variables are merely associated with one another. As such, it is proposed that jealousy-related emotion and rumination are positively related. Although each of these research findings or suggestions are made with regard to the person experiencing jealousy, the causal structure of these variables can also logically apply to the jealous target as well. The final hypothesis and the figure below detail this proposed model (see Figure 1): H9: Upon a close partner’s expression of jealousy, the causal structure of jealousy expression consequence variables is as follows: General partner uncertainty will precede relational uncertainty, which then leads to both jealousy-related emotions and rumination about the situation. 32 (+) (+) Close Partner’s Expression of Jealousy General Partner Uncertainty Jealousy-Related Emotion Relational Uncertainty (+) (+) Ruminative Thoughts Figure 1. Proposed path-analytic reaction model of jealousy. 33 CHAPTER 2 METHODOLOGY This chapter will describe the manner in which the dissertation’s eight proposed hypotheses were tested. First, the purpose, procedures, and results of two pilot studies intended to develop and refine the hypothetical scenarios and jealousy messages are discussed. Next, the main investigation’s participants and research design are detailed. Finally, the procedure for data collection, and the operationalization and measurement of the independent and dependent variables in the main investigation are presented. Pilot Studies Pilot Study I Purpose and participants. A pilot study was conducted with two distinct purposes: (a) to determine if each of the three relational context conditions and the final three jealousy strategies possessed statistically equivalent levels of realism and occurred at similar frequencies in participants’ own relationships; and (b) to verify that the distributive, integrative, and negative affect expression strategies were seen by participants as negative, positive, and neutral in nature, respectively. Sixty-seven students taking speech communication classes at a large, southern university comprised the sample for the pilot study. Sixty three percent of these respondents were female (n = 42). Eighty eight percent were seniors (n = 59; with 5 juniors, 1 sophomore, 1 graduate student, and 1 university employee) with a mean age of 22.4 years (SD = 2.14, range = 20 to 32 years). Finally, 87% of the sample classified 34 themselves as White (n = 58), 6% each indicated that they were either Black/African American or a part of the other category (n = 4), and one individual was Asian. Procedures. Respondents completed a questionnaire for a class research requirement. Participants received one of three relational context conditions: the sibling condition (n = 25), the cross-sex friend condition (n = 24), or the dating partner condition (n = 18). After reading and signing consent forms, questionnaires were randomly distributed to respondents, who were asked to read the definition of the relational context before them and raise their hands if they were not currently involved in the relational context condition that they received. Only four participants did so (all had received the dating condition) and they were then provided with relational contexts in which they were currently involved. Participants then wrote down the initials of their relational partner. Next, they read a scenario detailing an instance where their relational partners made it clear to them that they were jealous. Participants were again asked to write down their relational partner’s initials as they were reading the scenario. The directions then asked the participants to carefully consider the situation and imagine that it had actually occurred in their own relationship. Next, participants answered three questions previously used by Bevan (1999) about the realism of the relational context scenario on a 7-point, Likert-type scale with higher values indicating more realism (e.g., “This situation is easy to imagine”). Participants also responded to a three item, 7-point, Likert-type scale from Bevan (1999) about how frequently such a situation occurred in their own lives (e.g., “I have often experienced a situation like this one in my cross-sex friendship/dating/sibling 35 relationship”), with higher values indicating more frequency (see Appendix A for a full version of pilot study I). All scenarios used identical wording when operationalizing the jealousy message. Because no known attempts have been made to utilize specific jealousy messages in hypothetical scenarios, the exact wording for each jealousy expression condition was developed by the author using Guerrero et al.’s (1995) and Guerrero and Andersen’s (1998b) jealousy expression definitions and examples. For example, Guerrero and Andersen’s (1998b, p. 171) definition states that distributive communication consists of “direct, aggressive communication about jealousy with the partner,” examples of which include “accusing the partner of being unfaithful, being sarcastic or rude toward the partner, arguing with the partner, bringing up the issue over and over again to ‘bombard’ the partner.” From this information, the following distributive jealousy message was constructed: Your dating partner/sibling/cross-sex friend communicates this jealousy to you by accusing you, making rude comments, and arguing with you (see Appendix B for the original definitions, examples, and the subsequent jealousy messages that were created). Jealousy messages were written for Guerrero et al.’s (1995) six interactive jealousy responses (i.e., integrative communication, distributive communication, negative affect expression, avoidance/denial, active distancing, and violent communication/threats) so that each could be pilot-tested and three jealousy strategies could be selected that best represent positive, neutral, and negative messages. Upon answering the six realism and frequency items for the relational context scenario, participants then encountered each of Guerrero et al.’s (1995) six interactive jealousy messages in randomized order. Specifically, participants were asked to consider 36 each message as something their relational partners might use to communicate jealousy. After reading each message, respondents answered the same nine items. One semantic differential item assessed how frequently each message was actually used by the participants’ relational partners (1 = does not happen to me at all; 7 = happens to me a lot). Two semantic differential items measured the realism of the jealousy message (e.g., 1 = not at all realistic; 7 = completely realistic). Five semantic differential items assessed how positive, negative, or neutral each jealousy message was (e.g., 1 = completely negative, good, pleasant; 7 = not at all negative, bad, unpleasant) and were recoded such that higher values indicate more positive valence. Finally, the respondents answered a number of demographic items, turned in the instrument, were thanked, and provided with a debriefing sheet. Pilot study I results: Realism and frequency of the relational context scenarios. For all analyses, a probability level for Type I errors of .05 was used. The three realism items for the relational context scenario displayed reliability for each condition and overall (sibling = .89; dating partner = .95; cross-sex friend = .93; overall = .92) and a realism composite variable was created. Using ANOVA with the realism composite measure as the dependent variable and relational context as the fixed factor, realism levels (cross-sex friend M = 3.83, SD = 1.46; dating partner M = 3.56, SD = 2.14; sibling M = 3.12, SD = 1.72) did not significantly differ across conditions [F (2, 64) = 1.02, p = .37, power = .22] (see Table 1). The three frequency of occurrence items were reliable within and across relational contexts (sibling = .89; dating partner = .89; cross-sex friend = .84; overall = .87) and a frequency of occurrence composite variable was computed. Results of an 37 ANOVA with relational context as the fixed factor and frequency of actual occurrence as the dependent variable indicated that frequency levels (cross-sex friend M = 2.19, SD = 1.29; dating partner M = 1.91, SD = 1.26; sibling M = 1.96, SD = 1.39) did not significantly differ across the three conditions [F (2, 64) = .298, p = .74, power = .10]. Thus, the results for realism and frequency in terms of the jealousy scenarios provide preliminary evidence that each of the three relational contexts under investigation display fairly equal levels of realism and frequency of occurrence in participants’ own lives. Overall, participants found the jealousy scenarios to be moderately realistic (M = 3.45, SD = 1.76); however, this jealousy did not occur frequently in their own lives (M = 2.03, SD = 1.31). Pilot study I results: Valence of the jealousy messages. The five-item valence scale displayed strong internal consistency for all six jealousy messages that were tested (integrative communication = .92; violent communication/threats = .95; avoidance/denial = .88; active distancing = .92; distributive communication = .95; negative affect expression = .90). As such, six separate composite valence variables for each type of jealousy expression were computed for analysis. Integrative communication was perceived as the most positive jealousy message (M = 5.36, SD = 1.42). Negative 38 Table 1 Realism, Frequency of Occurrence, and Ever Occurred Variable Means and Standard Deviations for Relational Context Scenarios Examined in Pilot I and Pilot II Jealousy Scenario Realism Pilot I Pilot II Frequency Pilot I Pilot II Ever Occurred Pilot II Sibling 3.12 (1.72) 5.93 (.997) 1.96 (1.39) 4.04 (1.68) 4.31 (2.14) Cross-Sex Friend 3.83 (1.46) 5.29 (1.33) 2.19 (1.29) 2.63 (1.35) 2.63 (1.35) Dating Partner 3.56 (2.14) 5.71 (1.33) 1.91 (1.26) 3.45 (1.56) 4.14 (2.01) Overall 3.45 5.65 2.03 3.40 3.69 (1.76) (1.19) (1.31) (1.59) (1.83) Note. N = 67 for Pilot I; N = 24 for Pilot II. There were no significant differences at the .05 level for Pilot I and at the .10 level for Pilot II. Values in parentheses are standard deviations. The “ever occurred” variable was only employed in Pilot Study II. 39 affect expression was viewed as neutral (M = 3.43, SD = 1.24), whereas avoidance/denial (M = 2.82. SD = 1.19) and active distancing (M = 2.36, SD = 1.33) were both perceived as being between neutral and negative in valence. Distributive communication (M = 2.19, SD = 1.35) and violent communication/threats (M = 1.80, SD = 1.38) were each viewed as jealousy messages that were more negative in nature (see Table 2). Visual examination of the valence means suggested that integrative communication and negative affect expression are the strongest candidates to represent the positive and neutral jealousy messages in the main investigation. To further explore this possibility, a series of one-sample t-tests were computed. Results indicated that integrative communication was significantly more positive than the other five jealousy messages [violent communication threats t (65) = -20.89, p < .0001; avoidance/denial t (66) = -17.39, p < .0001; active distancing t (66) = -18.45, p < .0001; distributive communication t (66) = -19.28, p < .0001; negative affect expression t (66) = -12.64, p < .0001]. Negative affect expression was significantly more neutral than any other jealousy message [violent communication threats t (65) = -9.59, p < .0001; avoidance/denial t (66) = -4.22, p < .0001; active distancing t (66) = -6.60, p < .0001; distributive communication t (66) = -7.59, p < .0001; integrative communication t (66) = 11.06, p < .0001]. Next, visual inspection of the valence means suggests that either the distributive communication or violent communication/threats messages would be likely candidates to represent the negative jealousy expression condition in the main study. A series of onesample t-tests revealed that the distributive communication message was significantly more negative than three of the jealousy messages [avoidance/denial t (66) = 4.33, p < .0001; negative affect expression t (66) = 8.21, p < .0001; integrative communication t 40 (66) = 18.24, p < .0001] and did not significantly differ in valence from the active distancing message [t (66) = 1.09, p = .28]. However, distributive communication was significantly less negative than the violent communication/threat message [t (65) = -2.25, p < .05]. Further, results suggested that all five jealousy messages were significantly less negative than the violent communication/threat message [negative affect expression t (65) = 10.72, p < .0001; avoidance/denial t (66) = 6.94, p < .0001; active distancing t (66) = 3.45, p < .01; distributive communication t (66) = 3.32, p < .05; integrative communication t (66) = 20.44, p < .0001]. Pilot study I results: Realism of the jealousy messages. The two-item realism scale displayed acceptable internal consistency for all six jealousy messages that were tested (integrative communication = .76; violent communication/threats = .76; avoidance/denial = .89; active distancing = .87; distributive communication = .90; negative affect expression = .76). Realism composite variables for each jealousy expression strategy were thus created for analysis. Integrative communication was perceived as the most realistic jealousy message (M = 4.45, SD = 1.74). The avoidance/denial (M = 3.45, SD = 1.95), active distancing (M = 2.84, SD = 1.84), negative affect expression (M = 3.04, SD = 1.60), and distributive communication (M = 3.13, SD = 1.90) messages fall at the approximate midpoint of the realism scale and the violent communication/threat message appears to be less realistic to participants (M = 1.67, SD = 1.16; see Table 2). To further examine the potential differences in realism, a series of one-sample ttests were conducted. Specifically, integrative communication and negative affect expression were focused upon because they would likely serve as the positive and neutral 41 jealousy messages in the main study. Results indicated that the integrative jealousy message was judged by participants as more realistic than the other five jealousy messages [negative affect expression t (65) = -7.25, p < .0001; avoidance/denial t (66) = 4.20, p < .0001; active distancing t (66) = -7.14, p < .0001; distributive communication t (66) = -5.72, p < .0001; violent communication/threats t (65) = -19.54, p < .0001]. The negative affect expression message was not significantly different in realism from the distributive communication [t (66) = .39, p = .70], active distancing [t (66) = -.86, p = .39], or avoidance/denial [t (66) = 1.74, p = .09] messages. However, it was significantly less realistic than the integrative message [t (66) = 6.65, p < .0001] and significantly more realistic than the violent communication/threat message [t (65) = -9.60, p < .0001]. Based upon the results for message valence, the violent communication/threat message seemed the most likely choice for the negative jealousy message. However, the results of a series of one-sample t-tests indicated that the violent communication/threat message was significantly less realistic than the remaining five messages [integrative communication t (66) = 13.07, p < .0001; avoidance/denial t (66) = 7.48, p < .0001; active distancing t (66) = 5.21, p < .0001; distributive communication t (66) = 6.29, p < .0001; negative affect expression t (66) = 7.01, p < .0001]. Thus, the realism score for distributive communication was next tested against the other five jealousy messages. The distributive communication message did not significantly differ in realism from the 42 Table 2 Valence and Realism Variable Means and Standard Deviations for Jealousy Expression Types Examined in Pilot I Jealousy Expression Type Valence Mean SD Realism Mean SD Integrative Communication 5.36 1.42 4.45 1.74 Negative Affect Expression 3.43 1.24 3.04 1.60 Distributive Communication 2.19 1.35 3.13 1.90 Violent Communication/Threats 1.80 1.38 1.67 1.16 Active Distancing 2.36 1.33 2.84 1.84 Avoidance/Denial 2.82 1.19 3.45 1.95 Note. N = 67. 43 negative affect expression [t (66) = -.46, p = .65], active distancing [t (66) = -1.26, p = .21], or avoidance/denial [t (66) = 1.36, p = .18] messages. Further, the distributive message was significantly less realistic than the integrative message [t (66) = 6.23, p < .0001] and significantly more realistic than the violent communication/threat message [t (65) = -10.23, p < .0001]. Summary. To summarize, the first pilot study sought to confirm that the relational context scenarios and jealousy messages created for the main investigation were not viewed as having different levels of realism and frequency of actual occurrence. Results showed that each relational context scenario did display statistically equivalent levels of realism and frequency. Second, the pilot study examined which interactive jealousy messages were viewed as positive, neutral, and negative in nature. Results for message valence indicated that the integrative message was the most positive method of expressing jealousy, negative affect expression best represented neutral jealousy expression, and violent communication/threats was viewed as the most negative way to express jealousy. To ensure that the jealousy messages selected were both clearly positive, negative, or neutral, and at least moderately realistic to participants, the message valence results were compared to the results for message realism. For the neutral method of jealousy expression, the negative affect expression message was chosen for two reasons. First, the message was significantly more neutral (i.e., fell almost exactly at the scale’s midpoint and was significantly different from all other jealousy messages in terms of valence) than the other jealousy messages. Second, the message shared similar realism levels with a number of other jealousy messages. 44 Integrative communication was significantly more realistic than the other jealousy messages, whereas the violent communication/threat message was significantly less realistic than the remaining messages. Though the optimal situation is obviously one where each of the three messages possesses statistically equivalent levels of realism, the finding for integrative communication is somewhat expected and logical. Integrative communication has repeatedly been reported as the most frequent method individuals use to express jealousy (e.g., Bevan, 1999; Bevan & Lannutti, 2002; Guerrero et al., 1995). Further, because of its positive nature, it is likely that participants are best able to imagine it occurring in their own close relationships. Due to these explanations, and because it so clearly represented the most positive jealousy response, integrative communication was selected as the positive jealousy message. According to pilot study results, the violent communication/threat message was significantly less realistic than all other jealousy messages. Further, violent communication/threats occur very infrequently in close relationships (e.g., Bevan & Samter, 2001; Guerrero et al., 1995; Guerrero & Andersen, 1998a) and participants obviously had a difficult time envisioning their relational partners expressing jealousy in that manner. As such, distributive communication was instead chosen as the negative jealousy message, as it occurs more frequently than violent communication/threats (Guerrero et al., 1995) but is also considered to be a negative choice when expressing jealousy (Guerrero & Andersen, 1998b). Further, the distributive communication message was judged as more negative than both the integrative and negative affect expression messages and was viewed to be as realistic as the negative affect expression message (and more realistic than the violent communication/threats message). 45 Pilot Study II Purpose. Though the results of the first pilot study were generally useful and anticipated, one surprising and potentially problematic finding did emerge: The relational context scenarios were judged to be only moderately realistic and very infrequent in participants’ own relationships. A second pilot study was thus conducted to test improved relational context scenarios that participants would view as easier to imagine and more frequent than the scenarios in the first pilot study. First, more details were added to the scenarios with an introductory sentence that describes more about the situation (e.g., for the sibling jealousy scenario, the sentence “Your parents have recently been focusing on you and giving you a lot of attention” was added). This change allowed participants to receive more background about why their relational partners were upset. Next, the wording “Your cross-sex friend/dating partner/sibling approaches you and tells you that he/she is jealous. . .” was changed to “By the way your cross-sex friend/dating partner/sibling is acting, it is obvious that he/she is hurt and bothered by. . .” This alteration broadened the possible ways in which relational partners could communicate jealousy (i.e., the partner can behave in a certain way in front of the participant but does not necessarily have to tell the participant his or her feelings outright). Further, in accordance with Wiederman, Allgeier, and Ragusa (1995), the word “jealous” was replaced with “upset and bothered by.” These authors recommended using the word upset instead of jealous because being jealous may have a socially undesirable connotation and because “the term ‘upset’ may be construed as a general umbrella term under which more specific emotional labels can be subsumed” (Wiederman et al., 1995, p. 25). 46 Participants and procedure. The sample for the second pilot study consisted of 24 undergraduates taking speech communication classes at a large, southern university. Forty-six percent of these respondents were female (n = 11). Fifty-eight percent were either seniors or 5th year seniors (n = 14; with 4 juniors, 3 sophomores, and 1 freshmen) with a mean age of 20.7 years (SD = 1.16, range = 18 to 23 years). Finally, 91% of the sample classified themselves as White (n = 20), with 4% each indicating that they were either Black (n = 1) or Asian (n = 1). Two individuals did not report their demographic characteristics. Respondents voluntarily completed one of three survey versions during class time. As with the first pilot study, three relational context conditions were studied: the sibling condition (n = 9), the cross-sex friend condition (n = 8), and the dating partner condition (n = 7). Questionnaires were randomly distributed to participants after they had read and signed consent forms and asked respondents to read a relational context definition and raise their hands if they were not currently involved in that specific relationship. Only two participants did so (one each for the dating and cross-sex friend conditions); each were then provided with relational contexts in which they were currently involved. Participants next wrote down the initials of their relational partner, read one of the modified scenarios described above, and were again asked to write down their relational partner’s initials while reading the scenario. The directions following the scenario asked participants to carefully consider the situation and envision that it had actually occurred between themselves and their partners (identical to the first pilot study). Respondents next answered the same realism and frequency items used in the first pilot study on 7-point, Likert-type scales with higher 47 values indicating more realism and frequency of occurrence (e.g., “This situation is easy to imagine”; “I have often experienced a situation like this one in my cross-sex friendship/dating/sibling relationship”). In addition, two 7-point, Likert-type items assessing whether or not such a situation has ever occurred in the participants’ own relationships were created and included (e.g., “I have encountered a situation like this at least once in my own cross-sex friendship/dating relationship/sibling relationship”). Participants were next asked to briefly describe a situation similar to the scenario that actually happened between them and their relational partners. Finally, the respondents answered a number of demographic items, handed the surveys in, were thanked, and dismissed (see Appendix B for a full version of pilot study II). Pilot study II results. Because of the small sample and cell sizes, a probability level of .10 was selected. The three realism items were internally consistent across each of the three relational context conditions and overall (sibling = .75; dating partner = .82; cross-sex friend = .78; all conditions = .79). The mean for the realism composite variable suggests that the modified scenario was moderate-to-high in realism (M = 5.65, SD = 1.19). A one-sample t-test found that the scenario was judged to be significantly more realistic than the 4.0 scale midpoint [t (23) = 6.81, p < .0001]. Further, the results of a univariate ANOVA with the realism variable as the dependent variable and relational context as the fixed factor revealed that realism level (cross-sex friend M = 5.29, SD = 1.33; dating partner M = 5.71, SD = 1.33; sibling M = 5.93, SD = .997) did not significantly differ across relational contexts [F (2, 23) = .593, p = .56, power = .14]. The three frequency items were reliable within and across relational contexts (sibling = .83; dating partner = .81; cross-sex friend = .86; overall = .84). The 48 composite variable that was computed was moderate in frequency of occurrence (M = 3.40, SD = 1.59). Results of a univariate ANOVA with relational context as the fixed factor and frequency of actual occurrence as the dependent variable indicated that frequency levels (cross-sex friend M = 2.63, SD = 1.35; dating partner M = 3.45, SD = 1.56; sibling M = 4.04, SD = 1.68) did not significantly differ across the three conditions [F (2, 23) = 1.79, p = .19, power = .33]. The two items assessing whether each scenario had ever actually occurred were internally consistent within and across relational contexts (sibling = .75; dating partner = .76; cross-sex friend = .90; overall = .82). The composite variable indicated that participants agreed that similar situations had happened in their own relationship to a moderate degree (M = 4.85, SD = 1.92). A one-sample t-test found that the degree to which a similar situation had ever occurred in participants’ own lives was significantly higher than the 4.0 scale midpoint [t (23) = 2.18, p < .05]. Results of a univariate ANOVA with relational context as the fixed factor and the degree to which the scenarios ever occurred as the dependent variable indicated that frequency levels (cross-sex friend M = 2.63, SD = 1.35; dating partner M = 4.14, SD = 2.01; sibling M = 4.31, SD = 2.14) did not significantly differ across the three conditions [F (2, 23) = 2.36, p = .12, power = .42]. The results of the second pilot study showed that the modified scenarios were more realistic and frequent in participants’ own lives than the scenarios examined in pilot study one (see Table 1). Further, situations similar to the modified scenarios have occurred at least once in most participants’ own relationships and each of the three relevant relational contexts display fairly equal levels of realism and frequency of 49 occurrence in participants’ own lives and have occurred at least once at a relatively equal rate. As such, the modified scenarios were employed in the main investigation. Main Investigation Participants Three hundred and sixty four participants taking undergraduate speech communication courses at a large, southern university responded to a written questionnaire. Although college undergraduates represent a convenience sample, studying this population in the current study was appropriate for two reasons: (1) crosssex friendships are most common during young adulthood (Monsour, 2002; Reeder, 2000; Werking, 1997a); and (2) researchers have called for more sibling rivalry research within this age group (Cicirelli, 1982; Dunn, 1985). Of the original sample, six people incorrectly answered the manipulation check item for relational context and were eliminated from further analysis. Five individuals reported being either homosexual or bisexual, and one respondent indicated that he or she was reporting on a bisexual partner. These participants were also eliminated from further analysis because they were not a large enough group to compare with heterosexual participants and partners. A sample size of 352 was thus employed for data analysis. The final sample was predominantly female (n = 211, 60%) and averaged 20.87 years in age (SD = 3.29, range = 18 to 57 years). The vast majority of the participants reported being White (n = 314, 89.2%), 24 indicated that they were Black/African American (6.8%), nine reported being of Asian descent (2.6%), three stated that they were a part of an “other” category (.9%), and one each reported being Native American or Hispanic (.3%). The final sample was comprised of freshmen (n = 26, 7.4%), sophomores (n = 80, 50 22.7%), juniors (n = 94, 26.7%), seniors (n = 150, 42.6%), and graduate students (n = 2, .6%). The partners that participants reported upon were predominantly male (n = 199, 56.5%) and averaged 21.23 years in age (SD = 3.98, range = 5 to 52 years). Overall, the relationships participants reported on averaged eight years and 10 months in length (M = 106.72, SD = 104.53, range = two weeks to 684 months). Respondents indicated that they were both close (M = 5.95, SD = 1.01) and satisfied with their relationships (M = 5.67, SD = 1.29). Further, 153 participants (43.5%) indicated that they were in contact with their partner daily, 135 contacted one another weekly (38.4%), 51 were in monthly contact (14.5%), eight were in contact several times a year (2.3%), and three stated that they contacted one another once a year or less (.9%). One hundred twenty people responded to questionnaires on the sibling conditions, 114 answered dating partner surveys, and 118 completed the cross-sex friend instrument. For respondents in the dating partner conditions, two indicated that they were single and not dating (1.8%), one stated that he or she was single and dating many individuals (.9%), nine reported that they were in a non-exclusive relationship (7.9%), 41 indicated that they were in an exclusive relationship for less than one year (36%), 53 stated that they were in an exclusive relationship of more than one year (46.5%), eight individuals reported being engaged (7%), and three did not respond. The two individuals who reported being single and not dating were retained for data analysis because the term “dating” was broadly defined to participants as going on at least one date with another individual. For the cross-sex friend condition respondents, 21 (18%) reported that their crosssex friends were former romantic partners, 83 (70%) stated that they were not former 51 romantic partners, and 14 (12%) were not sure about the former relational status. Seventy-four cross-sex friends (63%) also reported never having engaged in sexual behavior with their partners, nine (8%) reported that sexual behavior had occurred once, 18 (15%) indicated that sexual behavior had occurred two to four times, and 17 (14%) stated that sexual behavior with a cross-sex friend occurred more than five times. Participants in the sibling conditions reported that they had an average of 1.97 siblings (SD = 1.14, range = 1 to 6). Participants’ siblings averaged 21.48 years of age (SD = 5.83, range = 5 to 47). One hundred eight participants (91%) reported on a full sibling (i.e., sharing both parents biologically), five (4%) reported on a half sibling (i.e., sharing one parent biologically), four (3%) reported on an adopted sibling, and two (2%) reported on a stepsibling (i.e., sharing both parents by marriage but not biologically). When asked about their current living situations with their siblings, 59 respondents (49%) lived with their siblings when they were not in school, 47 (39%) never lived with their siblings, seven (6%) lived with their siblings year round, and seven (6%) indicated ‘other’ (see Tables 3 and 4). Research Design The project employed a 3 (relational context: cross-sex friend, sibling relationship, dating relationship) X 3 (jealousy expression: distributive, integrative, negative affect expression) factorial design. Participants were nested within one of the nine experimental conditions. Because the nine conditions were distributed randomly to participants, a relatively equal number of students responded to each condition. If the participants received a questionnaire asking them to report on a relationship that they were not currently involved in, they were instructed to raise their hands to receive a 52 different relational context instrument. The need to switch questionnaires in this manner only occurred for approximately 10% of the participants, with the three dating partner conditions most likely to be exchanged. Specifically, the questionnaire first asked participants to consider either their closest present cross-sex friend, closest present dating partner, or sibling closest in age to them (consistent with Bedford, 1989; Cicirelli, 1982; Hapworth, Hapworth, & Heilman, 1994). Cross-sex friend was defined as a member of the opposite sex to whom the participant shares a close relationship and who is not either a family member or a current romantic partner. Dating partner was defined as someone the participant has dated for one week or more, including going on one or two dates with another person. Sibling was defined as a full brother or sister of the sibling who shares the same biological mother and father as the participant. If participants did not have full siblings, they were instructed to think of half, step, or adopted siblings if they wished. Upon reading the description of the relevant relationship, participants were asked to record the initials of the individuals that they selected before and while reading the experimental manipulation (see Appendix C for an example of one version of the full main instrument). To avoid participant biases in the recall and memory of actual jealousy situations that can often accompany retrospective recall techniques, hypothetical scenarios were chosen as the method of examining the predictions and questions set forth in this project. This methodological technique is consistent with previous jealousy research and is preferable to simply asking participants how jealous they are (e.g., Bringle & Buunk, 1986; Mathes, Roter, & Joerger, 1982). Further, as Knobloch and Solomon (2002a) point out, use of hypothetical scenarios allow both measurement of participants’ immediate and 53 Table 3 Demographic Variables Overall and by Relational Context Demographic Variable Overall Sibling Friend Dating Participant Gender Male Female 141 211 48 72 50 68 43 71 Participant Year of Age Mean Standard Deviation Range 20.87 3.29 18 - 57 21.00 4.37 18 - 57 20.84 3.23 18 - 48 20.77 1.66 18 - 27 Participant Ethnicity Asian Black/African American Hispanic Native American White/European American Other 9 24 1 1 314 3 5 8 0 0 105 2 2 9 0 0 106 1 2 7 1 1 103 0 Participant Year in School Freshman Sophomore Junior Senior Fifth Year Senior Graduate Student 26 80 94 94 56 2 10 33 28 28 21 0 10 22 37 33 15 1 6 25 29 33 20 1 Partner Year of Age Mean Standard Deviation Range 21.23 3.98 5 - 52 21.18 5.62 5 - 52 21.03 3.07 18 - 48 21.50 2.48 18 - 35 199 153 62 58 66 52 71 43 Partner Gender Male Female Note. N = 352. 54 Table 4 Relational Variables Overall and by Relational Context Relational Variable Overall Sibling Friend Dating Relationship Length* Mean in Months Standard Deviation Range in Months 106.72 104.53 .5 – 684 233.50a 62.86 60 – 684 61.07b 51.95 2 – 264 20.59c 19.59 .5 – 96 Frequency of Contact with Partner* Mean Standard Deviation Daily Weekly Monthly Several Times a Year Once a Year or Less 1.78 .840 153 135 51 8 3 2.17b .882 27 55 31 5 2 1.94b .791 34 61 18 3 1 1.20a .446 92 19 2 0 0 Relationship Closeness* Mean Standard Deviation Range 5.95 1.01 1–7 5.72a 1.05 1–7 5.75a .991 3–7 6.38b .849 3–7 Relationship Satisfaction Mean 5.67 5.48 5.70 5.86 Standard Deviation 1.29 1.41 1.20 1.26 Range 1–7 1–7 1–7 1–7 ________________________________________________________________________ Note. N = 352. * indicates that significant mean differences exist at p < .05; see subscript letters within rows that differ to detect specific differences. Higher values indicate more closeness and satisfaction; lower values indicate greater frequency of contact. 55 direct response to the relational event of interest and provide control over the specific situations the participants are to consider. As such, the use of hypothetical scenarios was deemed appropriate and useful for the purpose of this project. The scenario for the sibling conditions stated that the participant’s close sibling makes it clear to the participant that he or she is jealous because the participant seems to be receiving more attention and affection from their parents. The scenario for the crosssex friend conditions detailed an instance where the participant’s close cross-sex friend makes it clear to the participant that he or she is jealous because the participant is showing interest in doing things with another friend of the opposite sex. The wordings for both situations are adapted from similar ones used by Bringle (1991). The dating scenario described a situation where the participant’s dating partner tells the participant that he/she is jealous that the participant is showing interest in spending time with another potential dating partner. This wording is adapted from numerous romantic jealousy scenarios (e.g., Bevan, 1999; Hansen, 1982). According to the pilot test results described in an earlier section of this chapter, each were moderately realistic and accurately represented the concept that they were intended to measure. Two one-item manipulation checks within the main questionnaire also ensured that participants correctly interpreted relational context and mode of jealousy expression. Participants who incorrectly answered either manipulation check were not included in the sample examined for data analysis. Main Investigation Measures Scenario realism, frequency, and likelihood of ever occurring. Upon reading and considering the scenario, participants first answered items previously used in the first and 56 second pilot studies. Three items from Bevan (2002) each assessed situation realism and frequency of occurrence (e.g., “How realistic do you think this situation is?”; “How often has this situation occurred in your own relationship?”) by way of 7-point, Likert-type scales (e.g., 1 = not at all realistic; 7 = very realistic). Both scales have demonstrated strong reliability (i.e., Cronbach’s = .91 for realism and .88 for frequency) in previous research (Bevan, 2002). Similarly, these scales were found to have strong internal consistency in the main investigation ( = .86 for realism and .89 for frequency). In addition, as in the second pilot study, the two items assessing the degree to which each scenario ever occurred in the participants’ own lives by way of a 7-point, Likert-type scale were also included (e.g., “I have encountered a situation like this at least once in my own cross-sex friendship/dating relationship/sibling relationship”; 1 = strongly disagree; 7 = strongly agree). As in pilot study two, this scale was also reliable ( = .90). Composite variables created for analysis revealed that the jealousy situation was moderate-to-high in realism (M = 5.12, SD = 1.52) and occurred at a low-to-moderate frequency (M = 2.84, SD = 1.53). Further, there was moderate agreement that a similar situation had occurred at least once in participants’ own relationships (M = 4.27, SD = 2.04). Jealousy message realism and valence. Upon answering items about the situation itself, a number of semantic differential items measured the realism and valence of the jealousy message itself. Two items (1 = very difficult to imagine; not at all realistic; 7 = very easy to imagine; completely realistic) assessed jealousy message realism. These items were internally consistent ( = .87) and a composite variable was created for analysis (M = 4.69, SD = 1.62). Six semantic differential items measured message 57 valence (e.g., 1 = completely negative; not at all positive; bad; unpleasant; 7 = not at all negative; completely positive; good; pleasant) and formed a highly reliable scale ( = .94). A composite valence variable was created for data analysis (M = 3.52, SD = 1.37). General partner and relational uncertainty. General uncertainty about the partner was measured by way of eight items previously used by Parks and Adelman (1983). These items (e.g., “I can accurately predict what my sibling/cross-sex friend’s/dating partner’s attitudes are”; “I can accurately predict how my sibling/cross-sex friend/dating partner will respond to me in most situations”) were measured by way of 7-point, Likerttype scales (1 = strongly disagree; 7 = strongly agree). Though Parks and Adelman (1983) did not report the actual reliability value, they did state that the scale has demonstrated evidence of reliability and validity. In the present study, these eight items achieved strong internal consistency ( = .81) and a composite variable revealed that participants were moderate in partner certainty in response to the hypothetical situation (M = 4.61, SD = 1.09). Relational uncertainty was assessed using two of the four relationship uncertainty subscales developed by Knobloch and Solomon (1999). Specifically, uncertainty about the relationship’s behavioral norms and mutuality of feelings were chosen because they were deemed most appropriate and applicable to all three relational contexts. All uncertainty items were revised to reflect the specific relational contexts under investigation and to measure a participant’s reaction to a hypothetical scenario (e.g., “If this situation actually occurred, I would be confident of my ability to accurately predict my dating partner’s/cross-sex friend’s/sibling’s behavior”). 58 Four items each assessed behavioral norms (e.g., “I am certain about what I can or cannot say to my sibling/cross-sex friend/dating partner in this relationship”; “I am certain about the norms of this sibling/cross-sex friend/dating relationship”) and mutuality of feelings (e.g., “I am certain about whether or not my sibling/cross-sex friend/dating partner and I feel the same way about each other”; “I am certain about how my sibling/cross-sex friend/dating partner and I view this relationship”). Each of these items was measured using 7-point, Likert-type scales (1 = strongly disagree; 7 = strongly agree). Knobloch and Solomon (Knobloch & Solomon, 1999; Solomon & Knobloch, 2001) previously reported a satisfactory internal consistency ( = .73) for the behavioral norm subscale and a strong internal consistency for the mutuality subscale ( = .89). Two additional behavioral norms items (“If this situation actually occurred, I would be certain about how I can act in the presence of my dating partner/cross-sex friend/sibling”; “If this situation actually occurred, I would be certain about the rules of this dating/cross-sex friend/sibling relationship”) were created by the author in an attempt to bolster the internal consistency of Knobloch and Solomon’s (1999) scale. In terms of the present investigation, an exploratory factor analysis with varimax rotation was conducted to ensure that the two relational uncertainty variables would emerge as two separate factors. However, results of this factor analysis revealed a unidimensional relational uncertainty factor (eigenvalue = 5.73, 57% of the variance explained). A test of internal consistency also indicated that using the total number of relational uncertainty items results in a highly reliable scale ( = .90). As such, a composite relational uncertainty variable was created for data analysis (M = 4.58, SD = 1.22). 59 Jealousy-related emotion. The extent to which participants experienced emotions commonly associated with jealousy after the partner expresses jealousy to them was measured using seven semantic differential items (e.g., 1 = not sad at all; 7 = very sad): sadness, anger, frustration, guilt, fear, insecurity, and surprise. These emotions have been found to be relevant to the jealousy experience by previous researchers (Pfeiffer & Wong, 1989; White & Mullen, 1989; Zammuner & Fischer, 1995) and seem particularly applicable as consequences of a partner’s jealousy expression. To determine whether these emotions comprised a single jealousy-related emotion factor or if they were actually multiple emotions, an exploratory factor analysis using varimax rotation was conducted. This analysis revealed three separate emotion factors. Factor one (eigenvalue = 2.52, 36% of the variance explained) included fear (.808) and insecurity (.772). Factor two (eigenvalue = 1.26, 18% of the variance explained) was comprised of anger (.885) and frustration (.735). The third factor (eigenvalue = 1.06, 15% of the variance explained) included sadness (.777) and surprise (.674). Guilt cross-loaded on both the first and third factors (.591 and .524 respectively). However, when these three factors were examined separately for internal consistency, none displayed a Cronbach’s alpha over .68. A reliability test was then conducted using all seven emotions together, which revealed an alpha of .71 without the surprise item. To further explore this potential single emotion factor, a factor analysis with a varimax rotation that specified one factor with the six remaining emotion items was conducted. From this test, an eigenvalue of 2.46 was observed and 41% of the variance was explained by this single factor. In addition, factor loadings for each of the six emotion items ranged from .539 to .687. Thus, to satisfy both the need for internal 60 consistency and the general specifications suggested by the factor analyses, the six emotions of sadness, anger, frustration, guilt, fear, and insecurity were combined to create a single jealousy-related emotion variable. Participants were moderate in their emotional reaction to the hypothetical jealousy situation (M = 3.76, SD = 1.07). Rumination. Cloven and Roloff’s (1991) mulling scale was used to assess participants’ frequency of thinking about the jealousy interaction. Rumination was measured using five 7-point, semantic differential scales and item wording was altered to reflect the hypothetical nature of the jealousy situation (e.g., 1 = would not worry at all about the interaction; no time would be spent reflecting on this interaction; would never think about this interaction; 7 = would worry very much about the interaction; a lot of time would be spent reflecting on this interaction; would think about this interaction all the time). Strong internal consistency (i.e., Cronbach’s ranging from .88 to .93) for this measure has been reported by both Cloven and Roloff (1991) and Johnson and Roloff (1998). Similar strong internal consistency was observed in the main investigation ( = .88) and a composite rumination variable was computed for analysis. Respondents indicated that they would ruminate about the situation to a moderate degree (M = 4.73, SD = 1.22). Table 5 presents means, standard deviations, and correlations among the main investigation’s dependent variables. Relational and demographic information. All participants were then asked about a number of relevant relational variables, including the length of their relationships in months and the frequency of contact with their relational partners (daily, weekly, monthly, several times a year, once a year or less). Further, closeness (i.e., “How close would you say your sibling relationship/cross-sex friendship is?”) and satisfaction (i.e., 61 Table 5 Means, Standard Deviations, and Correlations for Main Investigation Dependent Variables Mean SD 1 2 3 4 1. PartUnc 4.61 1.10 2. RelUnc 4.58 1.22 3. Emotion 3.76 1.07 .74* -.16* -.17* -.08 -12* .47* 4. Ruminate 4.73 1.22 Note. All correlations are one-tailed. PartUnc = general partner uncertainty; RelUnc = relational uncertainty, * p < .01. 62 “How satisfied are you with this sibling relationship/cross-sex friendship?”) was assessed using one-item 7-point, Likert-type scales (1 = not at all close/satisfied; 7 = very close/satisfied). A number of items were particular to the sibling relationship. Specifically, participants in the three sibling conditions were asked to indicate how many total siblings they have, the type of siblings they have (natural, half, step, or adopted), the gender of each sibling, the age difference between each sibling, their current living situation with their siblings (i.e., live with them year round, live with them when not in school, never live with them), and whether or not their parents are divorced and/or remarried. Two items specifically applied to cross-sex friendships. Specifically, respondents in the three cross-sex friendship conditions indicated how frequently they have engaged in sexual behavior with their friend (more than five times, two to four times, once, never) and whether or not the cross-sex friend is a former romantic partner. For the dating partner conditions, participants were asked to describe the type of relationship they had (single – not dating, single – dating many individuals, in a relationship that is not exclusive, in an exclusive relationship for less than one year, in an exclusive relationship for more than one year, engaged). Finally, participants were asked to provide both their own and their relational partners’ age, gender, and sexual orientation. In addition, participants indicated their ethnicity and year in school. Procedures Students received required research credit for their participation. Participants either signed up for a time period outside of class where they completed one of nine questionnaire versions in groups of ten to thirty or completed the instrument during class 63 time. The questionnaire versions contained identical information, except for situations operationalizing the nine conditions of interest and a small number of questions specific to each of the three relational contexts under examination. After reading and signing a consent form, participants filled out the questionnaire (which took 10 to 15 minutes), turned it in to the investigator, and were given an opportunity to ask questions about the research. Then, debriefing forms were distributed and the participants were thanked and dismissed. 64 CHAPTER 3 RESULTS Preliminary Analyses Differences between relational contexts. A multivariate analysis of variance that examined potential differences in relationship length, closeness, satisfaction, and frequency of contact with relation to relationship context detected a significant effect [Wilks’ (8, 688) = .18, p < .0001, 2 = .58]; specifically, differences for relationship length [F (2, 347) = 641.42, p < .0001, 2 = .79], frequency of contact [F (2, 347) = 54.11, p < .0001, 2 = .24], and closeness [F (2, 347) = 17.01, p < .0001, 2 = .09] were observed (see Tables 3 and 4). Relationship satisfaction did not significantly differ by relationship context [F (2, 347) = 2.60, p = .08, power = .52]. According to Tukey HSD post hoc tests, the three relationship contexts each significantly differed from one another in terms of relationship length (sibling M = 233.50, SD = 62.86; cross-sex friend M = 61.07, SD = 51.95; dating partner M = 20.59, SD = 19.59). Further, dating partners were in significantly more frequent contact (M = 1.20, SD = .446) than either cross-sex friends (M = 1.94, SD = .791) or siblings (M = 2.17, SD = .882) and were significantly closer to their partners (M = 6.38, SD = .849) than were cross-sex friends (M = 5.75, SD = .991) or siblings (M = 5.72, SD = 1.05). Satisfaction levels did not differ by context (dating partner M = 5.86, SD = 1.26; cross-sex friend M = 5.70, SD = 1.20; sibling M = 5.48, SD = 1.41). 65 Scenario realism, frequency, and likelihood of ever occurring. The composite realism and the “ever occurred” variables were separately compared to the scale midpoint (4.0) using two one-sample t-tests. These tests revealed that both realism level [t (351 ) = 13.88, p < .0001] and the rate at which the situation occurred at least once happened at a rate that were significantly higher than the scale midpoint [t (351) = 2.47, p < .05]. In addition, results of a univariate ANOVA with scenario realism as the dependent variable and experimental condition as the fixed factor detected a significant effect for situation realism [F (8, 343) = 2.34, p < .05, 2 = .05]. Tukey HSD post hoc tests found that the dating partner-integrative communication message situation (M = 5.81, SD = 1.03) was significantly more realistic than the cross-sex friend-negative affect expression message situation (M = 4.63, SD = 1.51), with no other situations differing from one another in terms of realism. Because of this unwanted significant difference in scenario realism means across experimental conditions, the scenario realism variable was entered as a covariate when conducting analyses. Next, a univariate ANOVA examining the “ever occurred” variable in relation to experimental condition revealed a significant effect [F (8, 343) = 2.42, p < .05, 2 = .05]. Post hoc tests using the Tukey HSD procedure showed that the sibling-distributive communication message situation (M = 5.12, SD = 1.85) was significantly more likely to have occurred at least once compared with the sibling-integrative communication message situation (M = 3.69, SD = 2.08). No other situations differed significantly from one another with regard to this variable. As such, “ever occurred” variable was entered as a covariate when analyses were conducted. For the frequency of occurrence variable, a univariate ANOVA examining it in terms of experimental condition did not find any 66 significant differences across situations [F (8, 343) = 1.49, p = .16, power = .67] (see Table 6). Jealousy message realism and valence. Using a one-sample t-test, it was determined that the composite realism variable for jealousy message was significantly more realistic than the 4.0 scale midpoint [t (344) = 8.03, p < .0001]. In addition, a univariate ANOVA with jealousy message realism as the dependent variable and experimental condition as the fixed factor revealed no significant differences for the realism of the jealousy message used by another [F (8, 336) = 1.77, p = .08, power = .76]. A univariate analysis of variance conducted on the nine conditions to ensure that each jealousy message significantly differed as expected in terms of message valence found a significant main effect for message valence [F (8, 343) = 8.39, p < .0001, 2 = .16]. Tukey HSD post hoc analyses indicated that each of the distributive communication conditions were significantly more negative than the three integrative communication conditions (see Table 6). The three negative affect expression conditions did not significantly differ in valence from one another, but also did not form a distinct subset from the distributive and integrative communication conditions. This unexpected “blending” of conditions was deemed acceptable for two reasons: (a) the neutrality inherent in the negative affect expression condition could potentially lead to differential interpretations for different individuals; and (b) a univariate ANOVA and Tukey HSD post hoc analyses using composite variables for each of the three jealousy messages revealed the anticipated significant message valence differences [F (2, 349) = 28.14, p < 67 Table 6 Realism, Frequency of Occurrence, and Ever Occurred Variable Means and Standard Deviations for Jealousy Scenarios Examined in Main Investigation Jealousy Scenario Realism Mean SD Frequency Mean SD Ever Occurred Mean SD Sibling-Integrative 4.75a,b 1.75 2.63 1.55 3.70a 2.08 Sibling-Negative Affect Exp 5.04a,b 1.55 2.75 1.53 4.15a,b 2.06 Sibling-Distributive 5.23a,b 1.57 3.32 1.61 5.12b 1.85 Friend-Integrative 4.83a,b 1.44 2.50 1.48 3.90a,b 2.24 1.51 2.68 1.49 3.88a,b 2.08 Friend-Negative Affect Exp 4.63a Friend-Distributive 5.06a,b 1.37 3.27 1.62 4.50a,b 1.88 Dating-Integrative 5.81b 1.03 2.91 1.30 4.77a,b 1.86 Dating-Negative Affect Exp 5.37a,b 1.55 2.96 1.53 4.59a,b 1.97 Dating-Distributive 5.38a,b 1.59 2.58 1.51 3.79a,b 2.01 Overall 5.12 1.52 2.84 1.53 4.27 2.04 Note. N = 352. Within columns, means sharing superscript letters do not significantly differ at p < .05. There were no significant differences at p < .05 for the frequency variable. Higher values indicate more realism, more frequency of occurrence, and more agreement that the situation has occurred at least once in participant’s own relationships. 68 .0001, 2 = .14; distributive communication composite variable M = 2.96, SD = 1.11; negative affect expression composite variable M = 3.41, SD = 1.20; integrative communication composite variable M = 4.20, SD = 1.50] (see Table 7). Analysis Plan Hypotheses one and two were analyzed using MANCOVA, with general partner and relational uncertainty (which were highly correlated with one another) as the dependent variables and relational context as the independent variable. Similarly, hypotheses three and four were assessed using MANCOVA with jealousy message type as the fixed factor and both general partner and relational uncertainty as the dependent variables. Hypotheses five and seven were examined using separate univariate ANCOVAs, with relational context as the independent variable and the composite measure of jealousy-related emotion intensity (H5) and amount of rumination (H7) as the dependent variables. Hypotheses six and eight were also tested using separate univariate ANCOVAs, with jealousy message type as the fixed factor and intensity of jealousyrelated emotion (H6) and amount of rumination (H8) as the dependent variables. For the first eight hypotheses, the scenario realism and “ever occurred” variables were entered as covariates because they unexpectedly varied according to experimental condition. Planned polynomial contrasts were conducted to examine the specific differences proposed by the first eight hypotheses. Though no predictions were specifically forwarded, potential interactions between relational context and jealousy message used by another were examined for hypotheses one through eight. The final hypothesis was 69 Table 7 Valence and Realism Variable Means and Standard Deviations for Jealousy Expression Types Examined in Main Investigation Jealousy Expression Type Valence Mean SD Realism Mean SD Sibling-Integrative 4.25d,e 1.38 4.35 1.66 Sibling-Negative Affect Expression 3.29a,b,c 1.16 4.71 1.64 Sibling-Distributive 2.93a,b .849 4.99 1.44 Cross-Sex Friend-Integrative 3.97c,d,e 1.45 4.57 1.59 Cross-Sex Friend-Negative Affect Express 3.39a,b,c,d 1.17 4.17 1.97 Cross-Sex Friend-Distributive 3.36a,b,c,d 3.36 4.64 1.63 Dating Partner-Integrative 4.39e 1.46 5.36 1.10 Dating Partner-Negative Affect Express 3.56b,c,d,e 1.28 4.80 1.47 Dating Partner-Distributive 2.60a 4.71 1.75 1.22 Overall 3.52 1.34 4.69 1.62 Note. N = 352. Within columns, means sharing superscript letters do not significantly differ at p < .05 for the valence variable. There were no significant differences at p < .05 for the realism variable. Higher values indicate more realism and more positive valence. 70 tested using PATH (Hunter and Hamilton, 1993) (see Figure 1 for details about the paths predicted). Hypotheses One and Two The first two hypotheses predicted that general partner uncertainty (H1) and relational uncertainty (H2) would each be strongest when jealousy is expressed by a cross-sex friend, followed in strength by a dating partner, then a sibling. Significant differences between each of these three contexts were expected for both hypotheses. MANCOVA results including both general partner and relational uncertainty revealed a significant effect for relational context [Wilks’ (4, 692) = .92, p < .0001, partial 2 = .04]. In addition, a significant effect was observed for the scenario realism covariate [Wilks’ (2, 346) = .97, p < .01, partial 2 = .03]; specifically in relation to the general partner uncertainty variable [F (1, 341) = 8.87, p < .01, 2 = .03]. No significant effects were found for the “ever occurred” covariate. Specifically, differences for both general partner uncertainty [F (2, 341) = 9.42, p < .0001, 2 = .05] and relational uncertainty [F (2, 341) = 14.05, p < .0001, 2 = .08] were revealed. Planned polynomial contrasts indicate that, as predicted, cross-sex friends experience significantly more general partner uncertainty than either dating partners [t (349) = -4.97, p < .0001] or siblings [t (349) = 3.09, p < .01]. However, the dating partner and sibling relational contexts do not significantly differ from one another for general partner uncertainty [t (349) = 1.93, p = .05]. For relational uncertainty, as predicted, cross-sex friends experienced significantly more uncertainty than either siblings [t (349) = -3.36, p < .01] or dating partners [t (349) = -5.68, p < .0001]. However, siblings experienced significantly more 71 uncertainty than did dating partners [t (349) = 2.37, p < .05], a finding opposite of that predicted by H2. As such, H1 and H2 are each partially consistent with the data. Hypotheses Three and Four Hypotheses three and four predicted that general partner uncertainty (H3) and relational uncertainty (H4) would be strongest when another expressed jealousy using distributive communication, followed by negative affect expression and integrative communication. Significant differences between each jealousy expression strategy were anticipated for both hypotheses. MANCOVA results for both uncertainty types revealed a significant effect for jealousy message [Wilks’ (4, 680) = 3.19, p < .05, partial 2 = .02]. A significant effect was observed for the scenario realism covariate [Wilks’ (2, 340) = 4.78, p < .01, partial 2 = .03] in relation to the general partner uncertainty variable [F (1, 341) = 7.65, p < .01, 2 = .02]. No significant effects were revealed for the “ever occurred” covariate. Specific differences were detected for both general partner uncertainty [F (2, 341) = 5.94, p < .01, 2 = .03] and for relational uncertainty [F (2, 341) = 4.96, p < .01, 2 = .03]. Planned polynomial contrasts suggest a consistent pattern for each uncertainty variable. As predicted, another’s use of negative affect expression is associated with significantly more general partner uncertainty [t (349) = 3.22, p < .001] and relational uncertainty [t (349) = 2.85, p < .01) than another’s use of integrative communication. Another’s use of distributive communication did not significantly differ from either negative affect expression [general partner uncertainty t (349) = -1.69, p = .09, relational uncertainty t (349) = -1.52, p = .13] or integrative communication [general partner uncertainty t (349) = 1.55, p = .12, relational uncertainty t (349) = 1.35, p = .18] in terms 72 of either general partner or relational uncertainty. Thus, H3 and H4 are each partially consistent with the data. Hypothesis Five Hypothesis five predicted that intensity of jealousy-related emotion would vary according to the relational context another’s jealousy was expressed in. ANCOVA results revealed a significant effect for jealousy-related emotion [F (2, 346) = 10.67, p < .0001, 2 = .06]. The realism and “ever occurred” covariates were not significant. Planned polynomial contrasts suggest that both siblings [t (349) = -3.84, p < .0001] and dating partners [t (349) = -4.52, p < .0001] experience significantly more intense emotions in response to another’s jealousy expression compared with cross-sex friends. Siblings and dating partners do not differ in their intensity of jealousy-related emotion [t (349) = .726, p = .47]. Thus, H5 is consistent with the data. Hypothesis Six The sixth hypothesis proposed that another’s use of either distributive communication or negative affect expression to express jealousy to an individual would be associated with that individual’s more intense experience of jealousy-related emotions than another’s use of integrative jealousy expression. Significant differences for jealousyrelated emotion were not detected using ANCOVA [F (2, 340) = 1.74, p = .18, power = .36]. The realism and “ever occurred” covariates were not significant. Another’s use of distributive jealousy expression, negative affect expression, or integrative jealousy expression did not significantly differ in relation to the individual’s jealousy-related emotion intensity. Hypothesis six was thus not consistent with the data. 73 Hypothesis Seven Hypothesis seven predicted that jealousy expression from one’s cross-sex friend would be associated with that individual’s increased rumination compared with jealousy expression from a sibling or from a dating partner. ANCOVA results detected no significant differences for amount of rumination [F (2, 340) = 1.08, p = .34, power = .24]. The realism and “ever occurred” covariates were not significant. Specifically, jealousy expressed by cross-sex friends, dating partners, and siblings did not relate to significant differences in an individual’s amount of rumination about the situation. As such, the seventh hypothesis was not consistent with the data. Hypothesis Eight The eighth hypothesis stated that another’s use of both distributive communication and negative affect expression when expressing jealousy would be related to more rumination by an individual compared with another’s use of integrative jealousy expression. Using ANCOVA, a significant main effect was observed [F (2, 340) = 3.08, p < .05, 2 = .02]. The realism and “ever occurred” covariates were not significant. Planned polynomial contrasts suggest that another’s use of distributive communication and negative affect expression do not significantly differ from one another with regard to rumination [t (349) = -.113, p = .91]. Further, as predicted, another’s use of distributive communication [t (349) = -2.10, p < .05] and negative affect expression [t (349) = -1.99, p < .05] result in significantly greater rumination than another’s use of integrative communication. As such, hypothesis eight was consistent with the data. However, these results should be interpreted with caution due to the small effect size. 74 Table 8 Means and Standard Deviations for the Relationship Between Relational Context and the Dependent Variables Relational Context GPUnc RelUnc Emotion Ruminate Sibling 4.69b (1.11) 4.65b (1.21) 3.90b (1.03) 4.62 (1.33) Cross-Sex Friend 4.29a (1.12) 4.16a (1.19) 3.41a (1.03) 4.84 (1.12) 4.87b 4.94b 3.97b 4.74 (.966) (1.12) (1.07) (1.19) Note. N = 352. GPUnc = general partner uncertainty; RelUnc = relational uncertainty. Dating Partner Means reported here are from t-test conducted without the realism and “ever occurred” covariates used to test hypotheses one through eight. Within columns, means sharing superscript letters do not significantly differ at p < .05. There were no significant differences at p < .05 for the ruminate variable. Higher values indicate more certainty, stronger emotion, and more rumination about the situation. 75 Table 9 Means and Standard Deviations for the Relationship between Jealousy Expression Type and the Dependent Variables Jealousy Expression Type GPUnc RelUnc Emotion Ruminate Integrative Communication 4.86b (1.03) 4.90b (1.18) 3.62 (1.19) 4.51a (1.25) Negative Affect Expression 4.39a (1.11) 4.39a (1.29) 3.78 (.995) 4.84b (1.24) Distributive Communication 4.60a,b (1.11) 4.59a,b (1.15) 3.88 (1.01) 4.85b (1.13) Note. N = 352. GPUnc = general partner uncertainty; RelUnc = relational uncertainty. Means reported here are from t-test conducted without the realism and “ever occurred” covariates used to test hypotheses one through eight. Within columns, means sharing superscript letters do not significantly differ at p < .05. There were no significant differences at p < .05 for the emotion variable. Higher values indicate more certainty, stronger emotion, and more rumination about the situation. 76 Hypothesis Nine The final hypothesis tested a proposed reaction model of jealousy in close relationships wherein, after one’s close partner expresses jealousy to him or her, he or she first experiences general partner uncertainty, then relational uncertainty, which then leads to both ruminative thoughts and jealousy-related emotions. PATH results (Hunter & Hamilton, 1993) indicated that the proposed relationships between the variables were present with the exception of the association between relational uncertainty and jealousyrelated emotion. The chi-square goodness of fit test using ordinary least squares indicated that the proposed model did not fit the data based upon the path coefficients corrected for error of measurement using the reliability coefficients [2 (3) = 24.89, p < .0001]. Further, there was one error (obtained r – predicted r) greater than sampling error for the link between rumination and emotion (r = .46, z = 4.81, p < .0001). Based upon these analyses, the data were not consistent with the path model proposed by H9. Supplementary model testing results. Although the proposed reaction model of jealousy in close relationships was not consistent with the data, a number of predicted relationships were detected and an attempt to find a model that did fit the data seemed reasonable. Because a significant relationship was not observed between relational uncertainty and jealousy-related emotion and the individual link analysis suggested rumination and jealousy-related emotion should be linked, rumination was tested as a mediator between relational uncertainty and jealousy-related emotion. Consistent with this idea, McIntosh & Martin (1992) noted that rumination leads to negative affect because rumination is aversive and can cause individuals to polarize the unpleasant emotions that they associate with their inability to attain goals. This potential relationship 77 is in contrast to Martin and Tesser (1996a), who claim that rumination and affect only covary with one another (as tested by the model examined by H8). To determine if rumination does indeed result in jealousy-related emotion (which is operationalized as negative in this investigation), a model where general partner uncertainty leads to relational uncertainty, which then results in rumination, which then leads to jealousy-related emotion was tested. All relationships were significant as predicted and the chi-square goodness of fit test using ordinary least squares indicated that this model fit the data based upon the path coefficients corrected for error of measurement using the reliability coefficients [2 (3) = 2.06, p = .56]. Further, there were no errors (obtained r – predicted r) that were greater than sampling error. To further test the usefulness of the model, six separate replications were conducted using the three relational context conditions and the three jealousy message conditions. For each replication, no errors (obtained r – predicted r) greater than sampling error were found. In addition, the chi-square goodness of fit test using ordinary least squares indicated that each model fit the data based upon the path coefficients corrected for error of measurement using the reliability coefficients: sibling conditions [2 (3) = .53, p = .91], cross-sex friend conditions [2 (3) = 4.22, p = .24], dating partner conditions [2 (3) = 2.80, p = .42], distributive jealousy message conditions [2 (3) = 1.37, p = .71], integrative jealousy message conditions [2 (3) = 2.24, p = .52], and negative affect expression jealousy message conditions [2 (3) = .30, p = .96]. Finally, all predicted relationships were observed except for the association between relational uncertainty and rumination, which was not significant in the integrative jealousy message conditions ( = -.01), the distributive message conditions ( = -.10), the dating partner 78 conditions ( = -.09), and the cross-sex friend conditions ( = -.11). Despite these nonsignificant relationships, there is enough evidence that this model can represent the preliminary reactional model of jealousy in close relationships (see Figure 2). 79 (.74) Close Partner’s Expression of Jealousy General Partner Uncertainty (-.13) Relational Uncertainty (.47) Ruminative Thoughts Jealousy-Related Emotion Figure 2. Final path-analytic reaction model of jealousy with path coefficients. 80 CHAPTER 4 DISCUSSION, LIMITATIONS, AND CONCLUSIONS This project was executed in an attempt to learn about the consequences of jealousy expression in close relationships. To this end, two specific goals motivated the current investigation: (a) to determine the effects of different relational contexts and jealousy expression strategies on four proposed consequences of another’s jealousy expression – general partner uncertainty, relational uncertainty, rumination, and jealousyrelated emotion; and (b) to assess the interrelationships between these four proposed consequences by proposing and testing a reaction model of jealousy in close relationships. Two pilot studies and one main study using hypothetical scenarios and experimental conditions were conducted to assess the relationships proposed. Overall, general partner uncertainty, relational uncertainty, rumination, and jealousy-related emotion each represented consequences to another’s jealousy expression and the experience of these variables differed according to either the relational context in which the jealousy expression occurred or the way in which such jealousy was expressed. Further, a reactional model of jealousy delineating the relationships between these jealousy expression consequences was specified. The final section of this dissertation summarizes and discusses the implications of these findings, suggests avenues for future research, presents limitations to the project, and reports general conclusions about jealousy expression within close relationships. 81 Consequences of Another’s Jealousy Expression General Partner and Relational Uncertainty Because a robust relationship between uncertainty and jealousy has been demonstrated in previous research (e.g., Afifi & Reichert, 1996; Guerrero & Afifi, 1999; Knobloch et al., 2001), the first two proposed consequences of partner jealousy expression were general partner and relational uncertainty. Overall, the means for the two uncertainty variables indicated that participants reacted to another’s jealousy expression with low-to-moderate general partner and relational uncertainty. Further, general partner and relational uncertainty were the only proposed consequences where both relational context and jealousy expression led to differing uncertainty levels, suggesting that a series of complex relationships exist between uncertainty, relational context, and jealousy expression. According to the findings for H1 and H2, individuals whose cross-sex friends expressed jealousy experienced greater uncertainty than those whose siblings or dating partners communicated that they were jealous. These findings are consistent with Afifi and Burgoon (1998), who established that general partner and relational state uncertainty did occur in cross-sex friendships. However, although cross-sex friends experienced more uncertainty than did dating partners or siblings, it is important to emphasize that cross-sex friendships are not constantly plagued by uncertainty. Indeed, both this study and Afifi and Burgoon (1998) reported low-to-moderate uncertainty levels between cross-sex friends. Thus, globally characterizing cross-sex friendships by the ambiguity and uncertainty present between the partners may be inaccurate. Instead, as suggested by Afifi and Burgoon (1998), uncertainty between cross-sex friends may constitute an 82 additional “challenge” (Monsour, 2002; O’Meara, 1989) with which cross-sex friends must occasionally contend. Specifically, jealousy expression from one’s cross-sex friend seems to be one such occasion where uncertainty is elicited. Another notable implication of the first two hypotheses is that daters and siblings did not significantly differ in their uncertainty levels following another’s jealousy expression. No known research has employed the uncertainty construct in family relationships, perhaps because researchers assume that uncertainty is not present between family members. However, research on topic avoidance between parents and children (e.g., Golish & Caughlin, 2002; Guerrero & Afifi, 1995) certainly suggests that family members are not completely open with one another, especially about sensitive topics such as sex and money. Topic avoidance also very likely exists between siblings, and could give rise to uncertainty about one’s sibling and about the relationship. Further, topic avoidance and uncertainty both seem especially likely when at least one sibling is in college because the siblings might be geographically separated. Overall, the current findings introduce the presence of uncertainty in sibling relationships and create an interesting avenue of future research on uncertainty within families. As predicted, another’s use of negative affect expression to express jealousy resulted in greater levels of general partner (H3) and relational (H4) uncertainty compared with integrative communication. However, uncertainty levels in response to another’s use of distributive communication did not significantly differ from either of the other two jealousy expression strategies. Although negative and fairly uncommon as a choice for jealousy expression (e.g., Aylor & Dainton, 2001; Bevan & Lannutti, 2002), distributive communication only elicited low-to-moderate levels of partner and relational 83 uncertainty from an individual. Perhaps a partner’s use of distributive communication to express jealousy occurs more frequently than was previously believed and participants do not react with strong partner and relational uncertainty because such a situation has already taken place. Alternately, the hypothetical nature of the distributive communication scenarios might have tempered uncertain reactions to another’s jealousy expression. Actual instances where one’s partner uses distributive communication may result in higher uncertainty than what was presently observed. Overall, however, implications for H3 and H4 should be considered in light of the small effect sizes that were obtained. Though negative affect expression was not predicted to be the jealousy expression method that elicited the most partner and relational uncertainty, this finding makes sense for two reasons. First, the nonverbal display of jealousy-related emotions is consistently viewed as the most ambiguous method of expressing jealousy (Andersen et al., 1993; Guerrero et al., 1995). Thus, another’s use of negative affect expression could very likely result in an individual’s insecurity or uncertainty about what the partner is trying to say and what the interaction might mean for the relationship. Second, because one’s goal of reducing uncertainty about the primary relationship predicted the use of negative affect expression to express jealousy (Guerrero & Afifi, 1999), an inverse relationship where another’s use of negative affect expression results in an individual’s uncertainty about the partner and the relationship may also be likely. In general, the findings related to uncertainty and jealousy generate a number of interesting theoretical implications. First, unlike previous research examining uncertainty between dating partners (Afifi & Reichert, 1996; Knobloch et al., 2001) and cross-sex 84 friends (Afifi & Burgoon, 1998), findings for relational uncertainty and general partner uncertainty were found to differ from one another. That is, general partner uncertainty and relational uncertainty differed from one another in terms of relational context (H1 and H2). These findings support the argument that these two uncertainty types are conceptually discrete from one another (Knobloch & Solomon, 1999). However, because findings for H3 and H4 as well as other research have detected similarities between the two uncertainty types, it would be worthwhile to continue examining the similarities and differences between general partner and relational uncertainty to determine the degree to which they should be considered distinct. These findings also contribute to the body of research that highlights uncertainty negotiation and reduction across a variety of close relationships (e.g., Emmers & Canary, 1996; Knobloch & Solomon, 2002a; 2002b; Planalp & Honeycutt, 1985; Planalp et al., 1988; Turner, 1990). Namely, as Knobloch and Solomon (2002b) propose, the present results support the importance and usefulness of moving away from an uncertainty model where the reduction of uncertainty is primary (e.g., Berger & Calabrese, 1975). Specifically, this study contributes to knowledge about uncertainty negotiation across close relationships by highlighting general partner and relational uncertainty as integral aspects of a sequence of behaviors, cognitions, and emotions associated with jealousy expression and its immediate intrapersonal consequences. Overall, uncertainty not only serves as an antecedent or correlate of jealousy expression, but also represents an individual’s reaction to another’s jealousy expression. Thus, the importance of uncertainty in jealousy expression is again underscored and can 85 be extended to include an individual’s level of uncertainty as a potent consequence of another’s jealousy expression. Jealousy-Related Emotion The third proposed consequence of another’s jealousy expression was the elicitation of jealousy-related emotion. Jealousy is commonly characterized as an affect blend comprised primarily of negative emotions (Hupka, 1984) and an individual’s jealousy experience involves an emotional component (Pfeiffer & Wong, 1989). Thus, it seems logical that emotions culled from previous literature (Bringle, 1991; White & Mullen, 1989; Zammuner & Fischer, 1995) as frequent components of jealousy-related emotion might also arise in response to another’s jealousy expression. The final jealousyrelated emotion variable examined in data analyses included sadness, anger, frustration, guilt, fear, and insecurity. Findings for jealousy-related emotion revealed that participants experienced moderate emotional intensity after another’s jealousy expression, suggesting that experiencing negative jealousy-related emotions does seem to be a consequence of another’s jealousy expression. Findings for hypothesis five revealed that jealousy expressed by siblings and dating partners resulted in individuals’ more intense emotional experience compared with jealousy communicated by cross-sex friends. Because previous research did not provide a definitive prediction for the relationship between relational context and the intensity of jealousy-related emotions, H5’s findings could tentatively be explained in one of two ways. First, as H1 and H3 found, the cross-sex friend relational context involved greater uncertainty compared with the sibling and dating partner relational contexts. Because the reaction model of jealousy examined in this project states that uncertainty leads to 86 jealousy-related emotion, perhaps individuals whose cross-sex friends express jealousy are more concerned with their uncertainty and thus experience less intense jealousyrelated emotions than the other relational contexts. A second explanation is that siblings and daters may generally have stronger emotional reactions than do cross-sex friends. For example, Davies (2002) notes that shared history and common bonds can result in intense emotional experiences between siblings. In a similar vein, Dunn and McGuire (1992) found that children experience more anger in their sibling relationships then in their peer relationships. Further, individuals may experience strong negative emotional reactions when a dating partner expresses jealousy because, as Ellis and Weinstein (1986) suggested, the partner is implying that the individual is being dishonest or deceiving. Although Gaines et al. (1998) note that friendships involve moderate-to-high levels of emotional intimacy, little is known about situations that cause negative emotional reactions in cross-sex friendships. Further, comparisons between relational contexts in terms of emotional intensity have escaped empirical scrutiny. Clearly, these issues deserve further attention in future research on emotional reactions in close relationships. The sixth hypothesis revealed that no differences existed for an individual’s emotional intensity based upon how another communicated jealousy. This finding is particularly puzzling, as Guerrero et al. (1995) found that the three expression strategies examined were related to emotional jealousy (a construct very similar to jealousy-related emotion). Further, Guerrero and Afifi (1999) found that one’s intensity of jealous emotion predicted his or her use of distributive communication. However, these 87 relationships between jealousy-related emotion intensity and jealousy expression did not extend to one’s emotional reaction to another’s jealousy expression. One explanation for the failure to detect differences in emotional intensity with regard to another’s choice of jealousy expression is the conceptualization and measurement of the jealousy-related emotion variable. The difficulty in creating an internally consistent and unidimensional jealousy-related emotion scale provides evidence that multiple and separate negative emotions might occur within an individual when another expresses jealousy. Perhaps certain discrete emotions emerge as a consequence of another’s jealousy expression method. For example, the emotions of anger or frustration seem more likely to arise when another uses distributive communication compared with sadness or insecurity. An alternate explanation involves Ellsworth and Smith’s (1988) research on emotions and cognitive appraisals. Whereas the experience of emotions such as anger, sadness, fear, guilt, and surprise did not differ according to some cognitive appraisals such as certainty and attentional activity, they did vary in experience for the human agency appraisal (i.e., whether self or other was responsible or in control). Specifically, other-agency situations were associated with higher levels of anger and lower levels of guilt compared with self-agency situations. Perhaps the current jealousy situation was cognitively appraised in terms of human agency, and conceptualizing jealousy-related emotion unidimensionally would not account for the differential emotional experiences that might be present. Overall, to gain a broader understanding of emotional reactions to another’s jealousy expression, future research should examine jealousy-related emotion as both unidimensional and multidimensional concepts. 88 In general, an individual’s intensity of jealousy-related emotion can be viewed as a consequence of another’s jealousy expression. Further, findings for jealousy-related emotion suggest two things: (a) siblings and daters react with more emotional intensity than cross-sex friends when jealousy is expressed within their relationships; and (b) although another’s choice of jealousy expression method did not relate to levels of emotional intensity, a broader conceptualization of emotional reaction might allow significant differences to emerge. Overall, future research should attempt to learn more about emotional reactions to another’s jealousy expression by eliciting specific emotions experienced by individuals after others communicate jealousy to them. Rumination The final proposed consequence of another’s jealousy expression was rumination about the situation. Rumination was presumed to occur in a jealousy situation because it involves a reaction to a negative occurrence (e.g., Gold & Wegner, 1995). More specifically, rumination has been related to serial arguments (Johnson & Roloff, 1998), conflict (Cloven & Roloff, 1991), and, most importantly, romantic jealousy (Carson & Cupach, 2000). In terms of the present investigation, rumination could be considered the strongest consequence to another’s jealousy expression. Specifically, the rumination variable mean was significantly higher than the 4.0 scale midpoint [t (350) = 11.27, p < .0001]. As such, the link between jealousy expression and rumination can be extended to include rumination as a consequence of another’s jealousy expression. The seventh hypothesis found that relational context did not lead to differing amounts of rumination about another’s jealousy expression. Because cross-sex friends often avoid the expression of jealousy (Aune & Comstock, 1991; Bevan, 1999), it was 89 assumed that individuals would react to a cross-sex friend’s jealousy expression by ruminating about the situation to a greater degree than do siblings or daters. However, findings for H7 instead revealed that individuals across relational contexts consistently possessed moderate-to-high amounts of rumination after jealousy was expressed. Combining H7’s findings with those for the eighth hypothesis suggests that the communicative quality of the interaction, rather than the type of relationship it occurs in, plays a more significant role in how much an individual ruminates after his or her partner expresses jealousy. Though not significant, the H7’s results are of value because the vast majority of research on rumination either focuses upon a single relationship (e.g., Cloven & Roloff, 1991; Johnson & Roloff, 1998) or does not examine rumination that arises from a relational situation (e.g., Koole et al., 1999; Nolen-Hoeksema & Jackson, 2001). Thus, little is known about the effect that relational context has on one’s rumination about a partner’s behavior. As such, H7’s findings suggest that, across a variety of close relationships, a situation where one’s partner expresses jealousy leads to rumination. Thus, in terms of jealousy expression, relational context does not seem to exert a strong influence upon the amount to which an individual ruminates about a specific relational event. Despite a lack of support for the seventh hypothesis’s prediction, future research should continue to explore rumination and relational context. Conceivably, other relational situations previously linked to rumination such as serial arguments (Johnson & Roloff, 1998) and conflict episodes (Cloven & Roloff, 1991) might vary in the amount of rumination based upon relational context. Further, how relational contexts might differ in 90 other specific aspects of the rumination process might be of research value. For example, the extent to which close relational partners engage in Martin and Tesser’s (1989) goaldriven theory of rumination behavior sequence might provide increased insight into the role of relational context in the rumination process. As predicted, the eighth hypothesis found that, when another used distributive communication to express jealousy, an individual ruminated more compared with another’s use of integrative communication or negative affect expression. As stated above, the findings for rumination provide evidence that how one’s partner communicates jealousy plays a fairly important role in how much an individual ruminates about an event. The influence that communicative quality seems to have on rumination is consistent with Cloven and Roloff (1991), who made similar claims about roommates in conflict situations. Further, the results for H8 provide additional support for the consistently evidenced relationship between distributive communication and rumination. These two variables are strongly related both when distributive communication is predicted by (e.g., Carson & Cupach, 2000) and associated with (e.g., Cloven & Roloff, 1991) rumination. However, these implications and conclusions must be considered in light of the weak effect size observed. Overall, two theoretical implications for rumination are evident. First, the present results support Carson and Cupach’s (2000) argument that jealousy within romantic relationships constitutes a frustrated goal that stimulates rumination in Martin and Tesser’s (1989) goal-driven theory of rumination. Specifically, the current study broadens Carson and Cupach (2000) to include another’s expression of jealousy as a frustrated goal across platonic, romantic, and familial relationship contexts. Second, the current results 91 inform the debate over the relationship between rumination and negative affect. Some scholars (e.g., Martin & Tesser, 1996a, 1996b) believe that rumination and negative affect covary, whereas others (e.g., McIntosh & Martin, 1992) believe that rumination leads to negative affect. According to the reaction model of jealousy, rumination occurs before negative affect (i.e., jealousy-related emotion) is experienced, which is consistent with McIntosh (1996; McIntosh & Martin, 1992) and provides more insight into the variables relevant to the rumination process. Overall, the findings for rumination replicate the relationship between jealousy and rumination recently established by Carson and Cupach (2000). Further, the relationship between rumination and jealousy expression can be extended to include jealousy expression as an antecedent of rumination in a reactional situation. Future research should continue to explore the relationship between rumination and jealousy, perhaps by examining the impact of other relevant variables related to rumination or mulling, including controllability of negative emotions (e.g., Nolen-Hoeksema & Jackson, 2001), perceived resolvability of the issue (e.g., Johnson & Roloff, 1998), or perception of problem seriousness (e.g., Cloven & Roloff, 1991). The Reaction Model of Jealousy in Close Relationships The final hypothesis examined a proposed path model that included the four proposed consequences of jealousy expression. The original model, which began with close partners’ jealousy expression, stated that general partner uncertainty occurred first, followed by relational uncertainty, which then led to both rumination and jealousy-related emotions. This path model did not fit the data because the predicted path from relational uncertainty to jealousy-related emotion was not significant. Thus, based upon previous 92 research (McIntosh & Martin, 1992), an alternative model where rumination led to jealousy-related emotion was tested. The alternative model did fit the data and was replicated within each relational context and jealousy message conditions. As such, it serves as the reactional model of jealousy in close relationships (see Figure 2). The lack of relationship between relational uncertainty and jealousy-related emotion contrasts Knobloch and Solomon (2000), whose structural equation models examining various uncertainty-increasing events in close relationships found that relational uncertainty was directly and positively related to the experiences of anger, sadness, and fear. However, the mediating nature of rumination is consistent with Knobloch et al. (2001), who found that cognitive jealousy mediated the relationship between relational uncertainty and emotional jealousy. Overall, the inconsistency of the findings for relational uncertainty and jealousy-related emotion suggests that different uncertainty-provoking situations may result in differing experiences for individuals, at least where emotional experience is concerned. In jealousy situations, both cognitive jealousy and rumination serve as mediators between relational uncertainty and jealousyrelated emotion. Why this mediating relationship seems to exist only with regard to jealousy – as opposed to the other positive and negative uncertainty-increasing situations studied by Knobloch and Solomon (2002a) – is unknown. Continued examination of specific cognitive mediators between relational uncertainty and negative emotions would be fruitful for understanding how individuals interpret and process uncomfortable relational situations. As previous research found (Knobloch et al., 2001; Knobloch & Solomon, 2000), general partner uncertainty preceded relational uncertainty in the reaction model of 93 jealousy. Unlike the relationship between relational uncertainty and jealousy-related emotion, the association between partner and relational uncertainty spans relational contexts and situations. For example, one’s own jealousy (Knobloch et al., 2001), the possibility of a new romantic relationship (Knobloch & Solomon, 2002a), or a best friend pointing out that an individual is too busy with a romantic partner to spend time with friends (Knobloch & Solomon, 2002a) each result first in uncertainty about one’s partner, followed by uncertainty about the relationship. The present study also confirmed this association when one’s sibling, cross-sex friend, or dating partner expresses jealousy. Overall, the consistency of these findings informs the process of uncertainty arousal and the associations between partner and relational uncertainty introduced by Knobloch and Solomon (1999). Future research should also include self uncertainty as a potential consequence of another’s jealousy expression (i.e., “Did I do something wrong to make my partner act that way?”) to provide increased insight into uncertainty and jealousy expression processes. As predicted, relational uncertainty preceded rumination about another’s jealousy expression. Although Carson and Cupach (2000) predicted such a link, no known empirical research has studied the association between uncertainty and rumination. Because both are cognitive variables that arise in response to unpleasant situations, it is logical that a positive relationship exists between them. However, because only two of the six internal replications detected a significant path between these two variables, this relationship is the most tenuous of the model. One potential explanation for this weak link is that there is a restriction of range for the rumination variable, meaning that most participants experienced moderate-to-high amounts of rumination across experimental 94 conditions. Thus, the relationship between relational uncertainty and rumination may actually be stronger than what was observed. Overall, the relationship between uncertainty and rumination should continue to be examined in response to other unpleasant instances to determine if it emerges consistently. The final link established as part of the reaction model of jealousy is that rumination leads to the experience of jealousy-related emotion. As discussed previously, rumination researchers have contested the nature of the relationship between rumination and negative affect (which is similar to jealousy-related emotion). For example, Martin and Tesser (1996a, 1996b) argue that rumination and negative affect covary because both variables are influenced by similar factors and because rumination influences individuals’ existing affect instead of creating new affect. In contrast, McIntosh (McIntosh, 1996; McIntosh & Martin, 1992) believes that rumination does lead to negative affect for two reasons: (a) rumination itself is aversive; and (b) rumination can cause individuals to polarize the unpleasant emotions they associate with their inability to attain goals. Findings for the reaction model of jealousy support McIntosh’s argument and help to clarify the rumination process. Overall, the results for the reaction model of jealousy provide increased understanding of the processes individuals go through when their close relational partners express jealousy. No known research has attempted to elucidate a model that explains intrapersonal consequences of another’s jealousy expression, but attempting to understand these cognitive and emotional reactions is important for two reasons. First, this model provides insight into the impact of jealousy expression within close relationships. Second, the model underscores the importance of considering the 95 consequences of another’s jealousy expression. Determining the relationships between the consequences of another’s jealousy expression moves the study of jealousy expression from one that merely identifies the antecedents and correlates of jealousy expression strategies to one that delineates the short- and long-term outcomes of such strategies. Such a characterization rightly broadens the scope of jealousy research to one where jealousy expression is a situation that cognitively and emotionally impacts the jealous target. Limitations and Conclusions Limitations The findings of this dissertation provide interesting and significant implications for the study of jealousy expression in close relationships. Further, they contribute to knowledge about jealousy-related emotion and the theoretical concepts of uncertainty and rumination. However, a number of limitations to the generalizability and usefulness of the findings presented here do exist. Three such limitations are presented below. The first limitation to the current investigation’s findings is the use of hypothetical scenarios. Asking participants to imagine specific events, even if they are to consider actual relational partners, can reduce the external validity of the results. Specifically, the degree to which individuals would naturally react to another’s jealousy expression cannot be definitively ascertained from the data reported here. Further, the possibility that different jealousy expression situations might produce somewhat different results is present. Though utilizing hypothetical scenarios involves external validity limitations, there are three arguments as to why this is not a serious limitation. First, each of the 96 scenarios examined was deemed as significantly more realistic than average and participants agreed that similar situations had occurred in their own relationships at a significantly higher than average rate. Second, as stated previously, there are numerous benefits to hypothetical scenarios, including findings that are not complicated by recall and memory biases, the ability to control the situations participants react to, and the measurement of initial and direct reactions to the situations (Knobloch & Solomon, 2002a). Finally, according to self-perception theory (Bem, 1972), one does not need to be specifically involved in a situation to be able to infer how he or she would react, meaning that participants should be able to accurately surmise their reaction to a hypothetical situation. Overall, however, the findings are meaningful and generalizable only to the extent that the content of the hypothetical situations and the responses provided by participants are representative and accurate. A second limitation involves the difficulty in creating a unidimensional and reliable jealousy-related emotion scale. Though previous research (e.g., Pfeiffer & Wong, 1989; Guerrero et al., 1993; Zammuner & Fischer, 1995) determined the emotions used to conceptualize the scale, internal consistency tests and exploratory factor analyses did little to clarify whether jealousy-related emotion was a unidimensional or multidimensional concept. In this investigation, jealousy-related emotion was a single factor comprised of six related emotions; however, the internal consistency was not strong ( = .71). As such, future research should consider jealousy-related emotion as both a single factor and as multiple factors of distinct emotions (e.g., sadness could be measured using the terms sad, gloomy, and depressed, as in Knobloch & Solomon, 2002a). 97 A final limitation is one that occurs frequently in social science research – the use of a fairly homogenous college undergraduate convenience sample. Indeed, personal relationship research has been criticized for its focus upon younger adults at the expense of the perspective of older individuals (Fingerman & Hay, 2002). Despite this criticism, the current investigation exclusively examined college undergraduates with a mean age of 20.91 years who were from one region of the country and predominantly European American. Thus, findings for the present study can only extrapolated to individuals who share similar demographic characteristics. Though the convenience sample limitation is important, the decision to study college undergraduates was made for two specific reasons. First, cross-sex friendships are widely believed to be the most prevalent during individuals’ college years (e.g., Monsour, 2002; Reeder, 2000; Werking, 1997a), meaning that studying college undergraduates increases the likelihood that most of the sample will have a close, cross-sex friend. Second, sibling research has not extensively studied rivalry patterns between individuals who are in late adolescence or young adulthood. As such, researchers (e.g., Cicirelli, 1982; Dunn, 1985) have expressed the need for sibling rivalry research on an age group that comprises college undergraduates. Thus, a college undergraduate sample for the current study is both appropriate and informative for the cross-sex friend and sibling literature. However, to confidently generalize these findings to other age groups, this research should be extended to younger and older individuals, as well as those who are more ethnically and regionally heterogeneous. 98 Conclusions Findings support the idea that general partner uncertainty, relational uncertainty, rumination, and jealousy-related emotion each arise in response to another’s jealousy expression. For uncertainty, results confirm the importance and robustness of the relationship between uncertainty and jealousy expression and extend previous findings to include uncertainty as a consequence or reaction to jealousy expression. Rumination findings provide additional evidence that a strong relationship between rumination and jealousy expression is present, with jealousy expression serving as both an antecedent and consequence of rumination. In terms of jealousy-related emotion, findings indicate that negative emotions do seem to occur after another expresses jealousy, but understanding the composition of these emotions and their specific relationships to jealousy expression will require continued research. The current findings give rise to several directions for future research. First, how each of the consequences of jealousy expression might differ as a result of another’s use of the remaining eleven jealousy expression strategies (Guerrero et al., 1995; Guerrero & Andersen, 1998b) would be fruitful for continuing to understand the consequences of jealousy expression. For example, previous research has found that using avoidance/denial to express jealousy was positively related to both partner and relational state uncertainty (Afifi & Reichert, 1996). Further, Guerrero and Afifi (1999) reported that the intensity of an individual’s jealousy emotion predicted his or her use of surveillance/restriction and rival contacts. Finally, Carson and Cupach (2000) noted that rumination predicted a number of jealousy expression strategies, including rival contact, compensatory restoration, signs of possession, active distancing, and avoidance/denial. 99 Thus, it is conceivable that another’s use of a jealousy expression method not examined in the current project could also predict an individual’s partner and relational uncertainty, intensity of emotion, and rumination about the situation. In addition, how the consequences of another’s jealousy expression are each managed and communicated by the jealous target would be of interest to jealousy researchers. In other words, the current project’s findings must be extended to include not just the intrapersonal reaction of the individual, but also the way in which he or she communicates this reaction. Such an endeavor will provide more insight into the reactional nature of jealousy expression between close relational partners. Further, it will ultimately extend the reaction model of jealousy to include communicative reactions from both relational partners. In sum, the current project provides valuable insight into the effects of jealousy expression within cross-sex friend, dating, and sibling relationships. In addition, the reaction model of jealousy in close relationships is introduced as a preliminary step toward understanding the process of jealousy expression between both partners in close relationships. Because this model withstood the first stages of empirical scrutiny across multiple conditions, it will hopefully continue to inform the jealousy literature as a useful foundation for building theory about jealousy across close relationships. 100 REFERENCES Afifi, W. A., & Burgoon, J. K. (1998). “We never talk about that”: A comparison of cross-sex friendships and dating relationships on uncertainty and topic avoidance. Personal Relationships, 5, 255-272. Afifi, W. A., & Faulkner, S. L. (2000). On being ‘just friends’: The frequency and impact of sexual activity in cross-sex friendships. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 17, 205-222. Afifi, W. A., & Metts, S. (1998). Characteristics and consequences of expectation violations in close relationships. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 15, 365-392. Afifi, W. A., & Reichert, T. (1996). Understanding the role of uncertainty in jealousy experience and expression. Communication Reports, 9, 93-103. Allan, G. (1977). Sibling solidarity. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 39, 177-184. Andersen, P. A., Eloy, S. V., Guerrero, L. K., & Spitzberg, B. H. (1995). Romantic jealousy and relational satisfaction: A look at the impact of jealousy experience and expression. Communication Reports, 8, 77-85. Arnold, L. B. (1995). Through the narrow pass: Experiencing same-sex friendship in heterosexual(ist) settings. Communication Studies, 46, 234-244. Aune, K. S., & Comstock, J. (1997). Effect of relationship length on the experience, expression, and perceived appropriateness of jealousy. The Journal of Social Psychology, 137, 23-31. 101 Aune, K. S., & Comstock, J. (1991). Experience and expression of jealousy: Comparison between friends and romantics. Psychological Reports, 69, 315-319. Aylor, B., & Dainton, M. (2001). Antecedents in romantic jealousy experience, expression, and goals. Western Journal of Communication, 65, 370-391. Bank, S. P., & Kahn, M. D. (1997). The sibling bond. New York: BasicBooks. Baxter, L. A., & Montgomery, B. M. (1996). Relating: Dialogues and dialectics. New York: Guilford. Bedford, V. H. (1989). A comparison of thematic perceptions of sibling affiliation, conflict, and separation at two periods of adulthood. International Journal of Aging and Human Development, 28, 53-66. Bem, D. J. (1972). Self-perception theory. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology, vol. 6 (pp. 2-67). New York: Academic Press. Berger, C.R. (1987). Communicating under uncertainty. In M.E. Roloff & G.R. Miller (Eds.), Interpersonal processes: New directions in communication research (pp. 39-62). Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Berger, C.R., & Bradac, J.J. (1982). Language and social knowledge: Uncertainty in interpersonal relations. London: Edward Arnold. Berger, C.R., & Calabrese, R.J. (1975). Some explorations in initial interaction and beyond: Toward a developmental theory of interpersonal communication. Human Communication Research, 1, 99-112. Beske, A. L., & Buss, D. M. (2000). Can men and women be just friends? Personal Relationships, 7, 131-151. 102 Bevan, J. L. (1999, November). Understanding the role of intrapersonal uncertainty in the experience and expression of jealousy in cross-sex friendships. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the National Communication Association, Chicago, IL. Bevan, J. L. (2002, November). Expectation violations, predicted outcome value, and sexual resistance in close, cross-sex relationships. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the National Communication Association, New Orleans, LA. Bevan, J. L., & Lannutti, P. J. (2002). The experience and expression of romantic jealousy in homosexual and heterosexual dating relationships. Communication Research Reports, 19, 258-268. Bevan, J. L., & Samter, W. (2001, April). Toward a broader conceptualization of jealousy in close relationships. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Southern States Communication Association, Lexington, KY. Bradac, J. J. (2001). Theory comparison: Uncertainty reduction, problematic integration, uncertainty management, and other curious constructs. Journal of Communication, 51, 456-476. Brashers, D. E. (2001). Communication and uncertainty management. Journal of Communication, 51, 477-497. Bringle, R. G. (1991). Psychosocial aspects of jealousy: A transactional model. In P. Salovey (Ed.), The psychology of jealousy and envy (pp. 103-131). New York: Guilford. Bringle, R. G. (1995). Romantic jealousy. In M. G. Flaherty & C. Ellis (Eds.), Social perspectives on emotion (pp. 225-251). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press. 103 Bringle, R. G., & Buunk, B. (1991). Extradyadic relationships and sexual jealousy. In K. McKinney & S. Sprecher (Eds.), Sexuality in close relationships (pp. 135-153). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Bringle, R. G., & Buunk, B. (1986). Examining the causes and consequences of jealousy: Some recent findings and issues. In R. Gilmour & S. Duck (Eds.), The emerging field of personal relationships (pp. 225-240). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Brody, G. H. (1998). Sibling relationship quality: Its causes and consequences. Annual Review of Psychology, 49, 1-24. Bruess, C. J. S., & Pearson, J. C. (1997). Interpersonal rituals in marriage and adult friendship. Communication Monographs, 64, 25-46. Bryson, J. B. (1991). Modes of responses to jealousy-evoking situations. In P. Salovey (Ed.), The psychology of jealousy and envy (pp. 178-207). New York: Guilford. Buhrmeister, D., & Furman, W. (1987). The development of companionship and intimacy. Child Development, 58, 1101-1113. Buss, D. M. (2000). The dangerous passion: Why jealousy is as important as love and sex. New York: Free Press. Buunk, B., & Bringle, R. G. (1987). Jealousy in love relationships. In D. Perlman & S. Duck (Eds.), Intimate relationships: Development, dynamics, and deterioration (pp. 209-237). Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Buunk, B., & Dijkstra, P. (2001). Evidence from a homosexual sample for a sex-specific rival-oriented mechanism: Jealousy as a function of a rival’s physical attractiveness and dominance. Personal Relationships, 8, 391-406. 104 Carson, C. L., & Cupach, W. R. (2000). Fueling the flames of the green-eyed monster: The role of ruminative thought in reaction to romantic jealousy. Western Journal of Communication, 64, 308-329. Cicirelli, V. G. (1982). Sibling influence throughout the lifespan. In M. E. Lamb & B. Sutton-Smith (Eds.), Sibling relationships: Their nature and significance across the lifespan (pp. 267-284). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Cicirelli, V. G. (1985). Sibling relationships throughout the life cycle. In L. L’Abate (Ed.), The handbook of family psychology and therapy, vol. 1 (pp. 177-214). Homewood, IL: Dorsey Press. Cicirelli, V. G., & Nussbaum, J. F. (1989). Relationships with siblings in later life. In J. F. Nussbaum (Ed.), Life-span communication: Normative processes (pp. 283299). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Clanton, G. (1981). Frontiers of jealousy research. Alternative Lifestyles, 4, 259-273. Cloven, D. H., & Roloff, M. E. (1991). Sense-making activities and interpersonal conflict: Communicative cures for the mulling blues. Western Journal of Speech Communication, 55, 134-158. Constantine, L. L. (1976). Jealousy: From theory to intervention. In D. H. E. Olson (Ed.), Treating relationships (pp. 383-398). Lakeview, IL: Graphic. Dance, F. E. X. (1967). Toward a theory of human communication. In F. E. X. Dance (Ed.), Human communication theory: Original essays (pp. 288-309). New York: Holt, Rinehart, and Winston. Davies, B. (2002). The grief of siblings. In N. B. Webb (Ed.), Helping bereaved children: A handbook for practitioners (2nd ed.) (pp. 94-127). New York: Guilford. 105 Davis, K. E., & Todd, M. J. (1985). Assessing friendship: Prototypes, paradigm cases and relationship description. In S. Duck & D. Perlman (Eds.), Understanding personal relationships: An interdisciplinary approach (pp. 17-38). London: Sage. Davis, K. E., & Todd, M. J. (1982). Friendships and love relationships. In K. E. Davis & T. Mitchell (Eds.), Advances in descriptive psychology, vol. 2 (pp. 79-122). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press. Dunn, J. (1985). Sisters and brothers. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Dunn, J., & Kendrick, C. (1982). Siblings: Love, envy, and understanding. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Dunn, J., & McGuire, S. (1992). Sibling and peer relationships in childhood. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 33, 67-105. Ellis, C., & Weinstein, E. (1986). Jealousy and the social psychology of emotional experience. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 3, 337-357. Ellsworth, P. C., & Smith, C. A. (1988). From appraisal to emotion: Differences among unpleasant feelings. Motivation and Emotion, 12, 271-302. Emmers, T. M., & Canary, D. J. (1996). The effects of uncertainty reducing strategies on young couples’ relational repair and intimacy. Communication Quarterly, 44, 166-182. Fingerman, K. L., & Hay, E. L. (2002). Searching under the streetlight? Age biases in the personal and family relationships literature. Personal Relationships, 9, 415-433. 106 Fitzpatrick, M. A., & Badzinski, D. M. (1994). All in the family: Interpersonal communication in kin relationships. In M. L. Knapp & G. R. Miller (Eds.), Handbook of interpersonal communication, 2nd ed. (pp. 726-771). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Furman, W., & Buhrmeister, D. (1985). Children’s perceptions of the qualities of sibling relationships. Child Development, 56, 448-461. Gaines, S. O., Bledsoe, K. L., Farris, K. R., Henderson, M. C., Kurland, G. J., Lara, J. K., Marelich, W. D., Page, M. S., Palucki, L. J., Steers, W. N., & West, A. M. (1998). Communication of emotion in friendships. In P. A. Andersen & L. K. Guerrero (Eds.), Handbook of communication and emotion: Research, theory, applications, and contexts (pp. 507-531). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Gold, D. T. (1989). Sibling relationships in old age: A typology. International Journal of Aging and Human Development, 28, 37-51. Gold, D.B., & Wegner, D.M. (1995). Origins of ruminative thought: Trauma, incompleteness, nondisclosure, and suppression. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 25, 1245-1261. Goldsmith, D. J. (2001). A normative approach to the study of uncertainty and communication. Journal of Communication, 51, 514-533. Golish, T. D., & Caughlin, J. P. (2002). ‘I’d rather not talk about it:’ Adolescents’ and young adults use of topic avoidance in stepfamilies. Journal of Applied Communication Research, 30, 78-106. Guerrero, L. K. (1998). Attachment-style differences in the experience and expression of romantic jealousy. Personal Relationships, 5, 273-291. 107 Guerrero, L. K., & Afifi, W. A. (1999). Toward a goal-oriented approach for understanding communicative responses to jealousy. Western Journal of Communication, 63, 216-248. Guerrero, L. K., & Afifi, W. A. (1998). Communicative responses to jealousy as a function of self-esteem and relationship maintenance goals: A test of Bryson’s dual motivation model. Communication Reports, 11, 111-122. Guerrero, L. K., & Afifi, W. A. (1995). What parents don’t know: Topic avoidance in parent-child relationships. In T. J. Socha & G. H. Stamp (Eds.), Parents, children, and communication: Frontiers of theory and research (pp. 219-245). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Guerrero, L. K., Andersen, P. A., Jorgensen, P. F., Spitzberg, B. H., & Eloy, S. V. (1995). Coping with the green-eyed monster: Conceptualizing and measuring communicative responses to romantic jealousy. Western Journal of Communication, 59, 270-304. Guerrero, L. K., & Andersen, P. A. (1998a). The dark side of jealousy and envy: Desire, delusion, desperation, and destructive communication. In B. H. Spitzberg & W. R. Cupach (Eds.), The dark side of close relationships (pp. 33-70). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Guerrero, L. K., & Andersen, P. A. (1998b). Jealousy experience and expression in romantic relationships. In P.A. Andersen & L.K. Guerrero (Eds.), Handbook of communication and emotion: Research, theory, applications, and contexts (pp. 155-188). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. 108 Guerrero, L. K., Eloy, S. V., Jorgensen, P F., & Andersen, P. A. (1993). Hers or his? Sex differences in the communication of jealousy in close relationships. In P. Kalbfleisch (Ed.), Interpersonal communication: Evolving interpersonal relationships (pp. 109-131). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Hansen, G. L. (1982). Reactions to hypothetical, jealousy producing events. Family Relations, 31, 513-518. Hansen, G. L. (1985). Dating jealousy among college students. Sex Roles, 12, 713-721. Hansen, G. L. (1991). Jealousy: Its conceptualization, measurement, and integration with family stress theory. In P. Salovey (Ed.), The psychology of jealousy and envy (pp. 211-230). New York: Guilford. Hapworth, W., Hapworth, M., & Heilman, J.R. (1994). “Mom loved you best”: Understanding rivalry and enriching your adult sibling relationships. Middlesex, England: Penguin Books. Hetherington, E. M. (1988). Parents, children, and siblings: Six years after divorce. In R.A. Hinde & J. Stevenson-Hinde (Eds.), Relationships within families: Mutual influences (pp. 311-331). Oxford: Clarendon. Hill, R., & Davis, P. (2000). ‘Platonic jealousy’: A conceptualization and review of the literature on non-romantic pathological jealousy. British Journal of Medical Psychology, 73, 505-517. Hunter, J., & Hamilton, M. (1993). PATH software. Unpublished software, Michigan State University. Hupka, R. B. (1984). Jealousy: Compound emotion or label for a particular situation? Motivation and Emotion, 8, 141-155. 109 Johnson, K. L., & Roloff, M. E. (1998). Serial arguing and relational quality: Determinants and consequences of perceived resolvability. Communication Research, 25, 327-343. Kaplan, D. K., & Keys, C. B. (1997). Sex and relationship variables as predictors of sexual attraction in cross-sex platonic friendships between young and heterosexual adults. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 14, 191-206. Klinger, E. (1996). Theories of thought flow: Points of kinship and fertile contrasts. In R. S. Wyer (Ed.), Advances in social cognition: Ruminative thoughts (pp. 107120). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Knobloch, L. K., & Solomon, D. H. (2002a). Intimacy and the magnitude and experience of episodic relational uncertainty within romantic relationships. Personal Relationships, 9, 457-478. Knobloch, L. K., & Solomon, D. H. (2002b). Information seeking beyond initial interaction: Negotiating relational uncertainty within close relationships. Human Communication Research, 28, 243-257. Knobloch, L. K., & Solomon, D. H. (2000, November). A developmental model of the magnitude and experience of episodic relational uncertainty within romantic relationships. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the National Communication Association, Seattle, WA. Knobloch, L. K., & Solomon, D. H. (1999). Measuring the sources and content of relational uncertainty. Communication Studies, 50, 261-278. 110 Knobloch, L. K., Solomon, D. H., & Cruz, M. G. (2001). The role of relationship development and attachment in the experience of romantic jealousy. Personal Relationships, 8, 205-224. Koole, S. L., Smeets, K., van Knippenberg, A., & Dijksterhuis, A. (1999). The cessation of rumination through self-affirmation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 111-125. Leung, A. K. D., & Robson, W. L. M. (1991). Sibling rivalry. Clinical Pediatrics, 30, 314-317. Mander, G. (1991). Some thoughts on sibling rivalry and competitiveness. British Journal of Psychotherapy, 7, 368-379. Marelich, W. D., Gaines, S. O., & Banzet, M. R. (in press). Commitment, insecurity, and arousability: Testing a transactional model of jealousy. Representative Research in Social Psychology. Martin, L. L., & Tesser, A. (1996a). Some ruminative thoughts. In R. S. Wyer (Ed.), Advances in social cognition: Ruminative thoughts (pp. 1-47). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Martin, L. L., & Tesser, A. (1996b). Clarifying our thoughts. In R. S. Wyer (Ed.), Advances in social cognition: Ruminative thoughts (pp. 189-208). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Martin, L. L., & Tesser, A. (1989). Toward a motivational and structural theory of ruminative thought. In J. S. Uleman (Ed.), Unintended thought (pp. 306-326). New York: Guilford. 111 Masiuch, S., & Kienapple, K. (1993). The emergence of jealousy in children 4 months to 7 years of age. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 10, 421-435. Mathes, E. W., Roter, P. M., & Joerger, S. M. (1982). A convergent validity study of six jealousy scales. Psychological Reports, 50, 1143-1147. McCullough, M. E., Bellah, C. G., Kilpatrick, S. D., & Johnson, J. L. (2001). Vengefulness: Relationships with forgiveness, rumination, well-being, and the big five. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27, 601-610. McCullough, M. E., Rachal, K. C., Sandage, S. J., Worthington, E. L., Brown, S. W., & Hight, T. L. (1998). Interpersonal forgiving in close relationships: II. Theoretical elaboration and measurement. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 1586-1603. McIntosh, W. D. (1996). When does goal nonattainment lead to negative emotional reactions and when doesn’t it? The role of linking and rumination. In L. L. Martin & A. Tesser (Eds.), Striving and feeling: Interactions among goals, affect, and self-regulation (pp. 53-77). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. McIntosh, W. D., & Martin, L. L. (1992). The cybernetics of happiness: The relation of goal attainment, rumination, and affect. In M.S. Clark (Ed.), Emotion and social behavior (pp. 222-246). Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Miller, A. L., Volling, B. L., & McElwain, N. L. (2000). Sibling jealousy in a triadic context with mothers and fathers. Social Development, 9, 433-457. Monsour, M. (2002). Women and men as friends: Relationships across the life span in the 21st century. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. 112 Monsour, M. (1997). Communication and cross-sex friendships across the life cycle: A review of the literature. In B.R. Burleson (Ed.), Communication yearbook 20 (pp. 375-414). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Mullen, P. E., & Martin, J. (1994). Jealousy: A community study. British Journal of Psychiatry, 164, 35-43. Myers, S. A., Black, J., Bukaty, A., Callin, A., Davis, L. A., Fairbanks, S.L., Gieron, M. E., Ferry, M. F., Kappenman, K., King, M., McGuire, M., Nix, F. D., Saniuk, J., Tracy, M. M., Triolo, T. N., & Valentino, T. (2001). Relational maintenance behaviors in the sibling relationship. Communication Quarterly, 49, 19-34. Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Jackson, B. (2001). Mediators of the gender difference in rumination. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 25, 37-47. O’Meara, J. D. (1989). Cross-sex friendship: Four basic challenges of an ignored relationship. Sex Roles, 21, 525-543. Parks, M. R., & Adelman, M. B. (1983). Communication networks and the development of romantic relationships: An expansion of uncertainty reduction theory. Human Communication Research, 10, 55-79. Patford, J. L. (2000). Partners and cross-sex friends: A preliminary study of the way marital and defacto partnerships affect verbal intimacy with cross-sex friends. Journal of Family Studies, 6, 106-119. Pfeiffer, S. M., & Wong, P. T. P. (1989). Multidimensional jealousy. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 6, 181-196. Pines, A., & Aronson, E. (1983). Antecedents, correlates, and consequences of sexual jealousy. Journal of Personality, 51, 108-136. 113 Planalp, S. (1999). Communicating emotion: Social, moral, and cultural processes. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. Planalp, S., & Honeycutt, J. M. (1985). Events that increase uncertainty in personal relationships. Human Communication Research, 11, 593-604. Planalp, S., Rutherford, D. K., & Honeycutt, J. M. (1988). Events that increase uncertainty in personal relationships II: Replication and extension. Human Communication Research, 14, 516-547. Ponzetti, J. J., & James, C. M. (1997). Loneliness and sibling relationships. Journal of Social Behavior and Personality, 12, 103-112. Prisbell, M. (1999). Differences in perceptions of communication across types of relationships. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 89, 275-278. Pulakos, J. (1988). Young adult relationships: Siblings and friends. Journal of Psychology, 123, 237-244. Raffaelli, M. (1992). Sibling conflict in early adolescence. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 54, 652-663. Rawlins, W. K. (1982). Cross-sex friendship and the communicative management of sexrole expectations. Communication Quarterly, 30, 343-353. Reeder, H. M. (2000). ‘I like you…as a friend’: The role of attraction in cross-sex friendship. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 17, 329-348. 114 Ross, H. G., & Milgram, J. I. (1982). Important variables in adult sibling relationships: A qualitative study. In M. E. Lamb & B. Sutton-Smith (Eds.), Sibling relationships: Their nature and significance across the lifespan (pp. 225-249). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Rotenberg, K. J., Shewchuk, V. A., & Kimberley, T. (2001). Loneliness, sex, romantic jealousy, and powerlessness. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 18, 55-79. Sheets, V. L., Fredendall, L. L., & Claypool, H. M. (1997). Jealousy evocation, partner reassurance, and relationship stability: An exploration of the potential benefits of jealousy. Evolution and Human Behavior, 18, 387-402. Smith, L. G., & Clanton, G. (1983). The self-inflicted pain of jealousy. In O. Pocs & R. H. Walsh, (Eds.), Marriage and Family – 1983/84 (pp. 46-49). Dushkin: Guilford, CT. Solomon, D. H., & Knobloch, L. K. (2001). Relationship uncertainty, partner interference, and intimacy within dating relationships. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 18, 804-820. Staske, S. A. (1999). Creating relational ties in talk: The collaborative construction of relational jealousy. Symbolic Interaction, 22, 213-246. Stocker, C. M., & McHale, S. M. (1992). The nature of family correlates of preadolescents’ perceptions of their sibling relationships. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 9, 179-195. 115 Turner, L. H. (1990). The relationship between communication and marital uncertainty: Is “her” marriage different from “his” marriage? Women’s Studies in Communication, 13, 57-83. Werking, K. J. (1997a). Cross-sex friendship as ideological practice. In S. Duck (Ed.), Handbook of personal relationships: Theory, research, and interventions (pp. 391-410). New York: John Wiley. Werking, K. J. (1997b). We’re just good friends: Women and men in nonromantic relationships. New York: Guilford. White, G. L. (1981). A model of romantic jealousy. Motivation and Emotion, 5, 295-310. White, G. L., & Mullen, P. E. (1989). Jealousy: Theory, research, and clinical strategies. New York: Guilford. Wiederman, M. W., Allgeier, E. R., & Ragusa, D. M. (1995). Empirical investigation of the use of the term “jealousy” in survey research. Representative Research in Social Psychology, 20, 15-29. Yareb, P. E., Allgeier, E. R., & Sensibaugh, C. C. (1999). Looking deeper: Extradyadic behaviors, jealousy, and perceived unfaithfulness in hypothetical dating relationships. Personal Relationships, 6, 305-316. Yoshimura, S. M., Guerrero, L. K., & Trost, M. R. (1999, November). Toward a model of jealousy experience and expression: Associations between threat, emotion, cognition, goals, communication, and relational consequences. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the National Communication Association, Chicago, IL. Zammuner, V. L., & Fischer, A. H. (1995). The social regulation of emotions in jealousy situations. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 26, 189-208. 116 APPENDIX A EXAMPLE OF INSTRUMENT USED IN PILOT STUDY I (CROSS-SEX FRIEND CONDITION) In this study, you will be asked to read a number of messages and scenarios and then respond to questions about each message. It is CRITICAL that you read each message carefully, and answer all questions thoroughly and completely. Please provide answers to ALL sections of the questionnaire. As you answer the questions following a given message, you may feel free to look back at the message that you just read. Think of your present closest friend of the opposite sex (called a cross-sex friend) when completing this questionnaire. Think of a friend when answering these questions, not a romantic partner unless you were friends first; if you do this, recall the friendship, not the romantic relationship. However, you may think of a cross-sex friendship that has a physical or romantic component, but the relationship is not considered to be “boyfriend” and “girlfriend.” Remember — think of only ONE opposite sex friend as you answer these questions!! Write your cross-sex friend’s initials here: ________ Please read the following situation carefully. As you read it, imagine that a similar situation has occurred in between you and your own cross-sex friend. Your cross-sex friend __________ (write initials here) approaches you and tells you that he/she is jealous that you are showing interest in doing things with another friend of the opposite sex. Now we would like you to answer the following questions as if the situation you have just read has actually happened between you and your cross-sex friend. Genuinely think about how you would feel if this scenario occurred between you and the cross-sex friend that you have chosen. How realistic do you think this situation is? 1 Very Realistic 2 3 4 Somewhat Realistic 5 6 7 Not At All Realistic 117 Can you imagine that this situation would actually occur? 1 2 3 Yes 4 5 6 7 Maybe No This situation is easy to imagine. 1 2 3 Strongly Agree 4 5 6 Neutral 7 Strongly Disagree How often has this situation occurred in your own cross-sex friendship? 1 2 3 Never 4 5 6 Sometimes 7 Frequently How frequently does a situation like this occur in your own cross-sex friendship? 1 2 3 Not At All Frequently 4 5 6 Somewhat Frequently 7 Very Frequently I have often experienced a situation like this one in my cross-sex friendship. 1 Strongly Agree 2 3 4 5 Neutral **Please continue to the next page of the questionnaire** 6 7 Strongly Disagree 118 Please now read the following message that your cross-sex friend might use to communicate his/her jealousy to you and answer the questions that follow. Your cross-sex friend communicates this jealousy by calmly disclosing his/her feelings to you, trying to reach an understanding with you, and openly discussing the situation. The way my cross-sex friend communicated his/her jealousy is: Circle one of the three options directly below: NEGATIVE NEUTRAL POSITIVE The way my cross-sex friend communicated his/her jealousy is: completely negative 1 completely negative 1 very easy to imagine 1 completely positive 1 completely realistic 1 neutral 2 3 4 6 6 not at all negative 7 6 very difficult to imagine 7 6 not at all positive 7 6 not at all realistic 7 neutral 2 3 4 5 neutral 2 3 4 5 neutral 2 3 4 5 neutral 2 3 4 5 6 bad 7 5 6 unpleasant 7 6 does not happen to me at all 7 good 1 2 3 neutral 4 pleasant 1 2 3 neutral 4 happens to me a lot 1 5 completely positive 7 5 neutral 2 3 4 5 119 Please now read the following message that your cross-sex friend might use to communicate his/her jealousy to you and answer the questions that follow. Your cross-sex friend communicates this jealousy by acting anxious, looking hurt, and crying in front of you. The way my cross-sex friend communicated his/her jealousy is: Circle one of the three options directly below: NEGATIVE NEUTRAL POSITIVE The way my cross-sex friend communicated his/her jealousy is: completely negative 1 completely negative 1 very easy to imagine 1 completely positive 1 completely realistic 1 neutral 2 3 pleasant 1 happens to me a lot 1 5 6 6 not at all negative 7 6 very difficult to imagine 7 6 not at all positive 7 neutral 2 3 4 5 neutral 2 3 4 5 neutral 2 3 4 5 neutral 2 3 4 5 6 not at all realistic 7 2 3 neutral 4 5 6 bad 7 3 neutral 4 6 unpleasant 7 6 does not happen to me at all 7 good 1 4 completely positive 7 2 5 neutral 2 3 4 5 120 Please now read the following message that your cross-sex friend might use to communicate his/her jealousy to you and answer the questions that follow. Your cross-sex friend communicates this jealousy by accusing you, making rude comments, and arguing with you. The way my cross-sex friend communicated his/her jealousy is: Circle one of the three options directly below: NEGATIVE NEUTRAL POSITIVE The way my cross-sex friend communicated his/her jealousy is: completely negative 1 completely negative 1 very easy to imagine 1 completely positive 1 completely realistic 1 neutral 2 3 pleasant 1 happens to me a lot 1 5 6 6 not at all negative 7 6 very difficult to imagine 7 6 not at all positive 7 neutral 2 3 4 5 neutral 2 3 4 5 neutral 2 3 4 5 neutral 2 3 4 5 6 not at all realistic 7 2 3 neutral 4 5 6 bad 7 3 neutral 4 6 unpleasant 7 6 does not happen to me at all 7 good 1 4 completely positive 7 2 5 neutral 2 3 4 5 121 Please now read the following message that your cross-sex friend might use to communicate his/her jealousy to you and answer the questions that follow. Your cross-sex friend communicates this jealousy by giving you the silent treatment, storming out of the room, and giving you dirty looks. The way my cross-sex friend communicated his/her jealousy is: Circle one of the three options directly below: NEGATIVE NEUTRAL POSITIVE The way my cross-sex friend communicated his/her jealousy is: completely negative 1 completely negative 1 very easy to imagine 1 completely positive 1 completely realistic 1 neutral 2 3 pleasant 1 happens to me a lot 1 5 6 6 not at all negative 7 6 very difficult to imagine 7 6 not at all positive 7 neutral 2 3 4 5 neutral 2 3 4 5 neutral 2 3 4 5 neutral 2 3 4 5 6 not at all realistic 7 2 3 neutral 4 5 6 bad 7 3 neutral 4 6 unpleasant 7 6 does not happen to me at all 7 good 1 4 completely positive 7 2 5 neutral 2 3 4 5 122 Please now read the following message that your cross-sex friend might use to communicate his/her jealousy to you and answer the questions that follow. Your cross-sex friend communicates this jealousy by avoiding contact with you and denying that he/she is jealous. The way my cross-sex friend communicated his/her jealousy is: Circle one of the three options directly below: NEGATIVE NEUTRAL POSITIVE The way my cross-sex friend communicated his/her jealousy is: completely negative 1 completely negative 1 very easy to imagine 1 completely positive 1 completely realistic 1 neutral 2 3 pleasant 1 happens to me a lot 1 5 6 6 not at all negative 7 6 very difficult to imagine 7 6 not at all positive 7 neutral 2 3 4 5 neutral 2 3 4 5 neutral 2 3 4 5 neutral 2 3 4 5 6 not at all realistic 7 2 3 neutral 4 5 6 bad 7 3 neutral 4 6 unpleasant 7 good 1 4 completely positive 7 2 5 neutral 2 3 4 does not happen to me at all 5 6 123 Please now read the following message that your cross-sex friend might use to communicate his/her jealousy to you and answer the questions that follow. Your cross-sex friend communicates this jealousy by threatening to harm you, scaring you, and being rough with you. The way my cross-sex friend communicated his/her jealousy is: Circle one of the three options directly below: NEGATIVE NEUTRAL POSITIVE The way my cross-sex friend communicated his/her jealousy is: completely negative 1 completely negative 1 very easy to imagine 1 completely positive 1 completely realistic 1 neutral 2 3 4 6 6 not at all negative 7 6 very difficult to imagine 7 6 not at all positive 7 6 not at all realistic 7 neutral 2 3 4 5 neutral 2 3 4 5 neutral 2 3 4 5 neutral 2 3 4 5 6 bad 7 5 6 unpleasant 7 good 1 2 3 neutral 4 pleasant 1 2 3 neutral 4 happens to me a lot 1 5 completely positive 7 5 neutral 2 3 4 does not happen to me at all 5 6 124 Finally, please answer some questions about yourself. How old are you? _________________ (in years) What is your gender? (please circle one) Male Female What is your ethnic background or race? (circle number) 1 Asian 3 Hispanic 5 White 2 Black/African American 4 Native American 6 Other (please specify): ____________________________ What is your year in school? (circle number) 1 Freshman 3 Junior 5 5th Year Senior 2 Sophomore 4 Senior 6 Graduate Student Please place your completed survey facedown in the box by the door. THANK YOU VERY MUCH FOR YOUR PARTICIPATION! 125 APPENDIX B ORIGINAL DEFINITIONS AND EXAMPLES FROM GUERRERO ET AL. (1995) AND JEALOUSY EXPRESSION MESSAGES TESTED IN PILOT STUDY I Jealousy Expression Strategy and Definition Examples Pilot-Tested Message Specifically, your relational partner communicates this jealousy by… Integrative Communication: Direct, prosocial, nonthreatening responses to jealousy Negative Affect Expression: Nonverbal expression of jealousy-related emotion Distributive Communication: Direct, negative, antisocial jealousy responses Violent Communication/Threats: Direct, threatening responses to jealousy involving threats or actually hurting the partner Explaining, disclosing, discussing, trying to reach an understanding calmly disclosing his/her feelings to you, trying to reach an understanding with you, and openly discussing the situation. Displaying insecurity, appearing hurt, sad, or depressed, crying or sulking acting anxious, looking hurt, and crying in front of you. Yelling, cursing, acting accusing you, making rude, quarreling or arguing, rude comments, and making hurtful comments arguing with you. Using physical force, threatening to harm threatening to harm partner, you, scaring you, and pushing, shoving, hitting being rough with you. partner 126 Active Distancing: Indirect and threatening jealousy responses Avoidance/Denial: Indirect, nonthreatening jealousy responses Giving partner cold or dirty looks, physically pulling away, ignoring partner, giving partner silent treatment giving you the silent treatment, storming out of the room, and giving you dirty looks. Becoming silent, denying jealousy, pretending nothing is wrong avoiding contact with you and denying that he/she is jealous. 127 APPENDIX C EXAMPLE OF INSTRUMENT USED IN PILOT STUDY II (DATING PARTNER CONDITION) In this study, you will be asked to read a scenario about a close relationship in your life and then respond to questions it. It is CRITICAL that you read the scenario carefully, and answer all questions thoroughly and completely. Please provide answers to ALL sections of the questionnaire. Think of your present dating partner when completing this questionnaire. A dating partner is someone you have dated for one week or more – going on one or two dates with another person would constitute a dating relationship for this questionnaire. Do not think of an opposite-sex friend when completing this questionnaire. Remember — think of only ONE dating partner as you answer these questions!! Write your dating partner’s initials here: ________ Please read the following situation carefully. As you read it, imagine that a similar situation has occurred between you and your own dating partner. Because of circumstances beyond your control (for example, work or a school project), you have been spending a lot of time with someone that you recently met – who is just the type of person that you would date. By the way your dating partner __________ (write initials here) is acting, it is obvious that he/she is upset and bothered by the fact that you are showing interest in spending time with this person. Your dating partner obviously sees this person as a potential dating partner for you. Now we would like you to answer the following questions as if the situation you have just read has actually happened between you and your dating partner. Genuinely think about how you would feel if this scenario occurred between you and the dating partner that you have chosen. How realistic do you think this situation is? 1 Very Realistic 2 3 4 Somewhat Realistic 5 6 7 Not At All Realistic 128 How often has this situation occurred in your own dating relationship? 1 2 3 Never 4 5 6 Sometimes 7 Frequently A similar situation has occurred at some point in my own dating relationship. 1 2 3 Strongly Agree 4 5 6 Neutral 7 Strongly Disagree This situation is easy to imagine. 1 2 3 Strongly Agree 4 5 6 Neutral 7 Strongly Disagree How frequently does a situation like this occur in your own dating relationship? 1 2 3 Not At All Frequently 4 5 6 Somewhat Frequently 7 Very Frequently I have encountered a situation like this at least once in my own dating relationship. 1 2 3 Strongly Agree 4 5 6 Neutral 7 Strongly Disagree Can you imagine that this situation would actually occur? 1 2 3 Yes 4 5 6 Maybe 7 No I have often experienced a situation like this one in my dating relationship. 1 Strongly Agree 2 3 4 Neutral 5 6 7 Strongly Disagree Now, please briefly describe a situation similar to the one on the previous page that actually occurred between you and your dating partner. If you have never experienced a situation like this, please write about any time when your dating partner made it clear to you that he/she was upset with you about something. You may continue on the back of this page if you wish. 129 Finally, please answer some questions about yourself. How old are you? _________________ (in years) What is your gender? (please circle one) Male Female What is your ethnic background or race? (circle number) 1 Asian 3 Hispanic 5 White 2 Black/African American 4 Native American 6 Other (please specify): ____________________________ What is your year in school? (circle number) 1 Freshman 3 Junior 5 5th Year Senior 2 Sophomore 4 Senior 6 Graduate Student THANK YOU VERY MUCH FOR YOUR PARTICIPATION! 130 APPENDIX D EXAMPLE OF INSTRUMENT USED IN MAIN INVESTIGATION (SIBLINGNEGATIVE AFFECT EXPRESSION CONDITION) Think of the brother or sister who is closest in age to you (either older or younger) when completing this questionnaire. You and your sibling should share the same mother and father. If you do not have a sibling who shares the same biological parents as you do, you may think of an adopted sibling, a step-sibling, or a half-sibling who is closest in age to you. Remember — think of only ONE sibling as you answer these questions!! Write your sibling’s initials here: ________ Please read the following situation carefully. As you read it, imagine that a similar situation has occurred between you and your own sibling. Your parents have recently been focusing on you and giving you a lot of attention. By the way your sibling __________ (write initials here) is acting, it is obvious that he/she is hurt and bothered by the fact that you are receiving more attention and affection from your parents. Your sibling obviously believes that your parents prefer you to him/her. Specifically, your sibling communicates this jealousy by acting anxious, looking hurt, and crying in front of you. PART 1: The Situation Now we would like you to answer the following questions as if the situation you have just read has actually happened between you and sibling. Genuinely think about how you would feel if this scenario occurred between you and sibling that you have chosen. How realistic do you think this situation is? 1 2 Not At All Realistic 3 4 Somewhat Realistic 5 6 7 Very Realistic 131 How often has this situation occurred in your own sibling relationship? 1 Never 2 3 4 Sometimes 5 6 7 Frequently A similar situation has occurred at some point in my own sibling relationship. 1 Strongly Disagree 2 3 4 Neutral 5 6 7 Strongly Agree 3 4 Neutral 5 6 7 Strongly Agree This situation is easy to imagine. 1 Strongly Disagree 2 How frequently does a situation like this occur in your own sibling relationship? 1 2 Not At All Frequently 3 4 Somewhat Frequently 5 6 7 Very Frequently I have encountered a situation like this at least once in my own sibling relationship. 1 Strongly Disagree 2 3 4 Neutral 5 6 7 Strongly Agree 6 7 Yes Can you imagine that this situation would actually occur? 1 No 2 3 4 Maybe 5 I have often experienced a situation like this one in my sibling relationship. 1 Strongly Disagree 2 3 4 Neutral 5 6 7 Strongly Agree 132 PART 2: The Communication Message Used by Your Sibling Now, please consider the specific way that your sibling expressed to you that he or she was upset or bothered when answering the following questions. You may look back at the message you just read if you wish. The way my sibling communicated that he/she was upset or bothered is: Circle one of the three options directly below: NEGATIVE NEUTRAL POSITIVE The way my sibling communicated that he/she was upset or bothered is: completely negative 1 completely negative 1 very difficult to imagine 1 completely positive 1 not at all realistic 1 neutral 2 unpleasant 1 4 5 6 6 not at all negative 7 6 very easy to imagine 7 6 not at all positive 7 6 completely realistic 7 neutral 2 3 4 5 neutral 2 3 4 5 neutral 2 3 4 5 neutral 2 3 2 neutral 3 2 3 bad 1 3 completely positive 7 4 5 4 5 6 good 7 neutral 4 5 6 pleasant 7 133 Part 3: Your Thoughts and Feelings about the Situation: Now, please report about how uncertain or unsure you might feel about your sibling if this situation actually occurred in your own relationship. If this situation actually occurred, I would feel as if I did not know my sibling very well. Strongly Disagree 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Strongly Agree If this situation actually occurred, I would be certain about what I can or cannot say to my sibling in this relationship. Strongly Disagree 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Strongly Agree If this situation actually occurred, I would be confident of my ability to accurately predict my sibling’s behavior. Strongly Disagree 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Strongly Agree If this situation actually occurred, I would be certain about whether or not my sibling and I feel the same way about each other. Strongly Disagree 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Strongly Agree If this situation actually occurred, my sibling would have said or done something which surprised me. Strongly Disagree 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Strongly Agree If this situation actually occurred, I would be certain about the boundaries for appropriate and/or inappropriate behavior in this sibling relationship. Strongly Disagree 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Strongly Agree If this situation actually occurred, I would have a very good idea of what my sibling’s values and preferences were. Strongly Disagree 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Strongly Agree 134 If this situation actually occurred, I would be certain about how I can act in the presence of my sibling. Strongly Disagree 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Strongly Agree If this situation actually occurred, I would be certain about how my sibling and I view this relationship. Strongly Disagree 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Strongly Agree If this situation actually occurred, I would have trouble understanding why my sibling did what he or she did. Strongly Disagree 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Strongly Agree If this situation actually occurred, I would be certain about the norms for this sibling relationship. Strongly Disagree 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Strongly Agree If this situation actually occurred, I would be able to accurately predict what my sibling’s attitudes are. Strongly Disagree 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Strongly Agree If this situation actually occurred, I would be certain about whether or not my sibling likes me as much as I like him or her. Strongly Disagree 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Strongly Agree If this situation actually occurred, I would be able to tell what my sibling was feeling inside. Strongly Disagree 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Strongly Agree If this situation actually occurred, I would be certain about how I could or could not behave around my sibling. Strongly Disagree 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Strongly Agree 135 If this situation actually occurred, I would be able to accurately predict how my sibling would respond to me in most situations. Strongly Disagree 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Strongly Agree If this situation actually occurred, I would be certain about the current status of this sibling relationship. Strongly Disagree 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Strongly Agree If this situation actually occurred, I would be certain about the rules of this sibling relationship Strongly Disagree 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Strongly Agree Please indicate the extent to which you would you experience the following emotions if this situation actually occurred in your own sibling relationship by putting an X or a check mark on the line that best represents how you would feel. If this situation occurred between my sibling and I, I would feel: Not sad at all ______ : ______ : _____ : _____ : _____ : _____ : _____ Very sad Not angry at all ______ : ______ : _____ : _____ : _____ : _____ : _____ Very angry Not frustrated at all _____ : ______ : _____ : _____ : _____ : _____ : _____ Very frustrated Not guilty at all ______ : ______ : _____ : _____ : _____ : _____ : _____ Very guilty Not fearful at all ______ : ______ : _____ : _____ : _____ : _____ : _____ Very fearful Not insecure at all ______ : ______ : _____ : _____ : _____ : _____ : _____ Very insecure Not surprised at all _____ : ______ : _____ : _____ : _____ : _____ : _____ Very surprised 136 We are next interested in how frequently you would think about the situation you just read had it actually occurred in your own sibling relationship. Please circle the number that best represents how frequently you would think about or worry about the situation. 1 Would not worry at all about the situation 2 3 4 Neutral 5 6 7 Would worry very much about the situation 1 2 Thoughts of the situation would not interfere with daily activity 3 4 Neutral 5 6 7 Thoughts of the situation would interfere a lot with daily activity 1 2 Would put no effort into mentally appraising the situation 3 4 Neutral 5 6 1 2 No time would be spent reflecting on this problem 3 4 Neutral 5 6 7 A lot of time would be spent reflecting on this problem 1 2 Would not worry at all about the situation 3 4 Neutral 5 6 7 Would worry very much about the situation 1 2 Would never think about this situation 3 4 Neutral 5 6 The situation that I read involved my: Cross-sex friend (circle one) Sibling Sibling 7 Would put very much effort into mentally appraising the situation 7 Would think about this situation all the time 137 Part 4: Your Own Sibling Relationship Please answer the following questions about your sibling and your relationship with him/her. My sibling is: Female Male How old is your sibling? _____________ (in years) The category that best your sibling is: _________Gay _________Lesbian __________Bisexual ___________Straight _________Transgendered __________Other (Please specify)_____________________ How long have you known your sibling? ______________ How frequently are you in contact with your sibling? (circle number) 1 Daily 2 Weekly 4 Several times a year 3 Monthly 5 Once a year or less How close are you to your sibling? 1 Not Close At All 2 3 4 Neutral 5 6 7 Very Close 6 7 Very Satisfied How satisfied are you with your sibling relationship? 1 2 Not Satisfied At All 3 4 Neutral 5 How many adopted siblings do you have? __________ How many half-siblings do you have (i.e., you biologically share one parent)? __________ How many step-siblings do you have (i.e., your parents are remarried to one another but you do not biologically share them with your siblings)? _____________ 138 Please write how old each of your siblings are and their genders (you may write on the back of this sheet if you need more room): Age: ___________________________ Age: ___________________________ Age: ___________________________ Age: ___________________________ Gender: __________________________ Gender: __________________________ Gender: __________________________ Gender: __________________________ Please indicate which type of sibling you reported on in this questionnaire: Full Half Adopted Step What best describes your current living situation with the sibling that you reported on? 1 3 Live with him/her year round Never live with him/her _____________________ 2 Live with him/her when not in school 4 Other (describe): What best describes your parents’ current relationship with one another? Please consider the set of parents that you and the sibling you reported on are both associated with. (circle number) 1 3 4 6 Married 2 Unmarried but romantically involved with each other Separated 4 Divorced, neither parent remarried Divorced, one parent remarried 5 Divorced, both parents remarried Other (please describe): ________________________________ Part 5: Information about You Please answer the following questions about yourself. I am: Female Male The category that best describes me is: _________Gay _________Lesbian __________Bisexual ___________Straight _________Transgendered __________Other (Please specify)_____________________ How old are you? __________ (in years) 139 Which ethnic background or race best describes you? (circle number) 1 Asian 3 Hispanic 5 White 2 Black/African American 4 Native American 6 Other (please specify): ____________________________ What is your year in school? (circle number) 1 Freshman 3 Junior 5 5th Year Senior 2 Sophomore 4 Senior 6 Graduate Student THANK YOU VERY MUCH FOR YOUR PARTICIPATION!