leopard-formatted

advertisement
Behavioral and neurochemical changes in the zebrafish leopard strain
Caio Maximino1,2*, Bruna Puty1, Vanessa Miranda3, Karen Renata Matos Oliveira1, Anderson Manoel
Herculano1,2**
1
Laboratório de Neuroendocrinologia, Instituto de Ciências Biológicas, Universidade Federal do Pará
2
Zebrafish Neuroscience Research Consortium
3
Faculdade de Biotecnologia, Instituto de Ciências Biológicas, Universidade Federal do Pará
* and ** - Corresponding authors
Caio Maximino
caio@ufpa.br
Anderson Manoel Herculano
herculano@ufpa.br
Laboratório de Neuroendocrinologia
Instituto de Ciências Biológicas
Universidade Federal do Pará
Av.
Augusto
Corrêa,
01
–
66075-110
1
–
Belém,
PA,
Brazil
Behavioral and neurochemical changes in the zebrafish leopard phenotype
Abstract
The zebrafish leopard phenotype (leo) displays abnormal pigmentation and increased anxiety-like
behavior. The neurochemical changes associated with this anxious phenotype are not known. Here, we
demonstrate that leo show increased anxiety-like behavior in the light/dark box and in the novel tank
test. This anxious phenotype is rescued by acute treatment with a dose of a serotonin reuptake inhibitor,
fluoxetine, that is inactive in wild-type animals. Moreover, leo show decreased tissue levels of
serotonin, increased serotonin turnover, and slightly increased monoamine oxidase activity. These
results suggest that the anxious phenotype observed in leo zebrafish is caused by a decrease in
serotonin uptake.
Keywords: strain, zebrafish, anxiety, scototaxis, serotonin uptake
2
Introduction
Anxiety-like behavior in animals is controlled by a multiplicity of genes, leading to a plethora of strain
effects in different organisms. In zebrafish, for example, increased anxiety-like behavior has been
described in the leopard skin mutant, leo (Cachat et al. 2011, Egan et al. 2009), which present spots
instead of stripes (Gilbert 2003). This phenotype has been described as caused by mutations in the
cx41.8 gene which codes for a orthologue of the mammalian connexin 40 (Watanabe et al. 2006).
Connexins are multimeric proteins which mediate cell-to-cell signaling by forming hemichannels and
gap junctions; in the central nervous system, they mediate electrical coupling between neurons,
metabolic coupling between glial cells, as well as the release of gliotransmitters such as glutamate and
ATP (Sáez et al. 2003a, 2003b). Mice with astrocyte-targeted innactivation of connexin 43 show a
phenotype of motor impairment and increased exploratory behavior in an open-field, but not in the
elevated plus-maze (Frisch et al. 2003). Deletion of cx30, a protein which is expressed in astrocytes,
increases centrophobism/thigmotaxis in mice associated with decreased serotonin turnover in the
hippocampus (Dere et al. 2003).
It is not known if the cx41.8 mutation is the only capable of generating the leopard phenotype.
Nonetheless, the increased anxiety-like behavior of this strain suggests that they may be an important
tool in understanding the genetic underpinnings of anxiety and stress disorders. In spite of these initial
observations, the neurochemical bases of this behavioral phenotype was not investigated. An important
system in the control of anxiety is the serotonergic system (Maximino, 2012), suggesting an avenue of
inquiry to interpret the neurochemical correlates of strain-dependent anxiety-like behavior in zebrafish.
In this article, we test whether this phenotype is generalized to other behavioral contexts thought to
reflect anxiety/fear, and whether it is associated with changes in the serotonergic system.
Methods
3
Drugs and reagents
Fluoxetine was bought from Roche (Brazil). HPLC standards (5-HT, 5-HIAA and DHBA) and
kynuramine hydrobromide were bought from Sigma (St. Louis, USA). HPLC-grade methanol was
bought from Tedia (Fairfield, USA), and biotechnology-grade sodium dodecyl sulfate was bought from
Amresco (Solon, USA). All other reagents were analytical-grade and bought from Synth (Diadema,
Brazil).
Animals
Wild-type longfin (n = 20) and leopard mutant (n = 20) zebrafish, bought from the same local
ornamental aquarium shop, were used in the experiments. Phenotypically, leo zebrafish were identified
as homozygous t1 variants (Haffter et al., 1996; Watanabe et al., 2006), although this allele has been
identified in Tü, and not WT, zebrafish. Animals were collectively maintained in 40 L tanks, separated
by stripe pattern phenotype, for at least two weeks before onset of experiments. Tanks were kept at
constant temperature (28 °C), oxygenation, and light cycle (14:10 LD photoperiod). Animals were fed
daily with Alcon flake food, and twice a week with live brine shrimp.
Behavioral assays
Each animal was subjected to both behavioral tests, in a randomized and balanced order. The maximum
delay between tests was 3 min.
Scototaxis. Determination of drug effects on scototaxis were carried as described elsewhere (Araújo et
al. 2012). Briefly, after drug injection and effect onset animals were transferred to the central
compartment of a black and white tank (15 cm X 10 cm X 45 cm h X d X l) for a 3-min. acclimation
period, after which the doors which delimit this compartment were removed and the animal was
allowed to freely explore the apparatus for 15 min. The following variables were recorded:
4
time on the white compartment: the time spent in the white half of the tank (percentage of the
trial);
white avoidance habituation: the cumulative habituation ratio (CHR; Stewart et al., 2013), that
is, the modulus of the difference between the total time spent in the white compartment in the
last three and the first three minutes of the assay.
squares crossed: the number of 10 cm² squares crossed by the animal in the white compartment;
locomotion habituation: the CHR between the total number of squares crossed in the last three
and the first three minutes of the assay.
latency to white: the amount of time the animal spends in the black compartment before its first
entry in the white compartment (s);
entries in white compartment: the number of entries the animal makes in the white compartment
in the whole session;
erratic swimming: the number of “erratic swimming” events, defined as a zig-zag, fast,
unpredictable course of swimming of short duration; and
freezing: the proportional duration of freezing events (in % of time in the white compartment),
defined as complete cessation of movements with the exception of eye and operculae
movements.
thigmotaxis: the proportional duration of thigmotaxis events (in % of time in the white
compartment), defined as swimming in a distance of 2 cm or less from the white compartment’s
walls.
risk assessment: the number of “risk assessment” events, defined as a fast (<1 s) entry in the
white compartment followed by re-entry in the black compartment, or as a partial entry in the
white compartment (i.e., the pectoral fin does not cross the midline).
Novel tank test. The protocol for the novel tank diving test used was modified from Egan et al. (Egan
5
et al. 2009). Briefly, after drug injection animals (n = 9-10 per treatment) were transferred to the test
apparatus, which consisted of a 15 x 25 x 20 cm (width x length x height) tank lighted from above by
two 25 W fluorescent lamps, producing an average of 120 lumens above the tank. As soon as the
animals were transferred to the apparatus, a webcam was activated and behavioral recording begun.
The webcam filmed the apparatus from the front, thus recording the animal’s lateral and vertical
distribution. Animals were allowed to freely explore the novel tank for 6 minutes, after which they
were removed from it and exposed to the scototaxis tank. Video files were later analyzed by
experimenters blind to the treatment using X-Plo-Rat 2005 (http://scotty.ffclrp.usp.br), and the images
were divided in a 3 x 3 grid composed of 10 cm² squares. The following variables were recorded:
time on top: the time spent in the top third of the tank;
bottom-dwelling habituation: the minute-by-minute time spent in the top third of the tank
(Wong et al. 2010).
squares crossed: the number of 10 cm² squares crossed by the animal during the entire session;
erratic swimming: the number of “erratic swimming” events, defined as a zig-zag, fast,
unpredictable course of swimming of short duration; and
freezing: the total duration of freezing events, defined as complete cessation of movements with
the exception of eye and operculae movements.
Nervous tissue parcellation
Brains of WT and leo animals were dissected on ice-cold magnesium- and calcium-free phosphatebuffered saline (MCF) after sacrifice and homogenized in 200 µl of cold solution containing HClO4
70% [0.2 N], 10 mg EDTA, 9.5 mg sodium metabissulfite. The homogenates were briefly centrifuged
at 500 g for 5 min at 4 ºC, and supernatant aliquots were filtered through a 0.2 µm syringe filter. The
resulting samples were frozen at -20 ºC for posterior analysis in the HPLC.
6
For the formation of synaptossomal pellets for monoamine oxidase assays, brains were transferred to 1
mL TBS 50 mM, sonicated, and centrifuged at 1,000 g for 10 min at 4 °C. The supernatant was then
withdrawn and centrifuged at 17,000 g for 20 min at 4°C to form the synaptossomal pellet.
HPLC analysis of monoamines
Serotonin, 5-HIAA and 3,4-dihydroxybenzylamine (DHBA) (50 mg) were dissolved in 100 mL of
eluting solution (HClO4 70% [0.2 N], 10 mg EDTA, 9.5 mg sodium metabissulfite) and frozen at -20
°C, to later be used as standards. The HPLC system consisted of a delivery pump (LC20-AT,
Shimadzu), a 20 μL sample injector (Rheodyne), a degasser (DGA-20A5), and an analytical column
(Shimadzu Shim-Pack VP-ODS, 250 x 4.6 mm internal diameter). The integrating recorder was a
Shimadzu CBM-20A (Shimadzu, Kyoto, Japan). An electrochemical detector (Model L-ECD-6A) with
glassy carbon was used at a voltage setting of +0.72 V, with a sensitivity set at 2 nA full deflection. The
mobile phase consisted of a solution of 70 mM phosphate buffer (pH 2.9), 0.2 mM EDTA, 34.6765
mM SDS, 10% HPLC-grade methanol and 20% sodium metabissulfite as a conservative. The column
temperature was set at 17 °C, and the isocratic flow rate was 1.6 ml/min. 0.5 mL of extracellular fluid
solution (ECF) was mixed with 0.5 mL of eluting solution, filtered through a 0.22 µm syringe filter,
spiked with 0.22 µl of 2.27 mM DHBA (internal standard) and then injected into the HPLC system.
Protein content was assessed in the same samples as described elsewhere (Zor & Selinger, 1996)
Monoamine oxidase activity
The determination of MAO activity was done according to Weissbach et al. (1960). Synaptosomal
preparations were suspended in a reaction buffer containing 110 µM of kynuramine hydrobromide (~2
x Km; Aldeco et al., 2011) in 0.5 M phosphate buffered at pH 7.4. After the addition of kynuramine, an
initial reading was made at 360 nm using a Spectrum Meter SP2000 UV-VIS spectrophotometer, at a
7
temperature of 25 ºC. Posterior readings were made at 5-min. intervals until the absorbance
disappeared. Protein content was assessed in the same samples as described elsewhere (Zor & Selinger,
1996). Specific activity was defined as the amount of kynuramine consumed in one minute per mg of
protein, using an extinction coefficient of 10,800 M-1 cm-1.
Statistical analysis
Tissue monoamine content and MAO activity data were analyzed by t-tests. Behavioral tests were
analyzed using one-way parametric or non-parametric ANOVAs, followed by Tukey's or Dunn's posthoc tests whenever p < 0.05. Latency data for the light/dark test were transformed in survival curves,
and the difference in median survival was analyzed by the Mantel-Cox test.
Results
leopard zebrafish displayed more bottom-dwelling in the novel tank test (Figure 1A; F[3, 36] = 8.028, p =
0.0003; Tukey's HSD: q = 4.97, p < 0.05). Fluoxetine (5 mg/kg) decreased bottom-dwelling in leo
mutants, but not in WT zebrafish (Tukey's HSD: q = 5.052 [0 vs. 5 mg/kg, leo], q = 0.02857 [0 vs. 5
mg/kg, WT], q = 5.023 [5 mg/kg, leo vs. WT]). Nonetheless, albeit no differences were found between
WT and leo in habituation (Tukey's HSD: q = 3.193), fluoxetine promoted habituation in WT, but not
leo zebrafish (Figure 1B; F[3, 39] = 11.87, p < 0.0001). No further differences between strains or drug
effects were observed in this test (Figures 1C-1F).
In the scototaxis test, leo mutants spent less time in the white compartment than their WT counterparts
(F[3, 39] = 7.647, p = 0.0004; q = 5.152, Tukey's HSD; Figure 2A). Treatment with fluoxetine (5 mg/kg)
reverted this phenotype in leo animals (q = 6.314, 0 vs. 5 mg/kg, leo), without effects on white
avoidance in WT animals (q = 0.6424, Tukey's HSD). No differences between groups were observed in
8
the latency to explore the white compartment (
²
[df = 3]
= 4.154, p = 0.2453; Figure 2B) nor in the
habituation of white compartment exploration (F[3, 39] = 1.218, p = 0.3173; Figure 2C). The number of
entries in the white compartment (Figure 2D) and its habituation (Figure 2F), as well as the number of
squares crossed in the white compartment (Figure 2E), were likewise not affected (F[3, 39] = 1.09; H <
3.9, NS). Although thigmotaxis (Figure 2G) did not differ between phenotypes (q = 2.648, Tukey's
HSD), fluoxetine did decrease it in leo, but not WT zebrafish (F[3, 39] = 4.699, p = 0.0072). As in the
novel tank test, freezing (F[3, 39] = 0.8265, NS; Figure I) did not differ between groups, nor did erratic
swimming (H[df
= 4]
= 6.656, NS; Figure 2H). Risk assessment was greater in leo than WT, and
fluoxetine decreased it in leo but not WT (H[df = 4] = 21.19, p < 0.001; Figure 2I).
Tissue 5-HT levels were smaller in leo than in WT zebrafish (t[df = 18]= 3.093, p = 0.0063; Figure 3A),
while 5-HIAA levels were similar (t[df = 18] = 0.8371, NS; Figure 3B). 5-HT turnover was higher in leo
than WT zebrafish (t[df = 18] = 2.201, p = 0.041; Figure 3C). MAO activity was increased in leopard
strain (t[df = 4] = 3.437, p = 0.0264; Figure 4).
Discussion
In the present article, we demonstrated that leo zebrafish mutants show a selective impairment in
anxiety-like behavior in the light/dark preference (scototaxis) test and in the novel tank diving
(geotaxis) test. These tests have been shown to differ at the behavioral (Blaser &Peñalosa 2011, Blaser
&Goldsteinholm 2012, Blaser &Rosemberg 2012, Luca &Gerlai 2012, Maximino et al. 2010, Wong et
al. 2010) and pharmacological (Cachat et al. 2011, Egan et al. 2009, Maximino,Silva,Gouveia Jr
&Herculano 2011) levels. In addition, we found that fluoxetine rescued the anxious phenotype of leo
mutants in the light/dark test at a dose which was previously found to be inneffective in WT animals
(Maximino et al. 2011) and decreased measures of escape/avoidance in the novel tank test only in leo
9
mutants. These results suggest that 5-HT transport is constitutively down-regulated in leo zebrafish.
Consistent with this hypothesis, 5-HT tissue levels were smaller in leo than WT zebrafish. While
serotonin's metabolite, 5-HIAA, was not altered, 5-HT turnover was increased in leo mutants,
consistent with the increased MAO activity in synaptosomal preparations from leo brains. These
observations are somewhat similar to what is observed in Wistar-Zagreb rats selected for low platelet 5HT uptake activity, which show decreased [3H]-citalopram binding in the brain and decreased 5-HT
turnover (Cicin-Sain & Jernej 2010). However, this rat subline also shows decreased anxiety-like
behavior. Likewise, spiegeldanio zebrafish mutants, which show reduced fibroblast growth factor
receptor 1a function, show decreased anxiety-like behavior and decreased SERTa expression in the
raphe nucleus; nonetheless, administration of fluoxetine did not rescure the anxiolytic-like phenotype,
and zMAO expression was not different between spiegeldanio and WT animals (Norton et al. 2011;
Norton & Bally-Cuif, 2012). The suggestion that this behavioral phenotype is mediated by different
mechanisms than the anxious phenotype observed in the present work is supported by the observation
that spiegeldanio decreased anxiety is reversed by treatment with manipulations of the histaminergic
system (Norton et al. 2011). Recently, a mutation on the gene encoding the glucocorticoid receptor
(grs357) has been shown to present increased startle responses as larvae (Griffiths et al., 2012) and
exaggerated freezing responses in an open field as adults, as well as decreased SERTa expression in the
raphe nucleus (Ziv et al., 2012); both phenotypes were rescued by fluoxetine treatment. These
conflicting results point to a complex regulation of behavioral phenotypes by serotonin transporters in
zebrafish.
The mutation which produces the leopard phenotype has been described as a missense mutation in the
gene coding for a connexin, cx41.8 (Watanabe et al. 2006). This connexin is expressed in the central
nervous system of zebrafish, but its cellular distribution is not known; nonetheless, hemichannels
formed by connexins with the missense mutation show decreased coupling, which might decrease
10
functional coupling between neurons and glial cells or glia-glia communication in the central nervous
system. While serotonin transporters have been described in neurons and glial cells alike (Inazu et al.
2001, Pickel & Chan 2000), it is not clear whether the observations made herein point to a
dysregulation of 5-HT uptake by glial networks (by, e.g., decreased metabolic coupling) or by neurons.
Further studies are necessary to assess this hypothesis.
Bibliography
Aldeco M., Arslan B. K., Edmondson D. E. 2011, 'Catalytic and Inhibitor Binding Properties of
Zebrafish Monoamine Oxidase (zMAO): Comparisons with Human MAO A and MAO B',
Comparative Biochemistry and Physiology B, 159, pp. 78-83.
Araújo J., Maximino C., Brito T.M. et al.. Behavioral and Pharmacological Aspects of Anxiety in the
Light/Dark Preference Test. In Zebrafish Protocols for Neurobehavioral Research. Kalueff A.V. &
Stewart A.M. (Eds.). 2012. .
Blaser R.E.& Peñalosa Y.M. 2011, 'Stimuli Affecting Zebrafish (Danio Rerio) Behavior in the
Light/Dark Preference Test', Physiology & Behavior, 104, pp. 831-837.
Blaser R.E.& Goldsteinholm K. 2012, 'Depth Preference in Zebrafish, Danio Rerio: Control by Surface
and Substrate Cues', Animal Behaviour, In press.
Blaser R.E.& Rosemberg D.B. 2012, 'Measures of Anxiety in Zebrafish (Danio Rerio): Dissociation of
Black/White Preference and Novel Tank Test', PLoS ONE, 7, p. e36931.
Cachat J., Stewart A., Utterback E. et al. 2011, 'Three-Dimensional Neurophenotyping of Adult
Zebrafish Behavior', PLoS ONE, 6, p. e17597.
Cicin-Sain L.& Jernej B.. Wistar-Zagred 5HT Rats: a Rodent Model With Constitutional
Upregulation/Downregulation of Serotonin Transporter. In Experimental Models in Serotonin
11
Transporter Research. Kalueff A.V. & LaPorte J.L. (Eds.). 2010. pp. 214-243.
Dere E., De Souza-Silva M. A., Frisch C., Teubner B., Söhl G., Willecke K., Huston J. P., 2003.
'Connexin30-deficient Mice Show Increased Emotionality and Decreased Rearing Activity in the Openfield Along with Neurochemical Changes'. European Journal of Neuroscience, 18, pp. 629-638.
Egan R.J., Bergner C.L., Hart P.C. et al. 2009, 'Understanding Behavioral and Physiological
Phenotypes of Stress and Anxiety in Zebrafish', Behavioural Brain Research, 205, pp. 38-44.
Frisch C., Theis M., Silva M.A.D.S. et al. 2003, 'Mice With Astrocye-Directed Inactivation of
Connexin43 Exhibit Increased Exploratory Behaviour, Impaired Motor Capacities, and Changes in
Brain Acetylcholine Levels', European Journal of Neuroscience, 18, pp. 2313-2318.
Gilbert S.F. 2003, Developmental Biology, 7th, Sinauer Associates Inc.
Griffiths B. B., Schoonheim P. J., Ziv L. et al. 2012. 'A Zebrafish Model of Glucocorticoid Resistance
Shows Serotonergic Modulation of the Stress Response', Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 6, p.
68.
Inazu M., Takeda H., Ikoshi H. et al. 2001, 'Pharmacological Characterization and Visualization of the
Glial Serotonin Transporter', Neurochemistry International, 39, pp. 39-49.
Luca R.M.& Gerlai R. 2012, 'Animated Bird Silhouette Above the Tank: Acute Alcohol Diminishes
Fear Responses in Zebrafish', Behavioural Brain Research, In press.
Maximino C., Brito T.M., Colmanetti R. et al. 2010, 'Parametric Analyses of Anxiety in Zebrafish
Scototaxis', Behavioural Brain Research, 210, pp. 1-7.
Maximino C., Silva A.W.B., Gouveia Jr A. et al. 2011, 'Pharmacological Analysis of Zebrafish (Danio
Rerio) Scototaxis', Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, 35, pp. 624631.
Maximino C., 2012. Serotonin and Anxiety. Neuroanatomical, Pharmacological, and Functional
Aspects. New York: Springer.
12
Naus C.C.G., Bechberger J.F., Zhang Y. et al. 1997, 'Altered Gap Junctional Communication,
Intercellular Signaling and Growth in Cultured Astrocytes Deficient In Connexin43', Journal of
Neuroscience Research, 49, pp. 528-540.
Norton W.H.J., Stumpenhorst K., Faus-Kessler T. et al. 2011, 'Modulation of Fgfr1a Signaling in
Zebrafish Reveals a Genetic Basis for the Aggression-Boldness Syndrome', Journal of Neuroscience,
31, pp. 13796-13807.
Norton W.H.J. & Bally-Cuif L. 2012, 'Unravelling the Proximate Causes of the Aggression-Boldness
Behavioural Syndrome in Zebrafish'. Behaviour, 149, 1063-1079.
Pickel V.M.& Chan J. 2000, 'Ultrastructural Localization of the Serotonin Transporter in Limbic and
Motor Compartments of the Nucleus Accumbens', Journal of Neuroscience, 19, pp. 7356-7366.
Sáez J.C., Contreras J.E., Bukauskas F.F. et al. 2003a, 'Gap Junction Hemichannels in Astrocytes of the
CNS', Acta Physiologica Scandinavica, 179, pp. 9-22.
Sáez J.C., Berthoud V.M., Brañes M.C. et al. 2003b, 'Plasma Membrane Channels Formed by
Connexins: Their Regulation and Functions', Physiological Reviews, 83, pp. 1359-1400.
Stewart A. M., Cachat J., Green J. et al. 2013, 'Constructing the Habituome for Phenotype-Driven
Zebrafish Research', Behavioural Brain Research, 236, pp. 110-117.
Weissbach H., Smith T.E., Daly J.W. et al. 1960, 'A Rapid Spectrophotometric Assay of Monoamine
Oxidase Based on the Rate of Disappearance of Kynuramine', Journal of Biological Chemistry, 235,
pp. 1160-1163.
Wong K., Elegante M., Bartels B. et al. 2010, 'Analyzing Habituation Responses to Novelty in
Zebrafish (Danio Rerio)', Behavioural Brain Research, 208, pp. 450-457.
Watanabe M., Iwashita M., Ishii M. et al. 2006, 'Spot Pattern of Leopard Danio Is Caused by Mutation
in the Zebrafish connexin41.8 Gene', EMBO Reports, 7, pp. 893-897.
Ziv L., Muto A., Schoonheim P. J., et al. 2012. 'An Affective Disorder in Zebrafish With Mutation of
13
the Glucocorticoid Receptor', Molecular Psychiatry Advanced online publication.
Zor T., Selinger Z. 1996, 'Linearization of the Bradford Protein Assay Increases Its Sensitivity:
Theoretical and Experimental Studies', Analytical Biochemistry, 236, pp. 302-308.
14
Figure legends
Figure 1 – Differential effects of fluoxetine on WT and leotq270/tq270 zebrafish in the novel tank test. (A)
Schematics of the experimental tank and its division for manual recording of behavioral events. (B-F)
Effects of fluoxetine (0 or 5 mg/kg) on (B) bottom-dwelling, (C) habituation, (D) total locomotion, (E)
erratic swimming, and (F) freezing in the novel tank test in leo (black bars, black boxplots, black full
and empty circles) and WT (gray bars, gray boxplots, gray full and empty circles) zebrafish. **, p <
0.01. In B, C, and F, bars or circles represent mean ± S. E. M. In D and E, boxes represent data from
the 25th to the 75th percentiles, the trace represents the median, the plus (+) sign the mean, and
whiskers represent the 2.5th and 97.5th percentile.
Figure 2 – Differential effects of fluoxetine on WT and leotq270/tq270 zebrafish in the scototaxis test.
Effects of fluoxetine (0 or 5 mg/kg) on (A) white avoidance, (B) white avoidance habituation, (C)
latency to white, (D) total locomotion on white, (E) entries on the white compartment, (F) locomotion
habituation, (G) thigmotaxis, (H) freezing, (I) erratic swimming and (J) risk assessment in the
light/dark tank in leo (black bars) and WT (gray bars) zebrafish. ***, p < 0.001; **, p < 0.01; *, p <
0.05; N. S., non-significant. In A, B, C, F, G, and H, bars represent mean ± S. E. M. In D, E, I and J,
boxes represent data from the 25th to the 75th percentiles, the trace represents the median, the plus (+)
sign the mean, and whiskers represent the 2.5th and 97.5th percentile.
Figure 3 – Tissue levels of (A) 5-HT, (B) 5-HIAA, and (C) serotonin turnover (5-HIAA:5-HT ratio) of
leo (black bars) and WT (gray bars) zebrafish. **, p < 0.01; *, p < 0.05. Bars represent mean ± S. E. M.
Figure 4 – zMAO activity, measured as the rate of kynuramine disappearance in leo (black circles) and
WT (gray circles) zebrafish. ***, p < 0.001; *, p < 0.05. Points represent mean ± S. E. M.
15
Download