TITLE: Leaders and Followers: The Role of Achievement Motives and Their Effects on Motivating Strategies for Enhancing Performance AUTHORS: Patricia Ann Castelli, Ph.D. Lawrence Technological University, USA Frank Castronova, Ph.D. Lawrence Technological University, USA Jacqueline Stavros, EDM Lawrence Technological University, USA Jane Galloway Seiling, Ph.D. Taos Institute, USA ABSTRACT: Recognizing achievement motive disposition is important for leaders in understanding what motivates their followers. Incorporating motivating strategies into this process with the goal of enhancing performance, however, has not been sufficiently addressed in the literature. This study provided an analysis of low and high self-attributed need for achievement and their effects on the motivation needs of followers. The findings provide recommendations on how leaders can increase followers’ interest and effort to enhance performance. KEYWORDS: Achievement motive, leaders, followers, motivating strategies, motivation, performance TYPE OF PAPER: LEAD CONTACT: Journal Article Patricia Castelli, Ph.D. Associate Professor Outcomes Assessment Coordinator College of Management Lawrence Technological University 21000 West Ten Mile Road Southfield, MI 48075-1058 248.204.3066 castelli@ltu.edu Castelli et al. – Motivating Followers 1 LEADERS AND FOLLOWERS: THE ROLE OF ACHIEVEMENT MOTIVES AND THEIR EFFECTS ON MOTIVATING STRATEGIES FOR ENHANCING PERFORMANCE Abstract: Recognizing achievement motive disposition is important for leaders in understanding what motivates their followers. Incorporating motivating strategies into this process with the goal of enhancing performance, however, has not been sufficiently addressed in the literature. This study provided an analysis of low and high self-attributed need for achievement and their effects on the motivation needs of followers. The findings provide recommendations on how leaders can increase followers’ interest and effort to enhance performance. Castelli et al. – Motivating Followers 2 Acknowledgement: We thank Dr. Roy Bohlin and Dr. John Keller for permitting the use and modification of their Course Interest and Course Effort Survey instruments. Stimulated by the Hawthorne studies (Roethlisberger & Dickson, 1939), work motivation has been a focus of researchers since the 1930s. Researchers have taken various approaches from looking at congruence between individual’s needs and organizational demands (Argyris, 1957); sources of work satisfaction (work design and psychological processes); Herzberg, Mausner & Snyderman (1959); Vroom (1964) and his valence-instrumentality-expectancy model; McClelland & Winter’s (1969) personality-based approach to motivation; Maslow (1970) and his hierarchy of needs (motives)—which is the easiest to remember; and others. Yet with all this research, Levinson (2006) suggests there is still a “crisis in motivation.” He asked executives what the dominant philosophy of motivation is for American management. Their response was the typical carrot-and-stick philosophy, reward and punishment—and, they added, it is not working anymore. Motivational systems are at the center of behavioral organization (Emmons 1999; Steers, Mowday & Shapiro, 2004; Schein, 1980; and Knopf, 1967). Emmons states, “Behavior is a discrepancy-reduction process, whereby individuals act to minimize the discrepancy between their present condition and a desired standard or goal” (1999, p. 28). If we look at this from the standpoint of how leaders can motivate their followers to enhance their performance, participation in any organization involves exercising choice; a person chooses among alternatives, responding to the motivation to perform or ignore what is offered. This suggests that a follower’s consideration of personal interests and the desire to expand knowledge and skill has significant motivational impact, requiring the leader to consider motivating strategies to enhance performance. Castelli et al. – Motivating Followers 3 As noted above, there are many “competing” theories of motivation which are offered as explaining the behavior of people in organizations (Schein, 1980). The diversity of these theories brought Locke and Latham (2004) to recommend that the theory of motivation must be studied from new perspectives. Because the topic of employee motivation plays a central role in the field of management (Steers et al., 2004), attention must be paid to the prospect of motivation as it moves into the 21st century. The question must be asked, according to Steers et al, “how can we extend or modify current models of work motivation so they continue to be relevant in the future?” (p. 379). In response to this question, this writing will discuss achievement motive disposition as important for leaders in gaining an understanding of what motivates their followers. First, we will review how motivation has been defined and used in organizational settings. Next, we discuss the differences between low and high self-attributed needs for achievement and measurements. Third, we focus on both self-attributed and implicit motives to help leaders understand how they can best motivate their employees and bring it into action to align with organizational values, vision, mission, goals and objectives. Fourth, we present motivating strategies from the literature and a new application of the ARCS model as it pertains to an individual’s low or high self-attributed need for achievement. Fifth, we present recommendations for leaders aimed at increasing followers’ interest and effort, to enhance their performance. WHAT IS MOTIVATION? Steers et al. noted various definitions by writers who have attempted to define the term motivation, a term that is derived from the Latin word for movement (movere). They note that Atkinson offers the definition as “the contemporary (immediate) influence on direction, vigor, Castelli et al. – Motivating Followers 4 and persistence of action” (1964, p. 2) and Vroom offers “a process governing choice made by persons...among alternative forms of voluntary activity: (1964, p.6). According to Maddock and Fulton, “Motivation, surprisingly enough, has not been defined in a scientifically acceptable, reasonable and legitimate manner. It has not even been defined in a practical, commonsense or useful manner.” According to these authors, “leadership is defined in one word: motivation.” They suggest that motivation has not been adequately defined because it is too near to emotion “and no one wants to flirt with emotion” (1998, p. xii). Their suggestion that motivation is the “silent side of leadership” is pertinent to the tendency of researchers to describe motivation, but not to explain it. To prepare future leaders to motivate people they must understand how one is motivated. In the 1920s psychologists Thorndike, Woodworth, and Huss moved theorists toward the concept of learning in motivated behavior suggesting that past actions that lead to positive outcomes would tend to be repeated. Taylor, an industrial engineer, and his associates focused on the inefficiencies of factory production proposing a paternalistic approach to management. Social influences on behavior began to emerge in the 1930s. Group dynamics then emerged (e.g., Mayo, 1933; Roethlisberger and Dickson, 1939; Bendix, 1956) as significant to the motivation of the individual in the group. Etzioni (1961) offer three types of involvement of organization members which impact motivation: (1) alienative (not being psychologically involved and forced to be a member of the group); (2) calculative (involvement to the extent of going a “fair days work for a fair day’s pay;”) and (3) moral (the person intrinsically values the organization’s mission and his or her job and is personally involved (committed) and identified with the organization) (Schein, 1980). Castelli et al. – Motivating Followers 5 Moving back to the individual, Rokeach calls attention to determinants that enable or block motivation stating, “There is… the basic emotional and motivational attitude of the thinker to be reckoned with,” (1960, p. 177) suggesting the need for leaders to be aware of level of openness of the person to motivational activities. Leavitt (1972) firmly states that relevance to one’s needs is the most important determinant of one’s personal view of the world. It appears that factors regarding achievement motive are significant to motivational responses and tendencies. ACHIEVEMENT MOTIVE: LOW OR HIGH? In 1949, David McClelland & others reported that achievement motive could be induced. This finding is critical since it suggests that leaders have the ability to influence their followers behaviors by providing effective motivating techniques. McClelland found that two distinct motivational systems influence learning behaviors in different ways and that individuals require different incentives to exert effort and to perform based on their motive type. These motivational systems are referred to as low or high self-attributed need for achievement. Self-attributed needs for achievement are defined by Koestner et al. (1991) as selfreported attitudinal motives. The very first study of self-attributed need achievement (then called valuing achievement) was conducted by de Charms, Morrison, Reitman, and McClelland in 1955. In this study, subjects were asked to report their views on various paintings—with and without expert opinion. The findings indicated that subjects high in selfattributed need for achievement were more likely to change their views of the quality of paintings to be more in line with expert opinion than subjects low in self-attributed need for achievement. This study was significant in that it demonstrated a relationship between external salient social demands and high self-attributed needs for achievement. Castelli et al. – Motivating Followers 6 Since this time, numerous studies have been conducted under various achievementrelated testing situations with similar results. Under normal testing conditions, individuals with high self-attributed need for achievement did not perform better on a laboratory task than low need achievers. However, when an external demand for achievement was added, high need achievers did perform better than low need achievers (see Atkinson & Litwin, 1960; Koestner, Weinberger & McClelland, 1991; Patten & White, 1977; McClelland 1985a; Meyer, 1973; Rayor & Entin, 1982). Low self-attributed need for achievement is a motive disposition in which the individual does not attribute achievement to self and incentives are generally task-intrinsic. Becker (1960) states that task-intrinsic individuals define success by their own internal standard of excellence and that, furthermore, satisfaction is derived from doing the job well rather than from the enjoyment of the end product. Their motives are said to be implicit and primarily aroused by factors intrinsic to the process of performing an activity. Thomas (2002) states that rewards come from task purposes, namely meaningfulness and progress. The implication: leaders in organizations would motivate individuals by assigning challenging tasks that stretch their knowledge and skills. By contrast, according to McClelland, social-extrinsic individuals seem to have the goal of attaining approval from others rather than satisfying internal standards. Their motives tend to be highly self-attributed and are aroused by social factors that are extrinsic to the process of performing an activity. The inference here in the workplace is for leaders to provide external stimuli by way of social incentives related to success. Encouragement, ongoing feedback, and praise often motivate these types of individuals. These incentives and motive types are often not considered by leaders for improving followers’ performance. Castelli et al. – Motivating Followers 7 Currently, there is little research available which addresses the relationship between achievement orientation and specific motivating strategies to enhance performance. Even so, there is a logical implication that effort and performance can be enhanced when these aspects are taken into account. By understanding the differences in motivational systems, leaders may be able to provide incentives and apply various motivating strategies while satisfying both achievement orientations. This would seem a sensible approach when examining performance in real world settings. ACHIEVEMENT NEEDS AND MEASUREMENT Since the 50’s, numerous studies examined the relationship between implicit and selfreport measures of achievement motive. Researchers concluded that not only were self-report and implicit measures of achievement motive uncorrelated, but they possessed very different behavioral relationships (deCharms, Morrison, Reitman, & McClelland, 1955; Heckhausen, 1980; Kreitler & Kreitler, 1976; Korman, 1974; Lowell, 1952). They found that implicit needs are primarily aroused by factors intrinsic to the process of performing an activity. Self-attributed needs are aroused by social factors that are extrinsic to the process of performing an activity. These two different, independent systems of motivation differ in the way they energize, select, and direct behavior. Table 1 contrasts the two forms of motive based on research from the literature. --Insert Table 1 here— Koestner, Weinberger & McClelland (1991) designed a research study to examine possible relationships between motives and incentives. They sought to determine the manner in which the two types of motives (implicit and self-attributed) combine with two kinds of Castelli et al. – Motivating Followers 8 situational factors (task-intrinsic and social-extrinsic) to affect performance. The results support the hypothesis that extrinsic social factors in a performance situation are likely to combine with a person’s self-attributed achievement motive to influence performance, whereas task-intrinsic factors, such as level of challenge, influence performance in conjunction with a person’s implicit need to achieve. Thus, when a memory task was introduced with an explicit emphasis on achievement, subjects high in self-attributed need for achievement performed better than those who were low. On the other hand, in a neutral condition the reverse pattern was obtained. Importantly, it was shown that the implicit need for achievement did not interact with the social incentives regarding achievement to facilitate performance. These findings support earlier claims in research literature (Patten and White, 1977; Biernat, 1989). Koestner et al. (1991) conclude that these results suggest people scoring high in the selfattributed motives are more likely to selectively remember information relevant to their view of themselves. This implies that individuals who attribute high achievement motivation to themselves are vulnerable to performing quite poorly unless some other motivational factor, either in the form of external incentives or strong implicit motive, is also present. This research suggests that the challenge for leaders is to devise a systematic approach to coaching that considers both high and low achievement characteristic needs. In order to do this, leaders must understand the nature of self-attributed and implicit motives. SELF-ATTRIBUTED AND IMPLICIT MOTIVES In the past thirty years, researchers have focused more on information processing and the way in which motivational thoughts are converted into action (e.g., Anderson & Glassman, 1996; Heckhausen & Kuhl, 1985; Weiner, 1972, 1986). This cognitive reorientation of motive theory has called into question the use of the term "value" to describe self-reported motives. Value is a Castelli et al. – Motivating Followers 9 term that has come to be used to describe normative beliefs about desirable goals and modes of conduct (Chaiken & Stangor, 1987; Rokeach, 1973, 1979). To avoid misunderstanding, McClelland, Koestner, & Weinberger (1989) abandoned the term "value" and replaced it with "self-attributed motives" to describe attitudinal or self-reported motives. According to McClelland et al. (1989), the cognitive, information-processing model of human motivation in terms of needs, plans, and goals describes the way self-attributed motives function much better than the way implicit motives function. Self-attributed motives are characterized by organized thought; they start with an explicit goal that a person wishes for, then wants, and then becomes committed to pursuing in various ways (Heckhausen & Kuhl, 1985; Klinger, 1975, 1987). Klinger, Barta, & Maxeiner (1981) have studied empirically the varieties of current concerns that people report in interviews and questionnaires. Klinger developed the notion of a “current concern” defining it as the commitment to a goal and either the consummation of the goal or disengagement from it (Klinger, 1977, 1998, noted in Emmons, 1999). Most of the concerns have to do with unattained goals or unfinished business. The more committed people are to a goal or the more salient it becomes, the greater the likelihood that they will feel frustrated and unhappy for their slowness or failure in reaching it (McClelland et al.). In Fineman’s review, he states, “HRM interventions by positive scholars include empowerment programs to vitalize and positively energize organization, shifting employees toward greater positive commitment to organizational goals (2006, p. 277). The situation is different with implicit motives because they are aroused by affective experiences intrinsic to an activity and not by explicit references to unmet goals (McClelland et al., 1989). It is especially important to realize that failure to meet a goal is not as apparent to those with a strong implicit motive. Observers may presume that a person who scores high in Castelli et al. – Motivating Followers 10 implicit tests has a goal of doing better, but that person is not necessarily aware that he or she has such a goal. This premise is reinforced by Custers & Aarts (2005). They found that positive affect plays a key role in nonconscious goal pursuit. Their research revealed that nonconscious activation of desired behavioral states or behavioral goals promotes motivational activity to accomplish these states. Since there is no correlation between implicit and explicit desires to achieve, it is not as obvious to a low need achiever when a goal is not being met. In describing how an implicit motive functions, it is not appropriate to speak of wishing, wanting, and committing oneself to the goal that is recognized as the natural incentive for that motive (McClelland et al., 1989). Instead, the motive is better conceived of as leading to an activity that is the incentive for that motive. Thus, low need achievers have learned through experience to seek out certain activities that provide the pleasure of moderate challenge. However, they do not necessarily know that they have a goal of doing better. It seems sensible then, that low need achievers know less about what is guiding their behavior than do individuals with an explicit or high self-attributed achievement needs. The literature suggests that low need achievers are less able to plan appropriate corrective action when things go awry (McClelland, et al., 1989). According to Seiling & Roux (2006), motivation is seen as something that can be expanded through applying chosen and spontaneous instances of recognition, affirmation and reward. They caution, however, that these methods are temporary and less than effective in the long term. Their work on constructive accountability suggests that motivation processes are dependent upon ongoing interaction activities with respected others that can include peers, leaders and/or other influential people. These interactions are what stimulates connection to and interest in work. They argue that when others disappear or act disinterested in our work, we also lose interest. This view supports prior research from Koestner, Weinburger & McClelland Castelli et al. – Motivating Followers 11 (1991) regarding subjects who possess high self-attributed needs for achievement. Interaction with others, including the leader is important for high need achievers. This suggests that motivating strategies include opportunities for frequent interaction with the leader as well as team members. For both high and low need achievers, the leader’s use of an interesting variety of coaching techniques and feedback is critical for producing interest and effort. MOTIVATING STRATEGIES Locke & Latham (2002) state, “Motivation theory in the realm of work needs to draw on findings from other fields” (p. 393) suggesting that social psychology (Bandura, 1986), educational psychology (Dweck, 1986), and positive organizational psychology (Carmeon et al, 2003) have benefited the study of organization behavior. Keller’s work in instructional motivation is significant to this “crossover effect.” According to Keller (1983), instructional motivation attracts learners toward the instruction and increases their efforts in relation to the subject matter. Keller’s (1979) research on motivation, performance, and instructional influence illustrates how motivation can be integrated with the aspects of instructional science. Keller’s work helps explain what influences a person to approach or avoid a task, and how to make a task more interesting. Keller clearly distinguishes effort and performance as categories of behavior: “performance” means actual accomplishment, whereas “effort” refers to whether the individual is engaged in actions aimed at accomplishing the task. Therefore, effort is a direct indicator of motivation. Deci and Ryan offer self-determination theory proposing that “motivated behaviors vary in the degree to which they are self-determined (autonomous) versus controlled” (in Emmons, 1999). Consequently, according to Keller (1979), people can be viewed as more or less motivated by the vigor or persistence of their behavior. Intrinsic and Extrinsic Strategies Castelli et al. – Motivating Followers 12 According to Deci and Moller (1992), When people are experiencing satisfaction of their basic psychological needs, they tend to do what interests them. In other words, they tend to be intrinsically motivated. Thus, intrinsic motivation requires experiencing an activity as interesting, while also feeling some support for one’s basic needs. The fact that interest is so central to intrinsic motivation implies, of course, that if an individual did not find an activity interesting, he or she would not be intrinsically motivated for it. Under such circumstances, for the person to do all the activity at all would require some type of extrinsic motivation— “extrinsic motivation” being defined as doing an activity for some operationally separable consequence (p.588, emphasis added). Expectancy-valence theories (e.g., Porter & Lawler, 1968) had proposed that intrinsic and extrinsic motivation are additive, yielding total motivation. This led to the suggestion that activities (learning, work, etc.) should be designed to be as interesting as possible to stimulate intrinsic motivation and that social contexts should be organized to provide extrinsic rewards that are contingent upon effective performance at the activities. That way, there would be maximal motivation, consisting of the sum or the intrinsic motivation from the interesting activities and the extrinsic motivation form the contingent rewards (p.584). Attribution theory however, made a different prediction. deCharms (1968) suggested that when people perceive the locus of causality for their behavior to be within themselves, they tend to be intrinsically motivated, but when they perceive the locus of causality to be external, they tend to be extrinsically motivated. Harackewicz and Manderlink (1984) argue that performance-contingent rewards do not undermine intrinsic motivation but instead enhance it. Performance-contingent rewards are those given for doing well at an activity—that is, for meeting or surpassing some standard (p.585). Castelli et al. – Motivating Followers 13 Daft (2002) defines motivation as the forces either internal or external to a person that arouse enthusiasm and persistence to pursue a certain course of action. His simple model of motivation has four elements: First, a need creates desire to fulfill needs (food, friendship, recognition, achievement), next behavior results in actions to fulfill needs, third, rewards satisfy needs either intrinsically or extrinsically, and fourth, feedback informs a person whether the behavior was appropriate and should be used again. Daft states that intrinsic rewards appeal to the ‘higher’ needs of individuals, such as accomplishment, competence, fulfillment, and selfdetermination. Extrinsic rewards appeal to the ‘lower’needs of individuals, such as materials comfort and basic safety and security. The problem is that conventional management approaches often appeal to an individual’s lower, basic needs and rely on extrinsic rewards and punishments—carrot-and-stick methods—to motivate subordinates to behave in desired ways. According to Daft, Although extrinsic rewards are important, leaders work especially hard to enable followers to achieve intrinsic rewards—both individually and systemwide. Employees who get intrinsic satisfaction from their jobs often put forth increased effort…leaders also strive to create an environment where people feel valued and feel that they are contributing to something worthwhile, helping followers achieve systemwide intrinsic rewards (2002, p. 277). Hughes et al. (2006) describe performance as those behaviors directed toward the organization’s mission or goals, or the products and services resulting from those behaviors. They state that performance differs from effectiveness, which generally involves making judgments about the adequacy of behavior with respect to certain criteria such as work-group or organizational goals. In order for leaders to understand and influence follower motivation, Castelli et al. – Motivating Followers 14 leaders must be knowledgeable about different motivational theories (need, individual difference, cognitive, and situational). Hughes et al. state, “Leaders who are knowledgeable about different motivational theories are more likely to choose the right theory for a particular follower and situation, and often have higher-performing and more satisfied employees as a result” (p. 247). Thus, leaders would need to spend more time with their followers to determine what interests them intrinsically and whenever possible, provide opportunities to perform particular tasks they find rewarding. Hughes et al. understands that this is not always possible. However, they state that leaders may be able to get higher-quality work and have more satisfied employees by reassigning work according to values and intrinsic interests. Motivational Learning Strategy: The ARCS Model Keller (1987), Keller and Suzuki (1988), and Keller and Kopp (1987) identified four categories of motivation in learning situations: attention, relevance, confidence, and satisfaction (ARCS). According to Keller, the ARCS model contains specific methods or strategies that are aimed at producing motivational outcomes when learners are lacking sufficient conditions such as interest or motives. Attention refers to whether the learner’s curiosity is aroused and if stimulation can be sustained over time. Relevance refers to the learner’s perception of the personal need satisfaction in relation to the instruction, or whether a highly desired goal is seen as being related to the learning experience. Confidence refers to the perceived likelihood of success, and the extent to which success is up to the learner. Satisfaction refers to the combination of external rewards and internal motivation, and whether these motivators are compatible with the learner’s anticipations. Keller’s ARCS categories originate from a macro-theory of the relationships of individual and environmental characteristics on effort, performance, and outcomes. Castelli et al. – Motivating Followers 15 Using Keller’s ARCS model, Bohlin, Milheim & Viechnicki (1993) collected data regarding the instructional motivation perceptions of adults in a variety of learning environments. College students and community education students were used in this study. Two instruments were used by Bohlin, Milheim & Viechnicki (1990): the Course Interest Survey Revised (CISR) and the Course Effort Survey Revised (CESR). By utilizing these instruments, instructional motivation needs of the two groups of adult learners were identified and analyzed. The results of the first factor analysis (using the effort responses of learners in college classes) gave some support to the categories of the ARCS model with each of the first four factors entirely or predominately composed of items from one category of each (attention, relevance, confidence, and satisfaction). According to Bohlin et al. (1993), this suggests that the theoretical nature of the categories in the ARCS Model are consistent with the nature of the selfreported motivational needs of adults in college courses and workshops. Bohlin believes this also supports the long standing position that motivation often refers to time-on-task or similar measures of effort. Connecting ARCS to Achievement Motive Prior research from Koestner, Weinburger & McClelland (1991) regarding achievement motive and instructional motivation needs as assessed by Bohlin, Milheim & Viechnicki (1993) suggest a correlation between intrinsic/extrinsic needs on effort, performance, and outcomes. It is helpful to know that although many individuals may possess a mixture of both achievement orientations, one is usually predominant. Castelli (1994) used the ARCS model in conjunction with achievement motive to determine appropriate motivating strategies based on the need orientation of the learner. The major findings of this study centered on the interest variables as being most critical for predicting self-attributed needs of achievement. In fact, the interest Castelli et al. – Motivating Followers 16 variables were most common to both groups of low and high need achievers. Results that interest can be used to improve motivation in instruction indicate that learners will exhibit significant gains in continuing motivation when relevant selections of their interests are utilized. This is reinforced by Houtz (1994) who notes how interest is necessary for a transfer of learning from one situation or task to another. The findings also indicated that motivational strategies vary in their effectiveness dependent upon the need orientation of the learner. The results of this study suggested guidelines for selecting motivation strategies that may enhance effort and performance in classroom instruction. APPLYING THE ARCS MODEL IN ORGANIZATIONAL SETTINGS Castelli’s (1994) study was recently modified for application to organization settings since it provides useful information for leaders and managers concerning achievement need preference of their followers in task assignments, levels of challenge, feedback, and reward systems. Understanding achievement needs, motivational strategies, and profile characteristics for a given audience may greatly assist management in determining appropriate strategies to enhance performance output. The needs assessment instruments could also be used to measure the desires of a particular group (or groups) within an organization with the goal of obtaining general requirements for various populations. Given these implications, this study was conducted for use in organizational applications. Conceptual Model The conceptual model for this research is illustrated in Figure 1. The model shows behavioral characteristics that all individuals possess — incentives and motives for achievement. Interest and effort may be correlated to achievement motive. Relationships may also exist between gender, age and degree status. Castelli et al. – Motivating Followers 17 Figure 1. Conceptual Model for This Study Self-Attributed Achievement Motive = F (Interest + Effort + Gender, Age and Degree Status) Method A random sample of working professionals were determined and appropriate subject sample sizes were established that consisted of undergraduate, graduate and doctorate students in a college of management at a private university. The participants were located at various levels in organizations. Three survey instruments were used to conduct this research. For the first survey, subjects were asked to complete a self-report inventory of the achievement scale using Jackson’s Personality Research Form (1989). This information provided a basis for determining subjects’ low and high self-attributed needs for achievement. For the second and third surveys permission was granted to modify Bohlin et al. (1993) Course Interest Survey Revised and the Course Effort Survey Revised. The instruments were modified from instructor and student relationships to leader and follower relationships in order to determine how leaders can better motivate their followers to enhance performance within their organizations. For the Interest Survey Revised and the Effort Survey Revised (Castelli, 2006), subjects were asked to rate the importance of their leader’s various motivating strategies with regard to their own interest and effort, respectively. This information was used to determine strategies leaders can use to effectively motivate their followers in the workplace. In addition, critical demographic information (gender, age, degree status) was collected. Castelli et al. – Motivating Followers 18 The validity of Jackson’s Personality Research Form is discussed extensively by Jackson (1989) in the Personality Research Form Manual. Keller and Subhiyah (1987) and Bohlin and others (1993) also provide validity for the Course Interest and Course Effort Surveys. Analysis was also performed to determine the overall reliability for all survey instruments used in this study. The pooled results for Jackson’s achievement scale was .65. Individual item reliability ranged from .61 to .66. Spearman-Brown’s correction was .71. Bohlin and others Course Interest and Course Effort Survey’s Revised show consistent high reliability, with Effort (.89) slightly higher than Interest (.85). These survey instruments provide a strong basis for determining the motivation needs of followers and specific motivating strategies they value most from their leaders. Summary of Results The data indicates that age is significant in all categories of interest (except satisfaction) and all categories of effort (except relevance). Gender is not significant in the categories of effort and interest. Means and standard deviations were also analyzed for each of the items in the Interest and Effort Surveys. In comparing the results between groups with low or high self-attributed needs for achievement, the data indicates that nearly identical strategies (leader uses an interesting variety of coaching techniques, leader is a positive role model, leader builds selfesteem, appropriate challenge level) were found most important to both groups. The data also indicates that the high self-attributed need for achievement group rated all of the items as more important than subjects with low self-attributed need for achievement. Correlations of all variables were analyzed. The data indicates that age and degree status are common in self- Castelli et al. – Motivating Followers 19 attributed need for achievement. In the motivational characteristic categories of interest and effort, no significance was seen. Discriminant analysis was employed to determine if profile and motivational characteristics could be used to predict low and high self-attributed needs for achievement. The results of the canonical discriminant functions indicate that the variables used as predictors in this study (profile and motivation characteristics) is significant and, therefore, can be generalized to the population to which the study sample was drawn. Furthermore, the results indicate that in the category of interest, attention was most powerful in predicting self-attributed needs for achievement. Other powerful predictors were also in the area of interest (satisfaction and relevance). Satisfaction, confidence, and relevance (interest) and attention (effort) showed a negative correlation indicating an inverse effect on motivating strategies. Thus, not employing specific motivating strategies or not employing motivating strategies effectively can actually de-motivate followers. Finally, classification results were analyzed to determine how often low and high selfattributed need for achievement groups could be predicted. The data indicate that with the predictors used in this study, learners with low self-attributed needs for achievement could be correctly classified 52.8 percent of the time, and learners with high self-attributed needs for achievement could be correctly classified 65.4 percent of the time. The classification results indicate that the profile and motivational characteristics used in this research are fair predictors in determining self-attributed needs for achievement. The initial premise for this study suggested that self-attributed needs for achievement may not always be considered by leaders in organizational settings. Furthermore, failing to incorporate various motivational strategies to accommodate different need achievement Castelli et al. – Motivating Followers 20 orientations may inhibit follower performance. In addition, the specific motivational needs of followers may vary based on gender, age, and degree status. The relationships between these variables were analyzed and reported. The findings indicate that motivational strategies vary in their effectiveness dependent upon the need orientation of the follower. Therefore, the approach a leader takes in motivating his/her followers could accelerate or impede their performance outcomes. The results of this study suggest guidelines leaders can use for selecting motivation strategies that may enhance interest and effort to enhance performance. The implications are highlighted below. DISCUSSION 1. Attention is an important factor for gaining and sustaining the both need achievers’ effort. Motivating strategies should be incorporated that capture the followers’ interest. Using a variety of coaching techniques employed by the leader that include feedback on performance is also important. Making the follower feel enthusiastic about the challenge may enhance effort. Executive coaching has been found as most effective when it genuinely applies to one’s inner desires and capacities (Kauffman & Scoular, 2004). 2. Relevance is a very important component for both need achiever groups. This is evident in the area of interest where “leader viewed as a positive role model” is a critical attribute to all respondents. In the area of effort, appropriate challenge level is important to the low need achievers whereas working with others is most important to the high need achievers. 3. Confidence is a significant factor to both need achiever groups in both the interest and effort categories. A leader’s ability to build followers’ self-esteem is viewed as vital. Consistency should also be maintained to produce ongoing effort and for sustaining interest. However, Castelli et al. – Motivating Followers 21 interest and effort may decline for both groups if the leader fails to establish trust, or undermines the capabilities of followers’ worth. 4. Satisfaction is important to facilitate continuing motivation for both interest and effort. Finding levels of challenge that are appropriate is important to both need achiever groups. Results suggest that for the high need achievers, interest and effort may be contingent upon the personal satisfaction obtained from the learning experience. Therefore, projects and tasks should be designed to meet the personal needs of the individual. 5. High need achievers cited all categories of attention, relevance, confidence and satisfaction for both interest and effort as more important than low need achievers. This implies the increased need for the leader’s involvement in their daily activities. 6. The age of the individual may be correlated to self-attributed needs for achievement. The results suggest that the older the individual, the more they tend to be high need achievers. Similarly, the more education individuals possess, the higher the tendency for self-attributed needs for achievement. 7. Gender does not appear to be a factor in determining low or high self-attributed needs for achievement. 8. Overall, the leader’s ability to increase followers’ effort is most important in continuing motivation. The major findings of this study centered on the leaders’ ability to build self-esteem of their followers and importance of leaders to be viewed as positive role models. Results that interest and effort can be used to improve motivation indicate that followers will exhibit significant gains in continuing motivation when relevant selections of these strategies/attributes are practiced by the leader. Castelli et al. – Motivating Followers 22 Since effort categories were cited as most important for both low and high need achievers, improving their desire to return to task (effort) remains an important objective. The increased desire to persist in a task has long-range implications for advancements in learning and performance. This implies that a more intensified use of effort variables in motivating followers may prove beneficial. For convenience of the reader, Table 2 provides a brief overview of need achievement preferences. --Insert Table 2 here-Conclusion While a leader may be quite pleased with the output of his or her staff, it is more than likely that there is room for improvement. Understanding what motivates followers to perform their best work is key in order to achieve the highest level of satisfaction for both the leader and your team. Also, it is crucial that the leader puts this understanding to use by consistently providing the incentives and tools which he or she finds to be effective. The findings indicate that the motivational needs of the low and high need achievers do not differ as much as was first believed. Both achiever types indicated that effort was more critical than interest. The effort put forth by the follower is enhanced by the leader’s ability and willingness to use an interesting variety of coaching techniques, appropriate challenge levels, and self-esteem building methods for both achiever groups. The same is true in the area of interest, although both groups found that their interest in a given area was secondary to the effort they made when attempting to accomplish their goals. It was concluded that both the low and high need achievers require essentially the same qualities of their leaders in order to enhance their performance. It is the effective leader’s job to build self-esteem, to set appropriate challenge levels, to Castelli et al. – Motivating Followers 23 utilize motivating coaching techniques, and so on, regardless of the follower’s particular achiever traits. The leader’s role in promoting interest and effort is critical to the follower’s success. Also, the leader must serve as a positive role model, despite the indication that the low need achiever is intrinsically motivated. Proper application of specific motivating strategies will help both low and high need achievers, may increase interest and effort, and will ultimately fulfill the objective of enhanced performance. Castelli et al. – Motivating Followers 24 Table 1. A Summary of the Characteristics and Behaviors of Achievement Motive Describing Trait and Reference Low Self-Attributed Need for Achievement High Self-Attributed Need for Achievement Drive Koestner & McClelland, 1990 Energized by natural incentives for variety and challenge More extrinsic and outcomes focused in nature Discovery Approach Koestner & McClelland, 1990 Associated with feelings of interest and surprise Feelings of pressure and tension Performance Behavior Koestner & McClelland, 1990 Individuals cherish the process of performing an activity Individuals behave in a competent manner as defined by the particular situation Incentive Koestner & McClelland, 1990 Guided by self-reactions; satisfaction in anticipating task success Governed by an acquired desire to perform like an achiever; guided by social reactions Success Becker, 1960 Internal standard of excellence External standards that are recognized by others Risk Atkinson & Litwin, 1960 Preference for intermediate risk Greater avoidance of intermediate risk Persistence Atkinson & Litwin, 1960 Greater persistence Unrelated to persistence Conditioning McClelland, 1980 Operants – spontaneous behavior trends Respondents – predictive of immediate choice behaviors Castelli et al. – Motivating Followers 25 Table 2. A Summary of Need Achievement Preferences for Leader Motivation Characteristics Low Need Achievers High Need Achievers* Interest: Overall less critical than effort Overall less critical than effort Attention Leader uses an interesting variety of coaching techniques Leader uses an interesting variety of coaching techniques Relevance Leader is a positive role model Leader is a positive role model Confidence Leader builds self-esteem Leader builds self-esteem Satisfaction Appropriate challenge level Appropriate challenge level, Leader helps me accomplish my personal goals Overall more critical than interest Overall more critical than interest Attention Leader uses an interesting variety of coaching techniques Leader uses an interesting variety of coaching techniques Relevance Appropriate challenge level Working with other people Confidence Leader builds self-esteem Leader builds self-esteem Satisfaction Appropriate challenge level Leader helps me accomplish my personal goals ARCS strategies cited as less critical ARCS strategies cited as more critical Gender Least critical Least critical Age Younger learners more apt to be low need achievers Older learners more apt to be high need achievers Degree Status Less education apt to be low need achievers More education apt to be high need achievers Effort: Demographics: * Note: In all cases, the high need achievers cited each motivating strategy (ARCS) within the interest and effort variables as more important than the low need achievers. Castelli et al. – Motivating Followers 26 References Anderson, S.M., & Glassman, N.S. (1996). Responding to significant others when they are not there. In E.T. Higgins & R.M. Sorrentino (Eds.), Handbook of motivation and social cognition: foundations of social behavior. (Vol. 3, pp. 262-321). New York: Guilford Press. Argyris, C. (1957. Personality and organization. New York: Harper & Row. Atkinson, J.W., & Litwin, G.H. (1960). Achievement motive and test anxiety conceived as motive to approach success and motive to avoid failure. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 60, 52-63. Bandura, A. (1997). Social foundations of thought and action: A social-cognitive theory. Englewood, Cliffs, NU: Prentice-Hall. Becker, J. (1960). Achievement-related characteristics of manic-depressives. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 60, 334-339. Biernat, M. (1989). Motive and value to achieve: different constructs with different effects. Journal of Personality, 57, 69-95. Bohlin, R.M., Viechnicki, K.J., & Milheim, W.D. (1990). Course effort survey revised. From Keller, J.M., & Subhiyah, R.G. (1987). Florida State University. Bohlin, R.M., Viechnicki, K.J., & Milheim, W.D. (1990). Course interest survey revised. From Keller, J.M., & Subhiyah, R.G. (1987). Florida State University. Bohlin, R.M., Milheim, W.D., Viechnicki, K.J. (1993, January). Factor analyses of the instructional motivation needs of adult learners. Paper prepared for presentation at the Association for Educational Communications and Technology Annual Conference, New Orleans, LA. Cameron, K., Dutton. J.E. & Quinn, R.E. (2003). Positive organizational scholarship: Castelli et al. – Motivating Followers 27 Foundations of a new discipline, San Francisco: Berrett-Koehler. Castelli, P.A. (1994). An analysis of self-attributed achievement motives and their effects on instructional motivation needs of adult learners. Published dissertation, Wayne State University, Detroit, MI. Castelli, P.A. (2006). Effort survey. From Bohlin, R.M., Viechnicki, K.J., & Milheim, W.D. (1990). Course effort survey revised. From Keller, J.M., & Subhiyah, R.G. (1987). Florida State University. Castelli, P.A. (2006). Interest survey. From Bohlin, R.M., Viechnicki, K.J., & Milheim, W.D. (1990). Course interest survey revised. From Keller, J.M., & Subhiyah, R.G. (1987). Florida State University. Chaiken, S., & Stangor, C. (1987). Attitudes and attitude change. Annual Review of Psychology, 38, 575-630. Custers, R. & Aarts, H. (2005). Positive affect as implicit motivator: on the nonconscious operation of behavioral goals. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 82, (1), 129-142. Daft, R.L. (2002). The Leadership Experience, Second Edition. South-Western Publishers. deCharms, R., Morrison, H.W., Reitman, W.R., & McClelland, D.C. (1955). Behavioral correlates of directly and indirectly measured achievement motivation. In D.C.McClelland (Ed.), Studies in motivation (pp. 414-423). New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts. deCharms, R. (1968). Personal causation: the internal affective determinants of behavior. New York: Academic Press. Deci, E.L. & Moller, A.C. (1992). The concept of competence: a starting place for understanding intrinsic motivation and self-determined extrinsic motivation. In R. Schwarzer Castelli et al. – Motivating Followers 28 (Ed), Self-efficacy: thought control of action, pp. 579-597. Hemisphere Publishing Corporation. Emmons, R.A. (1999). The psychology of ultimate concerns: motivation and spirituality in personality. New York: Guilford Press. Etzioni, A. (1961). A comparative analysis of complex organizations. New York: Free Press. Fineman, S. (2006). On being positive: concerns and counterpoints. Academy of Management Review, 31, (2), 270-291. Heckhausen, H. (1980). Motivation and handeln [motivation and performance]. Berlin: Springer-Verlag. Heckhausen, H. & Kuhl, J. (1985). From wishes to action: the deadends and short cuts on the long way to action. In M. Frese & J. Sabini (Eds.). Goal-directed behavior: psychological theory and research on action (pp. 134-160). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Herzberg, F., Mausner, B., & Snyderman, B. (1959). The motivation to work. New York: Wiley. Houtz, J.C. (1994). Creative problem solving in the classroom: contributions of four psychological approaches. M.A. Runco (Ed), Problem finding, problem solving and creativity, pp. 271-290. Norwood, NJ: Ablex Publishing. Hughes, R.L., Ginnett, R.C. & Curphy, G.J. (2006). Leadership: enhancing the lesions of experience, 5th edition. McGraw-Hill/Irwin Publishing. Jackson, D.N. (1987). Personality research form-form E. Port Huron, MI: Sigma Assessment Systems, Inc. Jackson, D.N. (1989). Manual for the personality research form (3rd ed.). Port Huron, MI: Sigma Assessment Systems, Inc. Castelli et al. – Motivating Followers 29 Kauffman, C., & Scoular, A. (2004). Towards a positive psychology of executive coaching. In P.A. Linley and P.A. Josepha (Eds.). Positive psychology in practice. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley, 287-302. Keller, J.M. (1979). Motivation and instructional design: a theoretical perspective. Journal of Instructional Development, 2 (4), 26-34. Keller, J.M. (1983). Motivational design of instruction. In C.M. Reigeluth (Ed.), Instructional design theories and models: An overview of their current status. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Keller, J.M. (1987). Motivational design, In Encyclopedia of Educational Media, Communications, and Technology, 2nd ed. Westport, CT: Greenwood Press. Keller, J.M., & Kopp, T. (1987). Application of the ARCS model of motivational design. In C.M. Reigeluth (Ed.), Instructional theories and models. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, Publisher. Keller, J.M., & Subhiyah, R.G. (1987). Course effort survey. Florida State University. Keller, J.M., & Subhiyah, R.G. (1987). Course interest survey. Florida State University. Keller, J.M., Suzuki, K. (1988). Use of the ARCS model in courseware design. In D.H. Jonassen (Ed.), Instructional designs for computer courseware. New York: Lawrence Erlbaum. Klinger, E. (1975). Consequences of commitment to and disengagement from incentives. Psychological Review, 82, 1-25. Klinger, E. (1977). Meaning and void: inner experience and the incentives in people’s lives. Minneapolis, MN: University of Minnesota Press. Klinger, E., Barta, S.G., & Maxeiner, M. (1981). Current concerns: assessing therapeutically relevant information. In P.C. Kendall & S.D. Hollon (Eds.), Assessment strategies for cognitive-behavioral interventions (pp. 161-196). New York: Academic Press. Castelli et al. – Motivating Followers 30 Klinger, E. (1987). Current concerns and disengagement from incentives. In F. Halisch & J. Kuhl (Eds.), Motivation, intention, and volition (pp. 337-347). Klinger, E. (1998). The search for meaning in evolutionary perspective and its clinical implications. In P.T.P. Wong & P.S. Fry (Eds), The human quest for meaning (pp. 27-50). Mahwah, NJ: Eribaum. Koestner, R., & McClelland, D.C. (1990). Perspectives on competence motivation. In L. Pervin (Ed.), Handbook of personality theory and research (pp. 527-548). New York: Guilford. Koestner, R., Weinberger, J., & McClelland, D.C. (1991). Task-intrinsic and social-extrinsic sources of arousal for motives assessed in fantasy and self-report. Journal of Personality, 59:1, 57-81. Korman, A. (1974). The psychology of motivation. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. Kreitler, H. & Kreitler, S. (1976). Cognitive orientation and behavior. New York: Springer-Verlag. Leavitt, H.J. (1972). Managerial psychology, 3rd edition. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press. Levinson, H. (2006). The psychology of leadership. Boston, MA: Harvard Business School Publishing. Locke, E.A. & Latham, G.P. (2004). What Should We Do About Motivation Theory? Sic Recommendations for the Twenty-First Century. Academy of Management Review, 388-403. Lowell, E.L. (1952). The effect of need for achievement on learning and speed of performance. Journal of Psychology, 33, 1159-1177. Maddock, R.C. & Fulton, R.E. (1998). Motivation, emotions and leadership: The silent side of management. Westport, CN: Quorum (Greenwood). Castelli et al. – Motivating Followers 31 McClelland, D., & Winter, D. (1969). Motivating economic achievement. New York: Free Press. McClelland, D.C. (1980). Motive dispositions: The merits of operant and respondent measures. In L. Wheeler (Ed.), Review of personality and social psychology (Vol. 1, pp. 10-41). Beverely Hills: Sage. McClelland, D.C. (1985a). How motives, skills, and values determine what people do. American Psychologist, 40, 812-823. McClelland, D.C., Koestner, R., & Weinberger, J. (1989). How do self-attributed and implicit motives differ? Psychological Review, 96, 690-702. Meyer, W.U. (1973). Leistunssmotis und ursachenerklarung von erfolg und misserfolg [the achievement motive and causal attributions for success and failure]. Stuttgart: Klett. Patten, R.L., & White, L.A. (1977). Independent effects on achievement motivation and overt attribution on achievement behavior. Motivation and emotion, 1, 39-59. Porter, L.W. & Lawler, E.E. (1968). Managerial attitudes and performance. Homewood, IL: Irwin-Dorsey. Raynor, J.O., & Entin, E.E. (1982). Motivation, career striving, and aging. New York: Hemisphere. Rokeach, M. (1960). The open and closed mind. New York: Basic Books. Rokeach, M. (1973). The nature of human values. New York: Free Press. Rokeach, M. (1979). Understanding human values: individual and societal. New York: Free Press. Rothlisberger, F., & Dickson, W. (1939). Management and the Worker. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Castelli et al. – Motivating Followers 32 Schein, E. (1980). Organizational psychology, 3rd edition. Englewood Cliff, NY: NJ: Prentice-Hall. Seiling, J.G. & Roux. J. (2006). Constructive accountability: reconciling traditional accountability with collaborative practices of work. Publisher pending. Simpson, C & Rosenholtz, S. (1986). Classroom structure and the social construction of ability. In J. Richardson (Ed.), Handbook of theory and research for the sociology of education (pp. 113-138). Greenwood Press, Inc. Thomas, K. (2002). Intrinsic motivation at work: building energy and commitment. Berrett-Koehler Publishers, Inc. Weiner, B. (1972). Theories of motivation. Chicago: Rand McNally. Weiner, B. (1986). An attributional theory of motivation and emotion. New York: Springer-Verlag. Castelli et al. – Motivating Followers 33