(Please note - for the most accurate version of this paper, with final page layout, please refer to Mellor, N. ( 2005). Attention Seeking: The paradoxes of an under-researched concept. Educational & Child Psychology,22(4),94-107) Attention seeking: the paradoxes of an under-researched concept Nigel Mellor Abstract The term attention seeking has been used in association with major behaviour difficulties at home and school. The concept has, however, an ambivalent position in the literature and remains largely unresearched. To survey its use, electronic databases MEDLINE , PsychINFO and ERIC were searched (up to October 2003) using the key words “attention seeking”. In addition 133 books were surveyed in the fields of educational and clinical psychology and psychiatry. No comprehensive review of published material in this area was discovered from a period covering four decades. The term, however, had a wide spread, although very thinly spread, occurrence. It was, however, usually employed without any, or any adequate, definition. Overall the conclusion of the survey was that there was an absence of thorough discussion of a term in academic and common use that referred to potentially serious problems, and two paradoxes emerged. There was thus a possibility of great scope for misunderstanding of the implications of behaviours labelled attention seeking. Wide ranging research was needed. Introduction p.1 of 28 The purpose of this paper is to report the results of a survey of the extent of the use of, and some of the implications of, the term attention seeking. It is difficult to think of another concept in the fields of psychology, education or psychiatry with such extensive spread, which has had so little investigation. No comprehensive review of published material in this area has been discovered. This article seeks to begin to redress this omission and generate debate and research. Superficially it would be easy to assume that we all just “know” what attention seeking is, and that we all agree on its definition. In reality, however, there is apparently massive disagreement, and these disagreements cover its definition, its incidence, its perceived seriousness and whether or not it can be regarded as a specific phenomenon in its own right. The status of attention seeking behaviours – whether they represent a symptom of some other underlying cause, or whether they can legitimately be seen as a problem in themselves (whatever that might mean) - will only briefly be addressed here. The main focus will simply be on highlighting the widespread occurrence of the term and associated confusions, to open up the debate. There is evidence that the description attention seeking has been applied to behaviour difficulties associated with very serious problems for children, their carers and schools (see for example Berridge and Cleaver, 1987; Jones et al,1991; Donoghue & Abbas, 1971; Fuller & Sabatino, 1996; Mellor, 1997 & 2000; Peretti et al, 1984; Saxena, 1992; Surya Prakash Rao, 1977; Taylor & Carr, 1992; Taylor et al, 1993; Wiessen & Watson, 1967 ). However, many texts concerning behaviour difficulties have given the concept little or no consideration. Considering sources in educational psychology and education where the topic might reasonably be expected to appear (for instance Farrell, 1995; and the Elton report, DES, 1989) only passing comment is found. The term attention seeking appears very briefly in the psychiatric literature. In one common, extensive volume on children and adolescents there is scant reference (for instance in Cox, 1994). In adult psychiatry, attention seeking appears, for example, p.2 of 28 in the diagnostic criteria for Histrionic Personality Disorder ( American Psychiatric Association, 1994) and as a possible motivation in certain cases of factitious disorders ( Feldman & Ford, 1994; Munro & Thrusfield, 2001). This article will focus on children. The major points which arise from the survey below are (a) the occurrence of behaviours labelled attention seeking in a wide spread set of contexts, and examples of successful interventions but (b) confusion over the definition, origin, status and prevalence of attention seeking behaviour. Method Occurrence of the term attention seeking was determined by a search of MEDLINE, PsychINFO and ERIC from the earliest entries (1960s) onwards. As an additional approximate indication of occurrence, the index entries of 133 books were surveyed. These were drawn from the author’s personal collection, mainly of psychology and education texts, and from potentially relevant works, judged from book titles (i.e. implying coverage of child or adolescent emotional, behavioural or psychiatric problems) in the medical library of Newcastle University (psychiatry, clinical psychology and therapy sections). Results 1. Definitions of attention seeking were inconsistent and most authors used the term without definition (for example in 78 per cent of the articles surveyed). 2. An extremely wide range of behaviours has been labelled attention seeking (see Appendix 1 for examples) and attention seeking can present a serious problem (see above). 3. Citations of the term attention seeking appeared in an extremely wide range of contexts (see later discussion and Appendix 2). 4. These citations were, however, very thinly spread over four decades. 5. Estimates of the incidence of attention seeking varied widely. No reliable estimate of prevalence was discovered. p.3 of 28 6. There was widespread disagreement over the origins of, and implications of, attention seeking. 7. The apparent importance of attention seeking varied widely between authors. For example, judging by the occurrence of book index entries, 88 per cent of the books surveyed had no entry for attention seeking, and coverage within texts was almost invariably slight, yet six books reported attention seeking as a very common problem (see later). 8. Despite these confusions, a number of accounts of successful interventions were reported. Discussion Definition Part of the problem with any discussion of attention seeking behaviour, its implications and its prevalence, may lie in its actual definition (see Deb et al (2001) on parallel problems arising from having no agreed definition, with adults). Campbell (1996, p.72) and Dreikurs et al (1971, p.72) used the phrase “attention getting” and Wickstrom & Fleck (1983, p.227) used “negative attention seeking dependency” but neither terminology appears to have been adopted more generally. Dictionary definitions of attention seeking (e.g. Reber & Reber, 2001; Wolman,1989 ) potentially allow a limitless collection of behaviours to be included under that label. Reber and Reber (2001, p.62) for example have “generally, descriptive of any behaviour engaged in for the purpose of securing the attention of others”. This apparently simple concept, however, has many facets and may depend as much on the beliefs, attitudes and perceptions of those who define the behaviour this way as it does on the actual behaviours observed (again see Deb et al (2001) for a parallel). The definition below tried in part to capture one characteristic feature of attention seeking interactions: the individually perceived “irritating” nature of the problem (see Balson (1982) esp. ch.4, and a parallel discussion in Foote (1999)). p.4 of 28 As illustrated in Appendix 1, an immense spread of behaviours has been included under the label attention seeking (see also Mellor, 1997, 2000). In view of this, it would appear that any attempt to give examples of “typical attention seeking behaviours” as an aid to definition, would be unlikely to be successful. Mellor (1997) offered a working definition, as an aid to practitioners, although this should be balanced by careful discussion, observation and information from more than one source (see Dawson, 1985; Mellor & Harvey, 2003). ... attention seeking ... [refers] to those behaviours which, through their very irritating nature, bring a child to the attention of a number of adults in a persistent manner over a lengthy period of time, causing great concern. We would exclude, for instance, the sudden reaction of a child to an upset at home lasting for a few days or weeks. In addition, the pattern should appear in more than one setting (e.g. with several teachers or with teacher and parents / carers) (p13). But this definition itself may be too restrictive. Some children do display attention seeking interactions only at home and not at school for instance; the need to prioritise referrals lay partly behind this particular formulation. Through its wording, the definition may also carry an implication that the child is somehow the source of the problem. This was not the intention. Finally, the description of interactions which it tried to capture may best be seen as marking one extreme end of a continuum. A more general definition might simply exclude the last sentence. While everyday, unproblematic and also positive forms of attention seeking occur, it seems clear from the context that, when authors cited in this current paper have used the term attention seeking, they have been referring to negative forms or “inappropriate attention seeking” (e.g. Armstrong & Drabman, 1994 p.44). However, most authors have used the term without any definition at all (for instance 78% of articles cited in this paper had no definition and the rest generally simply used brief lists of example behaviours), yet even amongst professionals the term attention seeking has not been well understood and incorrect usage may be very frequent (see Morgan & Hastings, 1998). p.5 of 28 Thus, when the field is left, as it is, in a state of some confusion, a paradox arises. Two opposing forces may come into play: over-inclusion and under-inclusion. For some of those who accept the concept of attention seeking as a problem in its own right (whatever that might mean) there may be a danger of over-inclusion. Given the vast range of behaviours which have been labelled attention seeking, and given flexible definitions such as Reber & Reber (2001) and Wolman (1989), there may be a temptation to see many, or even most, behaviour problems as attention seeking. However, there may be at the same time, a parallel but opposing tendency to underinclusion. Given the general lack of discussion and recognition of attention seeking, as evidenced by, for instance, absence of reviews for 40 years, it would appear that there is little “official status” for the concept. There may thus be a number of practitioners who do not accept attention seeking as a problem in its own right. Thus many behaviours which could be part of attention seeking cycles may not have been included by practitioners or researchers within a category labelled “attention seeking”. The concept may thus simply have been overlooked. Overall, it appears that attempts at defining attention seeking have led to a number of still unresolved problems. Occurrence of the term The description attention seeking has generally not been applied to routine day-today interactions. Most commonly the term has been used to describe situations where a great deal of attention was being sought, and this was judged to be in some way inappropriate i.e. situations where “problem behaviour … is described” (Odendaal, 2000 p.276). However, not all demands for attention should potentially be labelled attention seeking (see below). In addition, attention seeking may also have been seen in a positive context, such as with the development of humour ( McGhee & Lloyd, 1982; Fern 1991; but see Damico & Purkey, 1978, on “class clowns”). p.6 of 28 The term was found to have occured explicitly in a number of checklists (e.g. Coopersmith, 1981; Sroufe et al ,1983; Stott, 1974, Williams et al,1990) there was, however, no consistency in the way the concept was defined within these, if it was defined at all. In other common instruments, however, it simply did not appear (see below). Finally, in view of the absence of discussion of attention seeking behaviours in many potentially relevant publications, a wide range of citations has been included below and in Appendix 2 to establish beyond doubt the occurrence of the concept in the academic literature. It should be emphasised, paradoxically, however, that the apparent breadth of material collected here and in the appendix gives a rather false picture of activity in this field, as most sources have given the subject cursory treatment. Furthermore they cover a very extensive period: some four decades of psychiatry, psychology and education research output. Using rather arbitrary categories, for convenience of reporting, attention seeking interactions being reported have been noted as occurring: (a) across the age range (b) alongside many special difficulties (c) in many cultures and (d) in different living arrangements. It must be stressed here that there is no implication that attention seeking was a necessary part of any of these categories, situations or difficulties; the breakdown is simply to aid discussion of a rather unwieldy list of citations. The pre-school articles have been considered first, in some detail, to highlight the confusions in the field, and point towards some resolution (older children are included in other categories) (a) Across the age range: the pre-school years The description “attention seeking” has been attached to children of all ages and to adults; this paper focuses on children. One of the earliest ages noted, where the label attention seeking has been applied, is two and a half years (Eisenberg et al,1996). In their abstract , however, Jacobs and Moss (1976) attached the description to three month old babies, but the body of the article implied a rather different connotation to that used by the present author. p.7 of 28 Green (1987) provided practical advice on intervention in the toddler stage. Enlow (1968), drew on behavioural psychology, and incidentally provided a good example of extensive negative attention seeking, illustrating the vast range of behaviours available. At all ages the field shows a broad pattern of a few authors providing detailed treatment in textbook or case study formats, in other research papers the topic has been given scant coverage (see examples following). Several studies addressed the pre-school years with a mixture of aims, but with no or no adequate definition of attention seeking and scant discussion, other than noting that boys predominated (Alfgren et al, 1979; Jacobs & Moss, 1976; Fahrenfort et al, 1996; Johnson and Breckenridge, 1982; Kashani et al, 1986; Luby et al, 1995; McGhee and Lloyd, 1982; Mendelson et al, 1994; O’Donnell & Van Tuinan, 1979; Turner, 1991). A number of surveys covered this age range, but again, with no definition of attention seeking and little discussion (Charlton et al, 1994; Faruk & Deb, 1992; Hannesdottir et al, 2000; Rai et al, 1993; Stjernqvist, 1996; Taleb, 2001). Problems arising from the structure of survey instruments are discussed later. One group of studies of preschool children, mainly drew on a framework of research into “dependency” (Bhogle, 1983; Cornelius & Denney, 1975; Gewirtz, 1956; Rosenthal, 1967 a & b; Scher, 1971; Stein & Wright, 1964). This group is bedevilled, as is so much work in this field, by inconsistency in the definition of attention seeking, in some cases implying little more than routine re-assurance seeking in one-off experimental arrangements. Sroufe et al( 1983) made the link between dependency and attachment perspectives, and more recent studies have considered attachment difficulties( Oppenheim, 1997; Shaw et al, 1994; Turner, 1991) or relationship disorders (Anders, 1989; Zeanah et al, 1997). These two later frameworks, appeared to offer more productive areas for current research. Although again, within these studies, attention seeking has remained a slippery concept, an association between anxious/ambivalent attachment and persistent negative attention seeking has been suggested (Crittenden, 2001, personal communication; see also Byng-Hall, 2001; Crittenden, 1999). p.8 of 28 (b) Alongside many special difficulties Attention seeking interactions have been recorded in accounts of children displaying a wide range of special difficulties - using this description very broadly, as a convenient umbrella term (see Appendix 2). A somewhat counterintuitive example, worthy of note, is the reported occurrence of attention seeking in autistic children (Howlin & Rutter, 1987). If confirmed by other work (see for example discussion in Jordan & Powell, 1995; Taylor & Carr, 1992) this might raise interesting questions about aspects of the nature of autism. Overall, the most frequent report of attention seeking appears to have been in children with severe learning difficulties. This might be simple a statistical quirk, since, for example, this group may have included many others. It could, however, have been a result of the, apparently, more common use of functional analysis (see below) with children with severe difficulties. (c) In many cultures Most sources which have been uncovered, originated in North America, Australia or the United Kingdom. The summary in Appendix 2 establishes that the term was current in other countries/cultures – from Bangladesh to Sweden, however, there are caveats. Such studies are comparatively rare and there is little evidence to judge how the label has been used. In addition, some of the children have been described as experiencing very distressing circumstances – it is clear that it is the circumstance that has needed to change (see Jessee et al, 1992). Lacking specific definitions of attention seeking, however, further investigation would be needed to clarify exactly the meaning of the term in these different settings. (d) In different living arrangements Attention seeking behaviour has been noted in children in a variety of family and living arrangements, for example, with birth parents, single parents, reconstituted p.9 of 28 families (Mellor, 2000) and different foster/ adoption/ residential settings (Bartollas & Sieverdes,1983; Berridge & Cleaver, 1987; Dubowitz & Sawyer, 1994; Lochman et al,1986; Massey & Murphy, 1991; Phillips, 1990). This suggests that there is vast scope for research into the many complex mechanisms that may have been involved in the development of attention seeking interactions. In conclusion, from this overview, there is some indication that the concept of attention seeking behaviour has been used in a very widespread range of circumstances. The prevalence of such behaviours is, however, a matter of some dispute. Prevalence of attention seeking behaviour Attention seeking, by some accounts, constitutes a very common problem. Montgomery (1989) reported attention seeking to be the highest priority of all behaviour problems identified by teachers on in-service courses. Balson (1982, 1987), Conway (1990), Grimshaw & Berridge (1994), Hannesdottir et al(2000), McManus (1995) and Peagam (1994) also emphasised its high incidence . In a recent study, 7 per cent of referrals to educational psychologists for behaviour emotional and social difficulties mentioned attention seeking as the reason (Rees et al, 2003). Fuller & Sabatino (1996) found 14 per cent of children in “alternative schools” displaying attention seeking behaviours. An attention seeking factor was confirmed by factor analysis in different contexts by Fuller & Sabatino (1996), McDermott (1984) and O’Donnell & Van Tuinan (1979), in addition, Sears et al(1965) examining clusters of behaviour associated with dependency, found only one achieved statistical significance: “negative attention seeking”. An ICM poll of 1050 adults (The Guardian 15.8.00) identified attention seeking as the most common childhood problem perceived in friends’ children. However, large scale research into the prevalence and socio-economic and cultural characteristics of attention seeking behaviour is notable by its absence. No specific surveys have been uncovered of the child population as a whole, the nearest being p.10 of 28 The National Child Development Study (Davie et al, 1972). The authors reported a rate of nearly 7 per cent of children displaying “anxiety for adult acceptance” using the 1963 Bristol Social Adjustment Guide. Stott et al (1975) interpreted this category as attention seeking, but this term only really appeared in later editions of the guide (which, judging by citations, is in any case now rarely employed). The rather striking variation in the reporting of attention seeking in the literature is difficult to explain; in many cases it simply does not appear (see earlier). Variations in definition, or even absence of definition, may be one cause. Another avenue to explore may be the actual content of questionnaires and checklists currently used in research. A brief inspection of examples readily to hand reveals that, apart from in those few listed earlier, the phrase “attention seeking” is rarely included. If further investigation showed that this is typical of the material in most common use, then the absence of reporting of attention seeking is a potentially unsurprising chicken and egg problem. It will only appear if already built into the instruments we employ. Moving away from such instruments, from experience, a functional analysis of behaviour (e.g. Taylor, 1994) is most useful in identifying attention seeking. Even quite simple questions such as “Does the child appear to gain anything from behaving this way?” have the potential to throw up clues to many aspects of behaviour, including attention seeking. Origins and implication of attention seeking While socially acceptable behaviours may be included in an attention seeking repertoire (see Mellor, 1997) of most concern to teacher and carers are persistent unacceptable behaviours. However, whether these are consciously displayed by the child as overt “demands” for attention, or have become a kind of habit, or whether these unacceptable behaviours represent a separate phenomenon or a symptom of some other malaise, are unresearched topics, as are the extent to which attention seeking could arise from maltreatment or organic causes. p.11 of 28 A number of authors have offered a variety of attempts at understandings of the development of attention seeking behaviour. Shaw et al(1994) for example addressed early mother-child interactions; Dreikurs et al(1971) and Griffin & de la Torre (1983) discussed a typical trigger – the arrival of a new baby; Peretti et al(1984) explored parental rejection and Hetherington (1972) father absence; Connor (2001) noted the impact of formal tests (SATs) on young children. Mellor (1997, 2003, 2004a) examined Adler’s views and a range of other perspectives (behavioural theories; a “maladjustment” viewpoint; an analogy with “deviance amplification”; attachment disorders; a view from chaos theory). While several authors apparently readily saw attention seeking as a problem in its own right, and a serious one at that, paradoxically, in the vast majority of books surveyed (88 per cent), it has not even rated an index entry. Overall there has been no systematic work on clarifying the origins and implications of attention seeking behaviour. Interventions Webster-Stratton & Herbert (1995 p.240) asserted that “the basic principle behind the development of many common behaviour problems is that children work for attention from others ...whether it is positive ...or negative”. The authors gave detailed advice on managing attention seeking behaviours and Herbert (1981) provided a lengthy case study of one four year old displaying marked attention seeking behaviour. In class and at home, many strategies used by teachers and parents, quite routinely, have been effective in dealing with moderate levels of attention seeking: selective ignoring, planned consequences and attention for acceptable behaviour, will come as no surprise to practitioners. What may be surprising is the range of barriers to effective intervention - conceptual, emotional and practical - when attention seeking is severe (see Mellor, 1997, or Mellor, 2000 on “troubleshooting”), particularly in the light of the enormous range of behaviours which may be p.12 of 28 considered, as illustrated in Appendix 1. Practitioners may find this work much more challenging. Taking a few other examples of interventions in illustration, Armstrong & Drabman (1994) described an imaginative use of the development of sports skills with pupils displaying attention seeking behaviour and Kearney (2001) explored approaches to school refusal associated with attention seeking. Hoffmann (1975) advocated an Adlerian counselling approach while Stein (1998) described a psychodynamic approach (which appeared to involve mainly increased adult attention). Barker & Fraser (1968) explored drug treatments. Cipani (1998) described attention seeking interactions in class, and gave some guidelines for intervention and considered the potential reinforcing role of other pupils (see also Rogers, 2000). More general school-based and early years issues were addressed in Balson (1982), Dreikurs et al (1971), Mellor (2001), Robertson (1989) and Welford (2002). Research into the intervention implications of the possibility of attention seeking arising from maltreatment or organic factors has been absent. However, in more general practice, some limited evidence of successful interventions has been presented via a selection of long term, case study follow-ups in Mellor (2000), and via ongoing work in Mellor (2004b) but large scale, well designed research has been lacking. One tentative approach to tailoring interventions is mentioned below. Conclusion Two paradoxes emerged from this survey. First, it was clear that attention seeking has been neglected in much of the literature, when the enormous time scales of the above survey is taken into account. Yet there is evidence that attention seeking, whatever it represents, has been a very widespread, and potentially serious, phenomenon which needs systematic research. Inconsistencies in its inclusion in questionnaires, and even its definition, may partly explain the reason for this paradox. p.13 of 28 Second, there may have been pressures towards both under-inclusion and overinclusion of behaviours under the label attention seeking. There is thus potentially great scope for misunderstanding of the implications of attention seeking behaviours and for misuse of the term. From experience, confusion with ADHD has been a particular hazard (see Mellor 2000, 2004a).To complicate matters further, it has appeared quite possible for attention seeking and ADHD to exist together. We also need to clarify how to differentiate attention seeking from other childhood problems such as conduct disorder and oppositional-defiant disorder. The question of how far and under what conditions attention seeking might indeed represent a separate entity (which might be appropriately addressed though targeted intervention), or how far the behaviours referred to are simply symptoms of other conditions, is a key aspect which urgently needs consideration. The research programme outlined below may go a long way to resolving this. An approach with the potential for getting an overview of some of the many issues involved may be to consider different developmental pathways. One categorisation of these would be to compare those more clearly reactive in origin (e.g. reacting to the arrival of a new baby or parental separation) with those more broadly interactive in origin (e.g. resulting from gradually developing attachment or relationship disorders, with no clear single trigger point). These histories may lead to shadings of interventions (for instance addressing sibling rivalry more, or attachment difficulties more – compare Speltz, 1990) although the main thrust of any effective strategy is likely to continue to focus on the three elements (ignoring etc) outlined earlier, which address those factors maintaining attention seeking cycles in the present. However, given the absence of systematic study in the field, the overall picture which emerged from the survey above was of more questions than answers. There was evidence that attention seeking could represent a serious problem which might occur extensively, but investigation is clearly needed into all aspects: definition and differentiation, questionnaire construction, origins, prevalence and intervention. Functional analysis, Adlerian and behavioural psychology, have offered useful p.14 of 28 perspectives on its origins and maintenance. However, the vast literature on attachment theory offers a promising framework for investigating the beginnings of attention seeking cycles. Overall, attention seeking offers a rich potential for research on many levels. Address for correspondence Nigel Mellor, Educational Psychology Team, Chapel Lane Education Centre, Chapel Lane, Monkseaton, Tyne and Wear, NE25 8AD Email : nigel.mellor@newcastle.ac.uk References Alfgren, S.H., Aries, E.J. & Oliver, R.R. (1979) Sex differences in the interaction of adults and preschool children. Psychological Reports, 44, 115-118. American Psychiatric Association. (1994) Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington DC: American Psychiatric Association. Anders, T.F. (1989) Clinical syndromes, relationship disturbances and their assessment. In: A.J. Sameroff & R.N. Emde (Eds) Relationship Disturbances in Early Childhood: A developmental approach. New York: Basic Books. p.15 of 28 Armstrong, K.J & Drabman, R. (1994) The clinical use of sports skills tutoring with grade school boys referred for school behavioural problems. Child and Family Behavior Therapy, 16, 43-48. Balson, M. (1987) Becoming a better parent. London: Hodder and Stoughton. Balson, M. (1982) Understanding classroom behaviour. Hawthorn, Victoria: The Australian Council for Educational Research. Barker, P. & Fraser, I.A. (1968) A controlled trial of Haloperidol in children. British Journal of Psychiatry, 114, 855-857. Bartollas, C. & Sieverdes, C.M. (1983) Institutional games played by confined juveniles. Adolescence, 18, 609-617. Barton, L.E., Brulle, A.R. & Repp. A.C. (1987) Effects of differential scheduling of timeout to reduce maladaptive responding. Exceptional Children, 53, 351356. Berridge, D. & Cleaver, H. (1987) Foster home breakdown. Oxford: Basil Blackwell. Besag, V.E. (1989) Bullies and victims in schools. Milton Keynes: Open University Press. Bhogle, S. (1983) Antecedents of dependency behavior in children of low social class. Psychological Studies, 28, 92-95. Buchanan,N. & Snars, J. (1993) Pseudoseizures (non epileptic attack disorder) – clinical management and outcome in 50 patients. Seizure, 2,141-146. Byng-Hall, J. (2001) Attachment as a base for family and couple therapy. Child Psychology and Psychiatry Review, 6, 31-36. Byrne, E.A., Cunningham, C.C. & Sloper, P. (1988) Families and their children with Down’s syndrome: one feature in common. London: Routledge. Campbell, R.J. (1996) Psychiatric dictionary (7th ed.). New York : Oxford University Press. Carr, E.G. & Durand, V.M. (1985 ) Reducing behaviour problems through functional communication training. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 18, 111-126. Charlton, T., Leo, E., Evans, P. & Flagg, P. (1994) Rates of problem behaviour amongst 3 and 4 year old children attending nursery classes in St Helena, South Atlantic. Support for Learning, 9, 129-135. p.16 of 28 Cipani, E. (1998) Three behavioural functions of classroom noncompliance: diagnostic and treatment implications. Focus on Autism and Other Developmental Disabilities, 13, 66-72. Coghlan, A.J. (1973) The adolescent drug abuser in child-caring institutions: facts, theories and suggestions. Child Care Quarterly, 2, 256-269. Connor, M.J. (2001) Pupil stress and standard assessment tasks. Emotional and Behavioural Difficulties, 6, 103-111. Conway, R. (1990) Behaviour disorders In: A. Ashman & J. Elkins (Eds.) (1990) Educating children with special needs (pp. 148-186). London: Prentice Hall. Cooper, P. & Ideus, K. (1996) Attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a practical guide for teachers. London: David Fulton. Coopersmith, S. (1981) The Antecedents of self-esteem. Palo Alto: Consulting Psychologists Press. Cornelius, S.W & Denney, N.W. (1975) Dependency in day-care and home-care children. Developmental Psychology, 11, 575-582. Cox, A.D. (1994) Diagnostic appraisal In: M. Rutter, E. Taylor & L. Hersov (Eds.), Child and adolescent psychiatry: modern approaches (3rd ed.) Oxford: Blackwell Scientific Publications. Crittenden, P.M. (1999) Danger and development: the organization of self-protective strategies. In: J.I. Vondra & D.Barnett (Eds) Atypical Attachment in Infancy and Early Childhood among Children at Developmental Risk. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, serial no. 258, vol. 64. D.E.S., Department of Education and Science (1989) Discipline in schools. Report of the committee of enquiry chaired by Lord Elton. London: H.M.S.O. Damico, S.B.& Purkey, W.W. (1978) Class clowns: a study of middle school students. American Educational Research Journal, 15, 391-398. Davie, R., Butler, N. & Goldstein, H. ( 1972) From birth to seven : the second report of the National Child Development Study. London: Longman. Dawson, R.L. (1985) (Ed) Attention seeking behaviour: the Macmillan teacher information pack. London: Macmillan Deb, S., Thomas, M. & Bright, C. (2001) Mental disorder in adults with intellectual disability.2: The rate of behaviour disorders among a community –based p.17 of 28 population aged between 16 and 64 years. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 45, 506-514. Dikaiou, M. (1989) Peer interaction in migrant children: observational data and parents’ evaluation. International Migration, 27, 49-67. Donoghue , E.C. & Abbas, K.A. (1971) Unstable behaviour in severely subnormal children. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 13, 512-519. Dreikurs, R., Grunwald, B.B. & Pepper, F.C. (1971) Maintaining sanity in the classroom: illustrated teaching techniques. New York: Harper & Row. Dubowitz, H & Sawyer, R.J. (1994) School behavior of children in kinship care. Child Abuse and Neglect, 18, 899-911. Eisenberg, A., Murkoff, H.E. & Hathaway, S.E. (1996) What to expect: the toddler years. London: Simon and Schuster. Enlow, R.V. (1968) A study of attention-seeking behaviour in young children. PhD dissertation, Texas technological college. Fahrenfort, J.J., Jacobs, E.A.M., Miedema, S. & Schweizer, A.T. (1996) Signs of emotional disturbance three years after early hospitalisation. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 21, 353-366. Farrell, P. (Ed.) (1995) Children with emotional and behavioural difficulties : strategies for assessment and intervention. London: Falmer Press. Faruk. T. & Deb, S. (1992) A comparative study between the trends of behavioural problems of Bengalee pre-school children of Calcutta and Rajsahi. Journal of the Indian Academy of Applied Psychology, 18, 13-17. Feldman, M.D. & Ford, C.V. (1994) Patient or pretender: inside the strange world of factitious disorders. New York: John Wiley. Fern, T.L. (1991) Identifying the gifted child humorist. Roeper Review, 14,30-34. Finnie, N.R. (1974) Handling the young cerebral palsied child at home. London: Heinemann Medical. Foote, B. (1999) Dissociative identity disorder and pseudo-hysteria. American Journal of Psychotherapy, 55, 320-343. Fuller, C.G & Sabatino, D.A. (1996) Who attends alternative high schools? High School Journal, 79, 293-297. Galloway, D. M. (1976) Case studies in classroom management. London: Longman. p.18 of 28 Gardner, W.I. (1971) Behavior modification in mental retardation. London: University of London Press. Gewirtz, J.L. (1956) A factor analysis of some attention seeking behaviors of young children. Child Development, 27, 17-36. Graham, J.M., Superneau, D., Rogers, R.C, Corning, K., Schwartz, C.E. & Dykens, E.M. (1999) Clinical and behavioral characteristics in FG syndrome. American Journal of Medical Genetics, 85, 470-475. Gray, J & Richer, J. (1988) Classroom responses to disruptive behaviour. Basingstoke: Macmillan. Green, C. (1987) Toddler taming: a parents’ guide to the first four years. London: Century Hutchinson. Griffin, E.W. & De La Torre, C. (1983) Sibling jealousy: the family with a new baby. American Family Physician, 28, 143-146. Grimshaw, R. & Berridge, D. (1994) Educating disruptive children - placement and progress in residential special schools for children with emotional and behavioural difficulties. London: National Children's Bureau. Hannesdottir, H., Sourander, A. & Piha, J. (2000) Comparison of behavioral problems between two samples of 2 to 3 year old children in Finland and Iceland. Nordic Journal of Psychiatry, 54, 13-17. Harris, J., Cook, M. & Upton, G. (1996) A whole school approach to assessment and intervention. Kidderminster, Worcs: B.I.L.D. Publications Harrison, F. & Crow, M. (1993) Living and learning with blind children: a guide for parents and teachers of visually impaired children. Toronto: University of Toronto Press. Herbert, M, (1981) Behavioural treatment of problem children: a practice manual. London: Academic Press. Hetherington, E.M. (1972) Effects of father absence on personality development in adolescent daughters. Developmental Psychology, 7, 313-326. Hingsburger, D. (1994) Masturbation: a consultation for those who support individuals with developmental disabilities. Canadian Journal of Human Sexuality 3, 278-282. Hoffmann, F.J. (1975) Use of the Adlerian model in secondary school counseling and consulting. Individual Psychologist, 12, 27-32. p.19 of 28 Howlin, P. & Rutter, M. (1987) Treatment of autistic children. Chichister: Wiley. Hunt, A. & Shepherd, C. (1993) A prevalence study of autism in tuberous sclerosis. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 23, 323-339. Irwin, K., Edwards, M & Robinson, R. (2000) Psychogenic non-epileptic seizures: management and prognosis. Archives of Disease in Childhood, 82, 474-478. Jacobs, B.S. & Moss, H.A. (1976) Birth order and sex of siblings as determinants of mother-infant interaction. Child Development, 47, 315-322. Jessee, P.O., Strickland, M.P. & Ladewig, B.H. (1992) The aftereffects of a hostage situation on children’s behavior. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry,62,309-12. Johnson, D.L. & Breckenridge, J.N. ( 1982) The Houston parent-child development center and the primary prevention of behavior problems in young children, American Journal of Community Psychology, 10, 305-316. Jones, C.C., Vallano, G., Ryan, E., Helsel, W.J. & Rancurello, M.D. (1991) Selfinjurious behavior : strategies for assessment and management. Psychiatric Annals, 21, 310-317. Jordan, R. & Powell, S. (1995) Understanding and Teaching Children with Autism. Chichester: John Wiley & Sons. Kashani, J.H., Horwitz, E., Ray, J.S. & Reid, J.C. (1986) DSM-III diagnostic classification of 100 preschoolers in a child development unit. Child Psychiatry and Human Development, 16, 137-147. Kearney, C.A. (2001) School refusal behaviour in youth: a functional approach to assessment and treatment. Washington DC: American Psychological Association. Lee, M.I. & Miltenberger, R.G. (1996) School refusal behavior: classification, assessment and treatment issues. Education and Treatment of Children,19, 474-486. Leung. A.K.C., Robson, W.L.M. & Lim, S.H.B. (1992) Stealing in children. The Journal of the Singapore Paediatric Society, 34, 136-140. Lochman, J.E., Bennett, C.L., Simmers, D.A. & Anderson, E. ( 1986) Multiple sources of behavioural ratings for aggressive youth: staff agreement and discordant youth. Journal of Child and Adolescent Psychotherapy, 3, 91-98. p.20 of 28 Lorber, N.M. (1969) Concomitants of social acceptance: a review of research. Psychology: a Journal of Human Behaviour, 6, 53-59. Lowenstein, L.F.(1978) The bullied and non bullied child. Home and School, 12, 34. Luby, J.L., Reich, W. & Earls, F. (1995) Failure to detect signs of psychological distress in the preschool children of alcoholic parents. Journal of Child and Adolescent Substance Abuse, 42,77-89. Lyle, J.G. (1961) Some personality characteristics of “trainable” children in relation to verbal ability. American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 66, 69-75. Maier, H. W. (1988) Three theories of child development. Lanham MD: University Press of America. Manheimer, D.I. & Mellinger, G.D. (1967) Personality characteristics of the child accident repeater. Child Development, 38,491- 513. Massey, O.T. & Murphy, S.E ( 1991) A study of the utility of the child behavior checklist with residentially placed children. Evaluation and Program Planning, 14, 319-324. McDermott, P.A. (1984) Child behavior disorders by age and sex based on item factoring of the revised Bristol guides. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 12, 15-36. McGhee, P.E. & Lloyd, S. (1982) Behavioral characteristics associated with the development of humor in young children. The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 141, 253-259. McManus, M. (1995) Troublesome behaviour in the classroom: meeting individual needs (2nd ed.) London: Routledge. Mellor, N. (1997) Attention seeking : a practical solution for the classroom. London: Paul Chapman. Mellor, N. (2000) The good, the bad and the irritating: a practical approach for parents of children who are attention seeking. London: Paul Chapman. Mellor, N. (2001) Dealing with those pupils who crave recognition. Early Years Educator, 3, 18-20. Mellor, N. (2003) The riddle of attention seeking. Paper presented at the BPS/DECP annual conference, Harrogate, January 2003. p.21 of 28 Mellor, N. (2004a ) Attention seeking: a perspective from chaos theory. Paper presented at the Association of Educational Psychologists conference, Stratford on Avon, November 2004. Mellor, N. (2004b) TIPS: time-limited intervention with parents service. DECP Debate, 112, 9-13. Mellor, N. and Harvey, E. (2003) Attention Seeking: parent interviews - a training pack for practitioners. Available from first author. Mendelson, M.J., Aboud, F.E. , Lanthier, R.P. (1994) Personality predictors of friendship and popularity in kindergarten. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 15, 413-435. Merrett, F (1993) Encouragement works best: positive approaches to classroom management. London: David Fulton. Montgomery, D. (1989) Managing behaviour problems. London: Hodder and Stoughton. Morgan, G. M. & Hastings, R.P. (1998) Special educators’ understanding of challenging behaviours in children with learning disabilities: sensitivity to information about behavioural function. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy,26, 43-52. Munro, H.M.C. & Thrusfield, M.V. (2001) “Battered pets”: Munchausen syndrome by proxy (factitious illness by proxy). The Journal of Small Animal Practice, 42, 385 –389. Murphy, G. & Wilson, B. (Eds.) (1985) Self injurious behaviour: a collection of published papers on prevalence, causes, and treatment in people who are mentally handicapped or autistic. Kidderminster: BIMH National Mental Health Education Project for Young Deaf People (1996). Project Update. October, 1, www.vicdeaf.com.au/ourservices/ youth/ youth_pdf/oct96_1.pdf O’Donnell, J.O. & Van Tuinan, M. (1979) Behavior problems of preschool children: dimensions and congenital correlates. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 7, 61-75. Odendaal, J.S.J (2000) Animal-assisted therapy – magic or medicine? Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 49, 275-280. p.22 of 28 Oppenheim, D. (1997) The attachment doll play interview for preschoolers. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 20, 681-697. Peagam, E. (1994) Special needs or educational apartheid? The emotional and behavioural difficulties of Afro-Caribbean children. Support for Learning, 9, 33-38. Peretti, P.O., Clark, D. & Johnson, P. (1984) Parental rejection as a criterion measure of negative attention seeking classroom behaviour among elementary school students. Psychologia, 27, 50-55. Phillips, R. (1990) Post-adoption services – the views of adopters. Early Child Development and Care, 59, 21-27. Pollard, A. (1985) The social world of the primary school. London: Cassell Educational. Rai, S., Malik, S.C. & Sharma, D. (1993) Behavior problems among preschool children. Indian Pediatrics, 30, 475-478. Reber, A.S. & Reber, E.S. (2001) The Penguin dictionary of psychology. London: Penguin. Rees, C., Farrell, P. & Rees, P. (2003) Coping with complexity: how do educational psychologists assess students with emotional and behavioural difficulties? Educational Psychology in Practice, 19, 35-47. Robertson, J. (1989) Effective classroom control: understanding teacher-pupil relationships. London: Hodder and Stoughton. Rogers, W.A. (2000) Classroom behaviour: a practical guide to effective teaching, behaviour management and colleague support. London: Books Education. Rosenthal, M.K. (1967 a) The generalization of dependency behaviour from mother to stranger. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 8, 117-133. Rosenthal, M.K. (1967 b) Effects of a novel situation and of anxiety on two groups of dependency behaviours. British Journal of Psychology, 58, 357-364. Saunders, M. (1979) Class control and behaviour problems: a guide for teachers. London: McGraw-Hill. Saxena, V. (1992) Perceived maternal rejection as related to negative attention seeking classroom behaviour among primary school children. Journal of Personality and Clinical Studies, 8,129-135. p.23 of 28 Scher, G. (1971) The relationship between parental reinforcement and attention seeking behaviors in four year olds. PhD dissertation, North Western University. Scott, E. (1980) David : a problem of soiling. In : S. Murgatroyd (Ed.) Helping the troubled child: interprofessional case studies. London: Harper and Row. Sears, R.R., Rau, L. & Alpert, R. (1965) Identification and child rearing. Stanford: Stanford University Press. Shaw, D.S., Keenan, K. & Vondra, J.I. (1994) Developmental precursors of externalising behavior: ages 1 to3. Developmental Psychology, 30, 355-364. Singh, S. & Kapur, R. (1984) Psychometric and behavioral correlates of group Rorschach measures of hostility. Indian Journal of Clinical Psychology, 11, 35-44. Speltz. M.C. (1990) The treatment of preschool conduct problems. In : M.T. Greenberg, D.Cicchetti & E.M. Cummings (Eds) Attachment in the Preschool Years. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Sroufe, L.A., Fox, N.E. & Pancake, Van R. (1983) Attachment and dependency in developmental perspective. Child Development, 54, 1615 –1627. Stabler, B., Clopper, R.R., Siegel, P.T., Stoppani, C., Compton, P.G. & Underwood, L.E. (1994) Academic achievement and psychological adjustment in short children. Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 15, 1-6. Stein, A.H. & Wright , J.C. (1964) Imitative learning under conditions of nurturance and nurturance withdrawal. Child Development, 35, 927-938. Stein, S.M. (1998) Enuresis, early attachment and intimacy. British Journal of Psychotherapy,15, 167-176. Stjernqvist, K. (1996) The birth of an extremely low birth weight infant (ELBW) <901g: impact on the family after 1 and 4 years. Journal of Reproductive and Infant Psychology, 14, 243-264. Stott, D.H. (1974) The social adjustment of children: Bristol Social Adjustment Guides Manual (5th ed.) London: Hodder and Stoughton. Stott, D.H., Marston, N.C. & Neill, S.J. ( 1975) Taxonomy of behaviour disturbance. London: University of London Press. Surya Prakash Rao, N.V.S. (1977) Adjustment reaction of childhood. Child Psychiatry Quarterly, 10, 8-26. p.24 of 28 Taleb, T.A. (2001) The prevalence of physical punishment choice among mothers of kindergarten children, the types of behaviors identified as deserving punishment and the relationship between punishment and children’s behavioral characteristics. Dirasat: Educational Sciences, 28, 229-246. Taylor, J. C. (1994) Functional assessment and functionally-derived treatment for child behavior problems. Special Services in the Schools, 9, 39-67. Taylor, J.C. & Carr, E.G. (1992) Severe behavior problems related to social interaction. 1: attention seeking and social avoidance Behavior Modification, 16, 305-335. Taylor, J.C., Sisson,L.A., McKelvey,J.L. & Trefelner, M.F. ( 1993) Situation specificity in attention seeking problem behavior. Behavior Modification, 17, 474-497. Tizard, J. (1970) New trends in developmental psychology. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 40, 1-7. Turner, P.J. (1991) Relations between attachment, gender and behavior with peers in pre-school. Child Development, 62,1475-1488. Van Dijk, J. (1991) Persons handicapped by rubella: victors and victims – a followup study. Amsterdam: Swets & Zeitlinger. Wade, B. & Moore, M. (1984) Coping with disruption at school. Special Education: Forward Trends. 11, 27-30. Webster - Stratton, C. & Herbert, M. (1995) Troubled families – problem children. Working with parents: a collaborative approach. Chichester: John Wiley and Sons. Welford, H. (2002) Look at me! Right Start, May/June, 21-23. Wickstrom, D.L. & Fleck, J.R. (1983) Missionary children : correlates of self-esteem and dependency. Journal of Psychology and Theology, 11, 226-235. Wiessen, A.E. & Watson, E. (1967) Elimination of attention seeking behavior in a retarded child. American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 72, 50-52. Williams, C.L., Ben-Porath, Y.S. & Weed, N.C. (1990) Ratings of behavior problems in adolescents hospitalised for substance abuse. Journal of Adolescent Chemical Dependency, 1, 95-112. Wolman, B.B. (1989) Dictionary of behavioral science (2nd edn.). San Diego: Academic Press p.25 of 28 Zeanah, C.H., Boris, N.W. & Scheeringa, M.S. (1997) Psychopathology in infancy. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 38, 81-99. Appendix 1 Examples of the wide range of behaviours which have been described in certain contexts as attention seeking. Aggression; disruption; self stimulation (Taylor & Carr, 1992). Challenging behaviour (Harris et al,1996). Coming in late; reading failure (Balson, 1982). Disruptive and easily distracted, lacking in concentration, seeking to gain friendship by any means, soiling himself frequently (Scott, 1980). Fighting, kicking, biting, boisterous, talkative, restless, swearing, quarrelsome, throwing things, whispering, crying, pouting, stubbornness, temper tantrums (Saxena, 1992); laziness (McManus, 1995). Licking her nose to make children sick, cheating, destroying her coat. Restlessness, touching other children, picking his nose, crawling under desks etc., dropping things thus providing excuses to grope around rocking on his chair, laughing quietly at nothing in particular, staring at his neighbour until their attention is obtained, moving furniture about, losing worksheets or destroying them, humming, banging under desk with feet. Tantrums; coming without equipment (Montgomery, 1989). Putting up his hand to ask unnecessary questions, lying on his back with his legs in the air and banging his head on the desk. Unable to remain in his seat during group work, continually interfering with other children (Merrett, 1993). Fake illnesses (Pollard 1985); accident repeating (Manheimer & Mellinger, 1967); pseudoseizures (Buchanan & Snars, 1993, Irwin et al, 2000 ). Running off, tantrums, shouting, swearing, kicking, stealing, urinating in containers (Wade & Moore, 1984). Scapegoating (Saunders, 1979). Running round barefoot, loud burps (Galloway, 1976). Screaming, whining and crying; biting hands or hitting head with hand; saying “No” to adult requests (Carr & Durand, 1985). Shouting out; asking questions unrelated to work (Gray & Richer, 1988). Stealing (Leung et al,1992); self injury ( Jones et al,1991). p.26 of 28 Tapping pencils; whining; remaining in the corridor; wearing long earrings; stealing; lying (Dreikurs et al,1971). Teasing; exhibitionism; practical joking (Maier, 1988). Tics, stammering, bedwetting, temper tantrums, thumb sucking, food refusal, pain in the knees, breath holding spasms, pain in abdomen, headache, hand shaking (Surya Prakash Rao, 1977) Appendix 2: Citations of attention seeking In conjunction with special difficulties Accident repeating (Manheimer & Mellinger, 1967); ADHD (Cooper & Ideus, 1996); autism (Howlin & Rutter, 1987); cerebral palsy (Finnie, 1974); deafblindness (Van Dijk, 1991); drug abuse (Coghlan, 1973); Down syndrome (Byrne et al,1988); FG syndrome and Williams syndrome (Graham et al,1999); hearing impairment (N.M.H.E.P., 1996); learning difficulties (Barton et al,1987, Carr & Durand, 1985; Donoghue & Abbas, 1971; Gardner, 1971; Hingsburger, 1994; Lyle 1961; Taylor & Carr, 1992;Tizard, 1970; Wiessen & Watson, 1967) and children in mainstream with apparent learning difficulties (Balson, 1982); poor social acceptance (Lorber, 1969); pseudoseizures (Buchanan & Snars, 1993, Irwin et al, 2000 ); school refusal (Kearney, 2001; Lee & Miltenberger, 1996); self injurious behaviour (Murphy & Wilson, 1985 provide a large number of examples; see also Jones et al,1991); short stature (Stabler et al, 1994); tuberous sclerosis (Hunt & Shepherd, 1993); victims of bullying (Lowenstein, 1978) and false victims (Besag, 1989); visual impairment (Harrison & Crow, 1993). In different countries/cultures Bangladesh (Faruk & Deb, 1992); Finland and Iceland (Hannesdottir et al, 2000); India (Bhogle, 1983; Rai et al, 1993; Saxena, 1992; Singh & Kapur, 1984; Surya Prakash Rao, 1977); Israel (Oppenheim, 1997); Jordan ( Taleb, 2001); Macedonia and Germany (Dikaiou, 1989); Mexican American ( Johnson & Breckenridge, 1982); Netherlands (Fahrenfort et al, 1996); St. Helena (Charlton et al, 1994); Sweden (Stjernqvist, 1996). p.27 of 28 p.28 of 28