Meditating Role of Cognitive Load on the Relationship Between Navigation Structure, Prior Knowledge, and Performance in Accounting Case Analyses Abstract: Accounting case analysis is an important part of accounting curriculum. Instructors encourage students to complete cases independently in order to learn from them. With availability of web technologies inside and outside the classroom, it is important to understand how self-directed learning can be promoted by using web technologies like hypertext. When web-based case studies are used, there is one common technological component: use of hypertext. Use of hypertexts can aid self-directed learning. However, hypertexts can facilitate learning only when properly structured, otherwise they cause disorientation (lost in space). Thus, it is imperative that we understand how navigation structure of hypertext allows for improved learning during accounting case analysis. One potential effect of hypertext is increasing the extraneous cognitive load, which distracts students from the core learning activity. Hypertexts arranged in different navigation structures, might affect extraneous cognitive load differently. In this paper I suggest a study to explore which hypertext navigation structure leads to decrease in cognitive load, and how cognitive load impedes learning from a case study. I also suggest how prior knowledge can moderate the effect of navigation structure on cognitive load. 1 INTRODUCTION Web-based learning is becoming popular. Many universities have started offering online accounting courses and these are becoming popular among employed students. For these online courses, instructors interact with students only via Internet. Even instructors teaching in conventional classrooms use Internet technologies (e.g. WebCT, Blackboard, customized websites) extensively to improve their instructional delivery. Accounting case analysis is an important part of any accounting curriculum and accounting educators advocate the benefits of these case materials (Gobeil and Philips, 2001). Accounting educators can encourage independent case-based learning by placing these case materials on the web. However, it is important to present these cases in a manner that extracts maximum leverage from web technology. One important component of web technology is hypertext. Hypertexts have the potential to be used as a tool for improving independent learning from accounting cases. For example, Crandall and Philips (2002) suggested that accounting hypertext-based case studies could be used from concept knowledge to applications to improve learning. But, the dilemma is how to present these cases because certain studies have exhibited the problem of disorientation because of the use of hypertext (e.g. Bible, Graham, and Rosman, 2005). Thus, we need to find ways to improve learning from hypertext-based cases in a way that causes least disorientation. Hypertexts can have different navigation structures and one pertinent question is how hypertext navigation structures affect learning from cases. This will allow educators to use the right navigation structure for imparting learning from hypertext-based case studies. 2 This paper outlines a model to study the impact of different hypertext navigation structures, and prior knowledge, on cognitive load of students when they attempt to learn from an accounting case. In particular, it studies two navigation structure: 1) hierarchical structures, where a node at one level can access another node directly at a level above or below, thus forming a hierarchy, 2) network structures, which allow a node to be connected to any other node in the hypertext forming a complex structure with many links. The paper also explores the moderating impact of prior knowledge on cognitive load. Case studies are also used as a training tool in auditing firms. Compared to students, auditors have well developed prior knowledge structures. Their prior knowledge has the potential to moderate the influence of navigation structures on cognitive load. Further, I outline a research method to validate this model. THEORY Cognitive load theory explains the linkage between instructional design and cognitive processes (Paas, Renkl et al. 2003). When new knowledge is to be acquired, the learning takes place through working memory. Working memory is a location “where current mental activity takes place and is a cognitive structure that is very limited in both capacity and duration” (Carlson, Chandler and Sweller, 2003; p.629). While short-term memory has been conceived as a passive storage buffer, working memory is assumed to have both processing as well as storage functions (Daneman and Carpenter, 1980). Working memory processes information, and stores the products of that processing for a short duration. After new information has been processed through working memory, schema formation in the long term memory can occur. Thus, the processing of new 3 information depends on both the existing schema as well as the learners’ processing capacity (Leidner and Jarvenpaa, 1995). Cognitive load theory suggests that learning occurs through a limited working memory. When information is presented to learners, working-memory will be critical in determining the effectiveness of learning (Carlson et al., 2003; Sweller et al. 1998). When learners’ limited working memory is exceeded, cognitive overload occurs. This impedes learning (Pierce et al., 1993). There are three components to cognitive load: intrinsic cognitive load, extraneous cognitive load, and germane cognitive load (Paas, Renkl et al. 2003). Intrinsic cognitive load is inherent to the level of complexity of the task. If a task is broken down into elements, then complex tasks require higher element interaction for overall understanding of the task as compared to simpler tasks. The elements of the concept to be learned cannot be understood in isolation. This high element interactivity imposes demands on working memory capacity and thus increases cognitive load. Learning vocabulary is an example of low element interaction, whereas learning a language is an example of high element interactivity. For example, learning from an accounting case analysis can be considered as a complex task. To understand the case requires high interactivity between the elements and simultaneous processing. For a risk-based auditing case, students must understand the client’s business strategy, the business processes to achieve the strategy, and the key indicators necessary to monitor performance of business processes in achieving the strategy. They must also find the linkages between the business environment, business strategy and the business processed. Subsequently, they decide how these interactions 4 affect the risk judgment at the transaction level. The intrinsic cognitive load for case analysis would be high because of this complexity. Intrinsic cognitive load is difficult to change with instructional designs. Extraneous cognitive load occurs because of instructional techniques that require learners to engage in activities that are not directed towards learning (Sweller 1994; Gerjets and Scheiter 2003). Evidence suggests that instructional designs that decrease extraneous cognitive load improve learning (e.g. Carlson et al., 2003; Brunken et al., 2002; Kirschner, 2002). Thus, using specific features in the computerized aids that decrease the cognitive capacity needed for activities extraneous to learning can improve learning from decision aids (Paas, 1992; Rose, 1998). For example, using a particular hypertext navigation structure might lead to increase or decrease in cognitive load. Germane cognitive load is related to processes that contribute to the schema development (Paas, Tuovinen et al. 2003). Unlike intrinsic and extraneous cognitive load, germane cognitive load contributes to learning. Germane cognitive load occurs when the task is simple enough to leave sufficient working memory available so that learners may devote more effort to processes that are directly relevant for learning (Sweller, van Merrienboer et al. 1998). Thus, when the task is highly complex, germane cognitive load does not occur. These three components of cognitive load are additive, i.e. the sum of intrinsic cognitive load, extrinsic cognitive load, and germane cognitive load form overall cognitive load, which must stay within the limits of working memory to promote learning. 5 PRIOR LITERATURE Hypertext-based learning Hypertexts can be effective in self-directed learning because they allow users to choose which links to follow and allow users to work their way through the material along various paths of their own (Muller-Kaltoff and Moller, 2003). Researchers posit that hypertexts are suited for learning in complex domains (Mao et al., 1996; Spiro et al., 1998). They suggest that when a complex domain is presented in a flexible way such that it is possible to revisit the same material through different hypertexts at different times and in rearranged contexts, hypertexts improve learning (Balcytiene, 1999; Spiro and Jehng, 1990). Some evidence suggests that hypertexts can enhance learning. For example, research shows that hypertexts effectively encourage contextualized access to knowledge within a decision aid (Mao and Benbasat, 1998). Further, educational researchers suggest that hypertexts facilitate learning with a task that includes a specific goal (Last and O’Donnell, 2001). However, researchers also contend that hypertexts can impair learning. The impairment in learning can occur if users have become disoriented because of confusion regarding search for information (Conklin, 1987; McKnight et al., 1991). Within accounting, there is little hypertext-related research (Dull et al., 2003). The few studies in accounting show mixed results. Crandall and Phillips (2002) suggest that the use of hypertext-based instructional materials produce greater knowledge application than identical instructional material that does not include hypertexts. In contrast, Hodge (2001) states that participants who use hypertext-based financial statements are more prone to grouping unaudited information with audited information, 6 as compared to participants who view the paper materials. Similarly, Bible et al. (2005) find that auditors using an electronic hypertext system were less able to identify seeded errors as compared to auditors in the traditional audit paper environment. Navigation Structures and hypertext: Though hypertexts are useful for learning, they might cause disorientation (Conklin, 1987; Foss, 1989; McKnight et al., 1991) because some hypertext navigation structure can affect an individual’s ability to locate and extract information (Van Dyke Parunak, 1989; Batra et al., 1993). However, McDonald and Stevenson (1996) suggested that disorientation (getting lost in space) can be minimized by use of right navigation structures. Shin et al. (1994) assessed that the most popular navigational structures for hypertext are hierarchical and network structures. In hierarchical structures, a node at one level can access another node directly at a level above or below, thus forming a hierarchy. On the other hand, network structures allow a node to be connected to any other node in the hypertext forming a complex structure with many links (McDonald and Stevenson, 1996). This study focuses on these two navigation structures. Prior knowledge and learning Auditing research studies comprise a vast literature exploring the effect of prior knowledge on auditing judgments. I illustrate two studies, rather than exhaustively review this literature. Libby and Luft (1993) proposed a model to study the effect of experience and knowledge on auditing performance (See Figure 1). Further, they conducted an experimental study based on this model. They find that professionals with 7 more domain knowledge make better decisions than professionals with less domain knowledge. Recently, O’Donnell, Koch and Boone (2005) examined how prior procedural knowledge interacts with task complexity when tax professionals develop client recommendations. They find that the prior knowledge of tax professionals influences their recommendations when the task complexity is high, but has no influence when the task complexity is low1. Researchers studying the effectiveness of the use of hypertexts in computerized aids considered the effect of prior knowledge (Hoffman and Oostendorp, 1999; Fang, 2001). Kelly (1993) found that users with little prior knowledge tend to get lost in the maze of hypertext easily. Similarly, Akanabi and Dwyer (1989) noted the importance of prior knowledge while studying the effect of hypertext structure on learning performance. They find that students with prior knowledge benefit more in less structured and inductive learning environment and students without prior knowledge benefit more in highly structured and deductive environment. HYPOTHESES DEVELOPMENT Based on Cognitive Load theory and prior literature, the overall model for the research study is as shown in figure 2. Effect of navigation structure Poorly designed hypertext may increase cognitive load, if users must remember their location in the network, make decisions about where to go next, and keep track of 1 These studies focused on schema that matched with the information to be learned. However, these results might change when information to be acquired significantly differs from the existing schema of auditors, i.e. there is a mismatch of information to be learned versus existing schema. 8 pages previously visited (Rouet et al., 1996; Wright, 1991). Network hypertext navigation structure provides more number of choices to the user, in terms of the different routes they may follow. The user must use additional effort and concentration to perform the tasks needed for navigation as the user is presented with an abundance of navigational alternatives. Thus, the limited working memory capacity is used for processing the decision on which route to follow, and the location in the network. This leads to an increase in extraneous cognitive load. Thus, H1: Participants who are presented with an accounting case having network navigation hypertext structure will experience more extraneous cognitive load than will participants who are presented with an accounting case having horizontal navigation hypertext structure. Effect of prior knowledge on learning from a hypertext-based computerized aid Learning occurs by mapping the concept to be learned to existing knowledge structures. The person with prior knowledge decreases the complexity of the task by his or her ability to link to the existing knowledge structures. Thus, intrinsic cognitive load, which is related to the complexity of the task, decreases leading to an overall decrease in cognitive load. So, I hypothesize: H2a: Participants with prior knowledge will have less cognitive load as compared to participants without prior knowledge when they are presented with an hypertextbased accounting case. 9 As intrinsic cognitive load decreases, participants have more working memory space to accommodate the demands of any presentation style. So, when these participants are presented with a network navigation structure, they are impacted less as compared to participants without prior knowledge with regard to cognitive load. H2b: Prior knowledge will moderate the impact of navigation structure on cognitive load. Effect of cognitive load on learning To facilitate learning, the web presentation structure should decrease this cognitive load so that cognitive resources are devoted to learning. With low cognitive load navigation structure, participants can take advantage of the flexibility provided by hypertext to foster learning. Thus, H3: Participants who experience low cognitive load will learn more from an accounting case analysis. RESEARCH DESIGN I will conduct an experiment with a 2x2 between-participants design. The 2x2 between-participants manipulations will be “navigation structure” and “prior knowledge” Prior knowledge has two levels: (1) Students who have taken the class related to the case study (2) Students who did not take that class yet. The variable “navigation structure” has two levels: 1) Hierarchical, and 2) Network. Masters level accounting students will be randomly assigned to one of the four groups in the 2x2 factorial designs. Participants have to go through an auditing case adapted from an auditing textbook that is a part of the curriculum. The case is related to 10 auditing the revenue cycle. Participants will be requested to participate either before or after the class on auditing the revenue cycle. Participants will go through the case using either hierarchical navigational structure or network navigational structure. The study will be conducted in a controlled environment, although the computerized aid will be accessed over the Internet. The universities would participate I this study will have similar computing facilities and they would have followed the same textbook for instruction. GMAT scores will be compared to find uniformity among participants’ ability. I choose Masters student participants for specific reasons in this project. First my study needs to be generalized to accounting student learning. So, student participants are appropriate. Second, case analysis is more prevalent with Masters program as compared to undergraduate programs. Third, the complexity of the task is best suited for Masters students. The task would have confused undergraduate students. MEASURING DEPENDENT VARIABLES There are two dependent variables: cognitive load and learning performance. Researchers measured cognitive load with either analytical methods, physiological techniques or rating scale techniques (Paas, Tuovinen, Tabbers, and Van Gervan, 2003). Researchers have also used NASA-TLX (Task Load Index) to capture cognitive load2(Gerjets and Scheiter, 2004; Morris, Speier, and Hoffer, 1999). I will use NASATLX. I have chosen this measure for two reasons. First, it has been extensively validated. Second, it is easy to administer as compared to other measurements. 2 For a detailed description of the NASA-TLX, please refer to Hart and Staveland (1988) 11 As stated by ACT theory (Anderson, 1982), learning occurs in two stages. First the learner acquires the relevant facts and second the learner applies the relevant facts to different situations. Thus, I measure learning in terms of 1) Acquisition of relevant facts in the accounting case 2) Application of relevant facts in the accounting case. A similar measure has been used and validated by Gobeil and Philips (2001). CONTRIBUTIONS AND LIMITATIONS This paper suggests a model for exploring the impact of hypertext navigation structure, and prior knowledge on learning in accounting case analysis. The study can contribute to the research on learning using web-based case analysis in several ways. The proposed research addresses an emerging accounting education issue: in recent years, web-based learning is becoming popular. In spite of pervasive use of hypertext in web-based learning, we don’t know yet how to structure hypertexts in a way that facilitates learning from accounting cases. This study will guide us in that direction. Second, the study will assist in finding how level of knowledge affects the effect of different navigation structures on learning from an accounting case. Someone who is more knowledgeable will more probably need a network structure as compared to someone who is less knowledgeable who will need a hierarchy structure. Third, the study improves our understanding about the effect of navigation structures on learning based on cognitive load theory. Fourth, the study may assist auditing firms in their training and in knowledge acquisition. Web-based auditing software and computerized aids are increasingly popular in auditing firms as knowledge acquisition tools. For example KPMG uses a web-based software called KRisk(SM) for strategic systems auditing (Bell 12 et al., 2002). Auditing teams of Ernst & Young use a web-based portfolio of audit tools called EY/NexGen. Similarly, Delloitte & Touche use web-based audit systems ACL Web (Krasss, 2002). Our study will be a preliminary stdu that can be replicated with auditors to understand impact of navigation structures on performance in auditing settings. In this study, I will limit the number of hypertext links per page to maximum 10 because I want to keep the study simple. However, the results might differ when more number of links is used. Further research needs to be done by varying the number of hypertext links. One possible study is to find out how the number of hypertext links provided in web-based cases affects cognitive load, and how that affects learning. This study attempts to explore prior knowledge that matches with existing knowledge structures. Future studies can also explore the effect of prior knowledge that is disparate and does not match with existing knowledge structure. That may have a negative effect on learning from 13 hypertext based case analysis. Experience Knowledge Performance Ability Figure 1: Libby and Luft Model of the Effect of Experience, Knowledge, and Ability on Performance (1993) Navigational Structure Cognitive Load Learning from Accounting Case Analysis Prior accounting Knowledge Figure2: Suggested Research model for hypertext-based accounting case learning 14 REFERENCES Anderson, J. R. (1982). "Acquisition of Cognitive Style," Psychological Review, 89, p. 369. Balcytiene, Aukse (1999). “Exploring Individual Processes of Knowledge Construction with Hypertext,” Instructional Science, 27, p. 303. Bell, T. B., Bedard, J. C., Johnstone, K. M., & Smith, E. F. (2002). “KRisk(SM): A computerized decision aid for client acceptance and continuance risk assessments,” Auditing: A Journal of Practice and Theory, 21, p. 97. Brunken, R., Steinbacher, S., Plass, J.L., Leutner, D. (2002). “Assessment of cognitive load in multimedia learning using dual-task methodology,” Experimental Psychology, 49, p. 109. Carlson, R., Chandler, P., and Sweller, J. (2003). “Learning and understanding science instructional material,” Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, p. 629. Conklin, J. (1987). “Hypertext: An introduction and Survey,” IEEE Computer, 20, p. 17. Crandall, D., and Phillips, F. (2002). “Using Hypertext in Instructional Material: Helping Students Link Accounting Concept Knowledge to Case Applications,” Issues in Accounting Education p, 163-183. Daneman, M. and Carpenter, P.A., (1980), “Individual Differences in Working Memory and Reading”, Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 19, p.450. Dull, R.B., Graham, A.W. and Baldwin, A.A. (2003) “Web-based financial statements: hypertext links to footnotes and their effect on decisions,” International Journal of Accounting Information Systems, 4, p.185. Fang, X. (2001). Web site design factors and usability: A navigation structure experiment, Ph.D. Dissertation, University of Kentucky. Foss, C L (1989), "Tools for reading and browsing hypertext," Information Processing and Management, 25, p. 407-418. Gerjets P. and K. Scheiter (2003). “Goal Configurations and Processing Strategies as Moderators between Instructional Design and Cognitive Load: Evidence from HypertextBased Instruction.” Educational Psychologist, 38, p. 33. Gerjets P. and K. Scheiter (2004). “Designing Instructional Examples to Reduce Intrinsic Cognitive Load: Molar versus Modular Presentation of Solution Procedures.” Instructional Science, 32, p. 33. Hodge, F.D. (2001). “Hyperlinking unaudited information to audited financial statements: Effects on investor judgments,” The Accounting Review, 76, p. 675. 15 Hoffman, R. & van Oostendorp, H. (1999). “Cognitive effects of a structural overview in a hypertext,” British Journal of Educational Technology, 30, p. 129. Kirschner, P.A. (2002). “Cognitive load theory: implications of cognitive load theory on the design of learning,” Learning and Instruction, 12, p.1. Krasss, P. (2002). “The Never-ending Audit: Can software prevent future Enrons?,” CFO Asia, 2002(Nov); http://www.cfoasia.com/archives/200211-08.htm (Accessed: 14th March, 2004). Libby, R. (1981). Accounting and Human Information Processing: Theory and Applications, Prentice Hall. Libby. R., R. Bloomfield and M. W. Nelson (2002). "Experimental Research in Financial Accounting," Accounting Organizations and Society, p. 775. Last, D.A., O’Donnell, A.M. (2001). “The effects of prior knowledge and goal strength on the use of hypertext,” Journal of Educational Multimedia and Hypermedia, 10, p. 3. Leidner, D.E., Jarvenpaa, S.L. (1995). “The use of Information Technology to enhance Management School Education: A theoretical view,” MIS Quarterly, 19, p. 24. Mao, J., and Benbasat, I. (1998). "Contextualized Access to Knowledge in Knowledgebased Systems: A Process Tracing Study," Information Systems Journal, 8, p. 217. McKnight C., Dillon A. & Richardson J., (1991). Hypertext in Context, Cambridge University Press. Muller-Kaltoff, T. and Moller, J. (2003). “The effects of graphical overviews, prior knowledge, and self-concept on hypertext disorientation and learning environment,” Journal of Educational Multimedia and Hypermedia, 12, p.117. Paas, F. (1992). “Training strategies for attaining transfer of problem-solving skills in statistics: A cognitive-load approach,” Journal of Educational Psychology, 84, p. 429. Paas, F., J. E. Tuovinen, et al. (2003), “Cognitive Load Measurement as a Means to Advance Cognitive Load Theory,” Educational Psychologist,” 38, p. 63. Paas, F., A. Renkl, et al. (2003). “Cognitive Load Theory and Instructional Design: Recent Developments,” Educational Psychologist, 38, p. 1. Pierce, K.A., Duncan, M.K., Gholson, B., Ray, G.E. and Kamhi, A.G. (1993). “Cognitive Load, Schema acquisition, and procedural adaptation in non-isomorphic analogical transfer,” Journal of Educational Psychology, 85, p. 66. 16 Rouet, J.-F., Levonen J. J., Dillon, A. & Spiro, R. J. (eds.), Hypertext and cognition. Mahwah, NJ. et al.: Erlbaum. Sweller, J., Chandler, P., Tierney, P. & Cooper, M. (1990). “Cognitive load as a factor in the structuring of technical material,” Journal of Experimental Psychology, 119, p. 176. Sweller, J. (1994). "Cognitive load theory, learning difficulty, and instructional design," Learning and Instruction, 4, p. 295. Sweller, J., van Merrienboer, J.J.G., and Paas, F.G.W.C. (1998). “Cognitive architecture and instructional design,” Educational Psychology Review, 10, p. 251. Tuovinen, J.E. and Sweller, J. (1999). “A comparison of cognitive load associated with discovery learning and worked examples”, Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, p. 334. 17