Achievement in Mathematics Running Head: Achievement in Mathematics A Determinate Model of Achievement in Mathematics 1 Achievement in Mathematics Abstract An expolinear function based on psychometric findings is derived to explain student achievement in mathematics over time. The function’s two parameters, Cm and k, are set by Spearman’s g, as measured by student mental age. Using data from large longitudinal samples, estimates of the function were made on 11 performance levels. Nonlinear regression analyses unexpectedly resulted in a significant R2 = 0.99. The disparity between previously reported results (R2 > .50) and these results imply the presence of a hidden variable in parameter, Cm. The hidden variable may be teacher effect. Keywords: asymptote, determinate growth, expolinear function, integration, logistic function, longitudinal achievement, mental age, mental development, rational function, Spearman’s g, teacher effect 2 Mathematics Achievement 3 A Determinate Model of Achievement in Mathematics There have been a great number of mathematical functions specified to explain student achievement in the forty years since the 1966 Coleman Report. Most of these functions explained achievement and attempted to measure effectiveness of schools or teachers by either of two statistical methods. Achievement has been analyzed primarily by using ANCOVA or multiple regression to find whether relationships existed between the myriad of factors thought to affect the learning of mathematics. The first model relied on averages, which has made finding small differences in achievement hard to detect. The second model included factors that were not under the control of schools (McCaffrey, Lockwood, Louis, Hamilton, 2004). A model that accurately explains achievement growth in mathematics is still needed. The purpose of this paper is to propose and test a functional model that relates student achievement in mathematics to time (development). Two studies support the idea that achievement is functionally related to intelligence. (Gedye, 1981) found by factoring teacher grading of elementary aged students (including math grades) that about half of the variance (r = 0.75, R2 = .56) could be explained by the Stanford-Binet test of intelligence. In addition, using a structural model, (Brodnick, 1995) found 46.4% of college student achievement, as measured by grades in mathematics and English, was causally linked with intelligence test scores. Both studies are consistent with Jensen’s conclusion that Spearman’s g is strongly correlated with student achievement (Jensen, 1998). Spearman’s g is a general mental ability that enters into every kind of activity requiring mental effort. Never observed directly, it underlies observed or measured variables and is called a latent variable. Since Mathematics Achievement 4 Spearman’s g cannot be directly measured, the best measure of it appropriate for education is the IQ test (Jensen, 1998). Since strong cases (Jensen, 1980, 1991, 1993, 1998; Matarazzo, 1972; Snow & Yalow, 1982) have been made that achievement is related to intelligence, it is paramount to know how students mature mentally. Mental growth of students is analogous to human physical growth from early childhood to maturity. It follows a determinate curve in time. Bloom (1964) thought systemic environmental influences could alter mental growth curves. He went on to assert, “We do not subscribe to the thesis that intelligence is a physical or neurological growth function analogous to height growth and that it must have a definite terminal growth point” (Bloom, 1964, p.81). First, we now know environmental influences on intelligence are not systemic, such as social and historical forces. Rather, the influences are small and idiosyncratic, like childhood diseases (Jensen, 1998, Chapter 7). Moreover, intelligence is indeed, a growth function like height growth. It has been demonstrated by studies of monozygotic twins (Jensen, 1998) that mental age grows in a determinate, biologic pattern over time set by inheritance. Moffitt (1993) found, while there were short run improvements in IQ for 90% of individual children, those improvements were not systematically associated with environmental changes. Of the remaining 10% with large improvements and plateaus, “this change is variable in its timing, idiosyncratic in its source, and temporary/transient in its course” (Moffitt, 1993, p.498). Overall, it was found IQ is “elastic, rather than plastic” (Moffitt, 1993, p.496). This implies stability in mental development as a student matures, and suggests that deep biological forces driven by heredity navigate the course for academic achievement. Mathematics Achievement 5 The key is to focus on developmental influences of intelligence. Since achievement is a function of intelligence, then the rise in student intelligence during maturation must accelerate the rate of achievement. Since intelligence is not a precise concept, it is replaced in the following discussion by the operational construct of mental age, because it is the measurable construct used to compare students on a relative basis to one another. The relationship between development (time) and student mental age is based on research demonstrating that mental age increases at a decreasing rate over time (Bayley, 1949, 1955; Heinis, 1924; Thorndike, 1927; Thurston, 1928). Also, Moffitt (1993) found, mental age changes in late adolescence were negligible. Bloom (1964) plotted five studies as shown in Figure 1 with time as the independent variable and percent of mature mental age as the dependent variable. The authors plotted the logistic curve over Bloom’s data for reference and discussed throughout the paper. They found percent of mature mental age ranged between 0% and 100% (one) at maturity. It has been found that infants can perform some mathematical operations without instruction shortly after birth (Wynn, 1992; Wynn, 1995; Slaughter, Kamppi & Paynter, 2006). Hence, 0% is set to conception, not birth. Mathematics Achievement 6 100 90 Percent of Mature Mental Age 80 70 60 50 40 Bayley Pt. Heinis 30 Thorndike Thurstone Bayley Grp. 20 Logistic 10 0 0 2 4 6 8 10 12 14 16 18 Age Since Conception Figure 1. Plot of Five Studies The exact curve of mental age growth has not been determined by research, although Bloom characterized this curve as parabolic (Bloom, 1964). This is consistent with his notion that there is no terminal limit to the growth of intelligence, since a parabola has no horizontal asymptote. Data from many studies demonstrating the mental age curve has a horizontal asymptote is summarized by Bayley (1955) and supported by Moffitt (1993). Instead of a parabola, it is likely a logistic curve, but not the curve of a rational function. Although both curves may be constructed, as in Bloom’s chart, with a zero intercept and a horizontal asymptote at 1, rational functions are unacceptable. This is because rational functions possess coefficients that can have no biological interpretation Mathematics Achievement 7 (Yin, 2003). The continuous changes inherent in biological growth are compatible with natural exponential growth found in logistic functions (Yin, 2003). Furthermore, logistic growth curves are based on the constraint of “carrying capacity,” a concept frequently found in biology (Weisstein, 2003). This constraint respects the finding by psychometry of mental maturity as the “carrying capacity” of intelligence. There may be a single logistic growth curve of mental age for all students. This is based on observations found in (Bayley, 1949, 1955; Heinis, 1924; Thorndike, 1927; Thurstone, 1928). It has not been determined by research to what degree the student logistic curves vary among one another. In summary, since mental age grows during students’ education, and mental age is highly correlated with achievement in mathematics, then there should be an impact from this monotonic increase. The model entitled, The Determinate Model of Mathematics Achievement uses time as the independent variable, and student achievement as the dependent variable. Model Description Because contemporary public, legislative, and academic communities are concerned with student achievement in mathematics, this investigation was designed to explain the learning of mathematics through a conceptual model. The determinate model is proposed herein as an alternative explanation of mathematics achievement. The model is based upon the most probable accounting of previously reported statistical findings explained and supported by the most current literature (Scllitz, 1966). The model is considered to be dynamic and thereby open to revision as new hypotheses are formulated Mathematics Achievement 8 and empirically tested. The following paragraphs present assumptions and considerations based upon epistemological and mathematical reasoning supported by the literature. The first postulate of the determinate model is student achievement in mathematics is produced by student intelligence. When students begin learning mathematics, they are not mentally mature. If they were mature, they could learn mathematics at a constant rate. In this model, the rate of learning starts at a slow pace and increases until it peaks at maturity. As discussed in the introduction, the authors postulate the rate of learning to be directly related to the maturation of student intelligence. The process of maturation in student intelligence can be tracked by mental age. Mental age is developmental and can be defined by using the following equation: (1) The term g in the equation is any student’s mental age now. M is a mature mental age, and ƒ is the percent of mature mental age. In concurrence with Bloom (Bloom, 1964) the ƒ term in the above equation grows over time with a definite shape. Research on the development of mental age verified the shape to be a logistic curve for all students. See Chart One for a scatter plot of data points taken from Bloom (1964) with a logistic curve applied for reference. The equation in (2) is a logistic function that relates how the percent increases with any student’s age. Mathematics Achievement 9 (2) The k term is a decimal exponent value, which is less than one and is called the coefficient of growth, and t is the term for time expressed in years since conception. This growth function adapted from biology (Goudriaan, 1990; Lee & Goudriaan, 2003) has a predictable path that begins at zero and increases to a horizontal asymptote; therefore, a basic assumption in this model is that mental development starts at 0 with conception and increases to 100% at maturity in late adolescence. The model claims the rate of learning is set by the mental age of students. The greater the mental age of a student, the faster new math content and skill can be learned. This functional relationship is in equation (3). (3) The term is the rate of learning of mathematics, whereas A is achievement, t is time, g is student mental age, and a is a parameter and the coefficient of proportionality between g and . In addition, a is expressed as units of math per year per mental age. Since student mental age grows to a maximum at maturity, then the maximum rate of growth in achievement can be represented by the following equation. Mathematics Achievement 10 (4) The term is the maximum rate of growth in achievement at maturity represented in units of mathematics per year and M is student mature mental age. As stated earlier, current student mental age is a percent ƒ of mature mental age. Then student achievement growth at any time is the product of the percent ƒ and the maximum rate of growth in achievement. Therefore, the equation becomes: (5) This means achievement growth is set by the percent of mature mental age, ƒ, and the maximum rate of growth in student achievement. The determinate model states that this percent grows along a path that is logistic and common to all students. Therefore, one equation builds upon another equation when the logistic expression from the second equation is substituted in for ƒ: (6) Mathematics Achievement 11 From this equation, we can predict student achievement levels. This is done by summing all of the achievement growth as students progress through math education. Student achievement growth changes each year because student effort becomes more productive due to maturation of their mental age. This continues until a maximum is reached at maturity. The summation of achievement growth is done by the mathematical operation of integration. To do this, equation (6) must be re-arranged in proper form. This is shown in equation (7) where the term dt is moved to the other side of the equation. (7) This function allows the summation of all achievement growth between conception and any time thereafter. Since Cm is constant, it is not summed. Now, the mathematical process of integration can occur as indicated in the following equation. (8) Mathematics Achievement 12 The integration process creates a function. Summing up all of the changes in achievement and all of the increases to student productive efforts over time by integration yields the final equation. (9) This model of mathematics achievement elucidates the assumption to be tested: Mathematics Achievement is a joint product of the maximum rate of mathematics achievement, Cm and the anti-derivative of the logistic mental maturity function. This function is more than just a summing of all the mathematics achievement since students become more mathematically productive as their mental age grows. The function in equation (9) is shown as a curve in Figure 2. It has two phases to it. The exponential phase, where the rate of learning begins at zero and rapidly rises as mental age grows in early childhood. In the second phase, the student’s mental development rate slows, causing the student’s rate of learning to grow at a constant or linear rate. Because of these two phases equation (9) is called an expolinear function. Mathematics Achievement 13 180 160 Linear Phase Math Achievement Score 140 120 100 80 Asymptote 60 40 Exponential Phase 20 0 0 2 4 6 8 10 12 14 16 Time Figure 2. Expolinear Growth Curve Testing the Determinate Model of Mathematics Achievement To test whether mathematics achievement is functionally related to a student’s age the expolinear function must be stable over different student achievement levels. To test the model longitudinal data was used with a uniform measure of mathematics achievement. The dataset used is from Choi, K. S., M. (2005). It is only available as CDROMs through UCLA, CREEST. The data points used in this study begin at Zero equaling conception. Whereas, the remaining sources for the data-points beginning at 0.75 and 1.75 years of age are from Wynn, K. (1992) and Slaughter, V. (2006). The dataset for kindergarten through eighth grade is called Early Childhood Longitudinal Study (ECLS). It is in three parts: Rathbun, A. W., West, J. (2004), and the second is found in Princiotta, D., Flanagan, K.D. & Germino Hausken, T. (2006) and the third is Mathematics Achievement 14 found in Walston, J., Rathbun, A., and Germino Hausken, E. (2008). The dataset for seventh through tenth grade is from the Longitudinal Study of American Youth (LSAY). It is found in Miller, J. D., Kimmel, L., Hoffer, T.B. & Nelson, C. (2000). All achievement data from the two longitudinal studies was converted from grade levels to mean age. The mean age at the beginning of the school year for kindergarten is 5.25, 6.25 for first grade, 7.25 for second grade and on in the same manner for each school year. The ECLS and the LSAY are compatible datasets. Both used item response theory methodology and NCES math standards arriving at identical sample distributions and difficulty levels. Though both studies were done at different times, the study periods are comparable. This is true because there are no practical differences in student achievement levels between the eras of each study on the National Assessment of Educational Progress (Perie, M., 2005). Adjustment for Differences in Relative Math Scales The adjustment for the differences in the relative scales for achievement on the LSAY and ECLS was made by the use of overlapping scores from the two datasets. The LSAY has a fall test score for eighth grade and ninth grades, while the ECLS has an eight grade spring test score that lies halfway between those two. To compare the scales for the ECLS and the LSAY, a mean was taken for the two LSAY scores and a ratio was found between the mean for the LSAY and the spring ECLS scores. This ratio was then used to adjust the LSAY to the ECLS scale. Statistical analyses of dataset and model parameters Regression coefficients for the two parameters, k and Cm, in equation (9) were estimated by minimizing residual sum of squares (least squares method). The Mathematics Achievement 15 independent variable is time and the dependent variable is math achievement with mental age latent. This was done for the eleven decile and quartile levels of the dependent variable by using the non-linear fitting procedure of the SPSS package. Eleven estimates for the parameters k and Cm resulted from these regressions. Statistical significance tests were then performed on the estimated parameters for hypothesis testing. Results of the Statistics on the Model Table 1 Summary of Non-linear Regression Analysis of Expolinear Function and Estimates of Parameters Performance C k R 10 10.953 0.234 0.979 20 11.843 0.257 0.984 25 12.287 0.267 0.986 30 12.495 0.272 0.987 40 13.073 0.285 0.988 50 13.603 0.295 0.988 60 14.137 0.306 0.990 70 14.728 0.317 0.991 Percentile Mathematics Achievement 16 Table 1 Summary of Non-linear Regression Analysis of Expolinear Function and Estimates of Parameters Performance C k R 75 15.052 0.323 0.991 80 15.409 0.329 0.991 90 16.367 0.344 0.992 Percentile The following tests were performed on the three hypotheses: 1. The achievement function (equation 9) was tested for eleven regressions. This function was estimated for all deciles and quartiles ranging from 90% to 10% (See figure 4). The F-statistics were significant, ranging successively from 1745 (90%) to 466 (10%) with p < 0.001. The R2 ranged successively from 0.992 (90%) to 0.979 (10%) with six regressions above 0.990. The results for the estimates of the two parameters, Cm and k, are given in Table Two. The standard error of the estimate for Cm ranged between 0.1526 and 0.09851 from the 10% level to the 90% level, respectively. All estimates of Cm were significant at p < 0.001. 2. The growth parameter, k, for the logistic function of mental age varied between 0.234 and 0.344. The standard error of the estimate varied between 0.0084 and 0.0073 from the 10% level to the 90% level, respectively. The estimates were found significant at p < 0.001. Mathematics Achievement 17 3. The logistic function’s growth parameter, k, was tested for homogeneity between decile and quartile levels using ANCOVA. The mean value for k, 0.294, was compared to the eleven estimated values for k. The covariates were the predicted achievement values. The null hypothesis was not rejected with F = 0.170, p = .680. 240 220 90% 200 Math Achievement Scores 180 50% 160 140 120 10% 100 80 60 40 20 0 0 2 4 6 8 10 12 14 16 Time Figure 3. Three Expolinear Curves, 90%, 50%, 10% Discussion The results are consistent with the mathematical model. Sample curves are shown 2 in figure 2. The achievement function has significant predictive capacity. The R statistics average about 0.988 for the eleven regressions (refer to Table 1). These are far higher than expected based upon past literature. The maximum rate of growth at maturity, Cm, was found to be stable and changes consistently with the level of achievement. It was found that the logistic function’s growth parameter, k, is stable and statistically significant. This is also consistent with psychometric studies of the relative stability of Mathematics Achievement 18 mental development. The results indicated that there may be minuscule variations in k. Furthermore, based on differences among achievement levels, the results from ANCOVA support Bloom’s theory that mental maturation is both law-like and uniform among students. There is statistical support, then, for a single logistic function operating for all students by achievement level. The dissonant finding that emerged in the study is the very high explanatory power of the achievement function. This is not consistent with past studies of the link between achievement and mental age. The variance explained was expected to be 50%. Instead, almost all of the variation in achievement is explained by the function. Since mature mental age, M cannot explain achievement by itself, the only possibility is a specification error somewhere with a hidden variable. Logically, the source of the unexpected explanatory power in the function rests in its components - the logistic function’s anti-derivative or Cm. There is nothing about the anti-derivative that would allow for the presence of a powerful underlying variable. It is strictly a function of time with two constraints. The likely source of the specification error, then, is in Cm. In equation (4), parameter a was hypothesized to directly link mature mental age, M with Cm. Parameter a may be confounding the existence of two things: an expected constant of proportionality and the activity of an underlying variable. If so, that underlying variable would have to be random and normally distributed like the other two variables. It would also have to be highly correlated with the dependent variable in equation (4) Cm. A likely candidate for that missing variable is the teacher input to achievement. To capture the input from teachers is most problematic. Teacher effect involves many variables and resists being represented by a single input number. To overcome this, some Mathematics Achievement 19 researchers have operationally defined it as a productive output. This single number is teacher effect. There are two suggestive studies on teacher effect. (Rivers, 1999) found in her large study of two school districts in Tennessee that teacher effect explains 49.6% of the variance in achievement. If true, then Rivers’ finding that 50% explained variation in achievement by teacher effect is of the right magnitude. It complements the demonstrated 50% explanatory power of mental age. Additionally, Hanushek (1992) found that teacher effect has a range from 0.5 grade level equivalents to 1.5 grade level equivalents and was a random variable with a normal distribution and mean at 1.0. These facts make teacher effect a good candidate for the missing variable in equation (4). It is also reasonable from a functional standpoint. If the unit of analysis is the classroom, the inputs to achievement come from the students and the teacher. It is not known, however, where the mean of teacher effect is located relative to the mean of math achievement. Aside from the existence of bias, both the standard deviation of teacher effect and its correlation with mental age are unknown. The implication of this study is that a functional relationship probably exists between mental age, teacher effect and achievement in mathematics for students. Further study should be conducted to test for these relationships. Mathematics Achievement 20 References Bayley, N. (1949). Consistency and variability in the growth of intelligence from birth to eighteen years. Journal of General Psychology, 75, 165-196. Bayley, N. (1955). On the growth of intelligence. The American Psychologist, 10, 805818. Bloom, B. S. (1964). Stability and change in human characteristics. New York, NY: Wiley. Brodnick, R. J., Ree, M.J. (1995). A structural model of academic performance, socioeconomic status, and spearman's g. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 55, 583-594. Choi, K. S., M. (2005). Modeling heterogeneity in relationships between initial status and rates of change. Los Angeles: CREEST/University of California, Los Angeles. Gedye, C. A. (1981). Longitudinal study (grades 1 through 10) of school achievement, self-confidence, and selected parental characteristics. Unpublished Dissertation, University of California, Berkeley, Berkeley, CA. Goudriaan, J. and Monteith, J.L. (1990). A mathematical function for crop growth based on light interception and leaf area expansion. Annals of Botany, 66, 695-701. Hanushek, E. A. (1992). The trade-off between child quantity and quality. Journal of Political Economy, 100(1), 107. Heinis, H. (1924). La loi du developpement mental. Archives de Psychologie, 74, 97-128. Jensen, A.R. (1980). Bias in mental testing. New York: Free Press. Mathematics Achievement 21 Jensen. (1991). Spearman's g and the problem of educational equality. Oxford Review of Education, 17, 169-187. Jensen, A.R. (1993). Psychometric g and achievement. In B.R. Gifford (Ed.), Policy perspectives on educational testing (pp. 117-227). Boston: Kluwer Academic Publishers. Jensen, A.R. (1998). The g factor: the science of mental ability. Westport, Connecticut: Praeger. Johnston, J. (1972). Econometric Methods. (2nd ed.). New York, N.Y.: McGraw-Hill. Lee, J.H., Goudriaan, J. and Challa, H. (2003). Using the expolinear growth equation for modelling crop growth in year-round cut chrysanthemum. Annals of Botany, 92, 697-708. McCaffrey, D. F., Lockwood, J.R., Koretz, D., Louis, T.A., Hamilton, L. (2004). Models for value-added modeling of teacher effects. Journal of Educational and Behavioral Statistics, 29(1), 67-101. Matarazzo, J.D. (1972). Wechsler's measurement and appraisal of adult intelligence (5th ed.). Baltimore: Williams & Wilkins. Miller, J. D., Kimmel, L., Hoffer, T.B. & Nelson, C. (2000). Longitudinal Study of American Youth: User's Manual. Evanston, Illinois: Northwestern University, International Center for the Advancement of Scientific Literacy. Moffit, T.E., Caspi, A., Harkness, A.R., and Silva, P. (1993). The natural history of change in intellectual performance. Journal of Child Psychology, 34(4), 455-506. Mathematics Achievement 22 Perie, M., Moran, R., & Lutkus, A.D. (2005). NAEP 2004 trends in academic progress three decades of student performance in reading and mathematics. Retrieved. from http://www.edpubs.org. Princiotta, D., Flanagan, K.D. & Germino Hausken, T. (2006). Fifth Grade: Findings from the fifth-grade follow-up of the early childhood longitudinal study. Retrieved. from http://www.edpubs.org. Rathbun, A. W., J. (2004). From kindergarten through third grade. Retrieved. from http://www.edpubs.org. Rivers, J. C. (1999). The impact of teacher effect on student math competency achievement. Unpublished Dissertation,University of Tennessee, Knoxville, TN. Scllitz, C., Jahoda, M., Deutsch, M., and Cook, S. (1966). Research methods in social relations. New York: Rinehart, and Winston. Slaughter, V., Kamppi, D., and Paynter, J. (2006). Toddler subtraction with large sets. Developmental Science, 9(1), 33-39. Snow, R.E. & Yalow, E. (1982). Education and intelligence. In R.J. Sternberg (Ed.), Handbook of human intelligence (pp. 493-585). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Thorndike, E.L. (1927). The measurement of intelligence. New York: Teachers College, Columbia University. Thurstone, L. (1928). The absolute zero in intelligence measurement. Psychological Review, 35, 175-197. Mathematics Achievement 23 Walston, J., Rathbun, A., and Germino Hausken, E. (2008). Eighth Grade: First Findings From the Final Round of the Early Childhood Longitudinal Study, Kindergarten Class of 1998-99. Retrieved. From http://www.edpubs.org. Weisstein, E.W. (2003). CRC concise encyclopedia mathematics. New York: Chapman and Hall/CRC. Wynn, K. (1992). Addition and subtraction by human infants. Nature, 358, 749-750. Wynn, K. (1995). Infants possess a system of numerical knowledge. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 4(6), 172-175. Yin, X., Goudriaan, J., Lantinga, E.A., Vos, J., and Spiertz, H.J. (2003). A flexible sigmoid function of determinate growth. Annals of Botany, 91, 361-371.