dickerson_neurol_2011_appendix

advertisement
Dickerson et al. Neurology
Appendix-e1
Supplemental Materials and Methods
Participants
Sample 1 (MGH)
Recruitment and inclusion/exclusion criteria
The participants in this study were drawn from a longitudinal study (total n=379) that
recruited subjects from the community with and without memory difficulty to examine
the preclinical predictors of AD 1. Volunteers who responded to the advertisements then
underwent a multistage screening procedure. To be included in the parent longitudinal
study, participants had to be primarily English speaking, 65 or older; non-demented; and
free of significant underlying medical, neurological, or psychiatric illness (based on
standard laboratory tests and a comprehensive evaluation described below). Individuals
with major vascular risk factors or disease (i.e., atrial fibrillation, insulin dependent
diabetes mellitus, cerebral infarcts, cardiac bypass graft surgery) at baseline were
excluded. All subjects and informants provided informed consent in accordance with the
Human Research Committee of the Massachusetts General Hospital, Boston, MA.
Comprehensive evaluation of cognitive status and diagnostic assessment
A semi-structured evaluation was administered annually to determine each participant’s
overall cognitive status 2, 3. It includes a set of questions regarding memory and cognitive
abilities in daily life and functional status in complex and basic activities of daily living,
asked of the subject and an informant, and a standardized neurologic, psychiatric, and
mental status evaluation of the subject. At baseline in the longitudinal study, as a result of
this evaluation, participants were classified as cognitively normal (CN) or
questionably/very mildly impaired. The same evaluation was performed annually, and at
each subsequent visit participants were classified similarly as CN, questionably/very
mildly impaired, or—for those who had progressed to dementia at subsequent visits—
demented.
A consensus clinical diagnosis was assigned to participants who developed
significant cognitive and functional impairment, incorporating clinical history, medical
records, laboratory evaluation, mental status evaluation results obtained during the office
evaluation, and neuroimaging studies 1, 4. Individuals with dementia were classified as
AD or another diagnosis (e.g., frontotemporal dementia, vascular dementia) according to
standard criteria 4-6.
Neuropsychological measures
At baseline, a neuropsychological battery was administered to all subjects, as previously
described 1. The neuropsychological test battery was administered in a separate session
from the cognitive evaluation described above, and was scored in a fashion blind to other
information about the subject.
For the purposes of the present study, we computed a composite episodic memory
score based on three measures, including the California Verbal Learning Test (CVLT)7
Total Learning and Delayed Free Recall measures, as well as the Free and Cued Selective
Reminding Test8 Delayed Free Recall measure. First we calculated standardized scores
for each of these tests adjusted for age, gender, and education as follows. Using baseline
data from the sample as a whole, we performed a linear regression for each test score
using age, gender, and educational attainment as predictors, and saved the residuals from
this regression. For the subjects who were CN at baseline, we computed the mean and
standard deviation of these residuals. Standardized scores were then calculated for all
subjects by subtracting the mean of the CN subjects, and dividing by the standard
deviation in these CN subjects 9. For the present article, the standardized scores for the
three memory tests were averaged into a single Episodic Memory Z score. Thus, a
standardized score of -1.0 indicates that the subject was 1 standard deviation below
expected mean for a CN subject of the same age, gender, and level of educational
attainment.
Subject selection and outcome measures in the present analysis
For the present analyses, we included all subjects with a baseline cognitive evaluation
that indicated CN status and at least four annual follow-up visits, for a total of at least 5
years of follow-up (n=73). Based on the hypotheses of the present analysis, we further
restricted the sample to those individuals who remained CN at most recent follow-up
(CN-Stable) or those who were diagnosed with probable AD dementia (CN-AD
Converter), excluding individuals who were diagnosed with MCI or non-AD dementia.
This resulted in a sample size of 38, including 28 CN-Stable and 10 NC-AD Converters.
Of these, 3 CN and 2 AD participants had MRI scans that were not useable for the
present analyses.
Sample 2 (Rush)
Recruitment and inclusion/exclusion criteria
All participants were recruited for a longitudinal imaging project 10 from a) the
community by the Rush Alzheimer’s Disease Center (RADC; Chicago, IL); b) the
Religious Order Study (ROS), a longitudinal, clinico-pathologic investigation of aging
and AD in older nuns, priests and brothers 11-13; or c) the Rush Memory and Aging
Project (MAP), a separate longitudinal clinicopathologic investigation of aging and AD
14
. Subjects were excluded if there was evidence of other neurologic, psychiatric or
systemic conditions. It is important to note that all individuals included in the present
study who were classified as CN at baseline evaluation were not recruited from clinical
populations in which some individuals came in for a work-up but tested within normal
limits. Informed consent was obtained from all participants according to the rules of the
Human Investigation Committee of Rush University Medical Center.
Comprehensive evaluation of cognitive status and diagnostic assessment
All evaluations were carried out by the Rush Alzheimer’s Disease Center (RADC,
Chicago, IL) as previously described 10, 15. Briefly, the evaluation, which was given to all
participants in the study, incorporated the Consortium to Establish a Registry for
Alzheimer’s Disease (CERAD, 16) procedures and included a medical history,
neurological examination, neuropsychological testing, informant interview and blood
tests. All participants in the present study were classified as CN at baseline. This
diagnostic classification required the absence of neurologic, psychiatric or systemic
conditions that could cause cognitive impairment (e.g., stroke, alcoholism, major
depression, a history of temporal lobe epilepsy), a normal neurological examination,
normal cognition as determined by performance on neuropsychological tests, and an
MMSE 17 score ≥27. Longitudinal followup evaluations of each individual were
conducted annually, as part of the parent studies. For individuals determined to have
dementia, a consensus diagnosis was determined. Individuals with dementia were
classified as AD or another diagnosis (e.g., frontotemporal dementia, vascular dementia)
according to standard criteria 4-6. CDR ratings were not performed until relatively
recently in the Rush study, and so are not available for the present analysis.
Neuropsychological measures
The episodic memory tests administered to all participants and used to define a memory
deficit consisted of immediate and delayed recall of the East Boston Story 18 and of Story
A from the Logical Memory of the Wechsler memory scale–Revised 19. An additional
test involved the learning and retention of a 10-word list from the CERAD battery 16. The
three scores for this test included Word List Memory (the total number of words
immediately recalled after each of three consecutive presentations of the list), Word List
Recall (the number of words recalled after a delay), and Word List Recognition (the
number of words correctly recognized in a four-alternative, forced-choice format,
administered after Word List Recall). Summary scores were calculated for combined
performance on these memory tests by standardizing each of the seven episodic memory
scores 20. For this purpose, we used the mean and standard deviations of each test from
the baseline visits of the first wave of 86 participants entered into an ongoing longitudinal
project 10 and averaged the standardized values to obtain an Episodic Memory Z score.
Subject selection and outcome measures in the present analysis
For the present analyses, we included all subjects with a baseline cognitive evaluation
that indicated CN status and at least four annual follow-up visits (with the exception of
one participant who was followed for 3 years). Based on the hypotheses of the present
analysis, we further restricted the sample to those individuals who remained CN at most
recent follow-up (CN-Stable) or those who were diagnosed with probable AD dementia
(CN-AD Converter). This resulted in a sample size of 33, including 25 CN-Stable and 7
CN-AD Converters. All of these individuals had MRI scans that were useable for the
present analyses.
MRI Data Acquisition and Analysis
MRI Data Acquisition Parameters
For the MGH sample, the parameters were: repetition time/echo time (TR/TE) = 35
msec/5 msec; field of view = 22 cm; flip angle = 45°; number of excitations = 1; slice
thickness = 1.5 mm, 124 slices; matrix size = 256 x 256). For the Rush sample, the
parameters were: TR/TE = 33.3 msec/7 msec; field of view = 22 cm; flip angle = 35°;
number of excitations = 1; slice thickness = 1.6 mm, 124 slices; matrix size = 256 x 192).
MRI morphometric data analysis – Automated surface reconstruction and alignment of
participants
The MRI morphometric data analysis methods have been previously described in detail
21-26
. The FreeSurfer software used to perform the analyses and visualization employed in
this study, along with complete documentation, is freely available via the internet at
http://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu). The raw MRI volume for each participant was used
to segment cerebral white matter 25 and multiple subcortical grey matter and ventricular
regions 27, and to estimate the location of the gray/white boundary. Topological defects in
the gray/white boundary were corrected 28, and this gray/white boundary was used as the
starting point for a deformable surface algorithm designed to find the pial surface with
submillimeter precision 26. Cortical thickness measurements were obtained by calculating
the distance between those surfaces at each of approximately 160,000 points (per
hemisphere) across the cortical mantle (Fischl and Dale 2000). Mean thickness of each
subject’s entire cerebral cortex was then calculated. The methods for generation of
cortical surfaces and resultant thickness measurements have been shown to be reliable in
a test-retest study of a group of older participants scanned twice on the same scanner, as
well as across scanner manufacturers and 1.5T and 3.0T field strengths 29, 30, and in a
study of different sequence parameters 31. The accuracy of the thickness measures
derived from this technique has been previously validated by direct comparisons with
manual measures on postmortem brain 23 and on MRI data 21.
The surface representing the gray-white border was ‘‘inflated,” differences among
individuals in the depth of gyri and sulci were normalized as previously described 24, 25, 32,
and each subject’s reconstructed brain was then morphed and registered to an average
spherical surface representation that optimally aligns sulcal and gyral features across
participants. Thickness measures were then mapped to the inflated surface of each
participant’s reconstructed brain (Fischl, Sereno and Dale 1999). This procedure allows
visualization of data across the entire cortical surface (i.e., both the gyri and sulci)
without interference from cortical folding. The data were smoothed on the surface using
an iterative nearest-neighbor averaging procedure. One hundred iterations were applied,
which is equivalent to applying a 2-dimensional Gaussian smoothing kernel along the
cortical surface with a full-width/half-maximum of 18.4 mm. Data were then resampled
into a common spherical coordinate system (Fischl, Sereno, and others 1999). The
procedure provides accurate matching of morphologically homologous cortical locations
among participants on the basis of each individual’s anatomy, while minimizing
geometric distortion, resulting in a mean measure of cortical thickness for each group at
each point on the reconstructed surface.
MRI morphometric data analysis – Generation of cortical ROIs and quantification of
magnitude of thinning
The primary analytic approach employed in this study made use of “AD-signature”
regions of interest (ROIs) generated from a previous study and applied in an a priori
fashion to the subjects in the present analysis. In the previous study in which these ROIs
were identified, cortical thickness in patients with mild AD dementia was compared to
that of CN and regions of significant thinning were identified 33. The analysis generated
nine ROIs that were demonstrated to be consistently affected across four separate
samples of patients with mild AD 33, and which were subsequently demonstrated to be
useful in predicting dementia in patients with MCI 34. As illustrated in the Figure e2, the
MTL cortex ROI was localized in the rostral MTL spanning the crown of the
parahippocampal gyrus and extending into the fundus of the collateral sulcus, in a region
typically considered to include both entorhinal and perirhinal cortex. In addition to these
9 AD-signature ROIs, an ROI from the primary visual cortex (PVC) was used as in the
previous studies as a control region, hypothesized not to be affected in AD relative to
NC. The map in Supplemental Figure e2 shows the ROIs.
Using the spherical registration of each subject to the template, the ROIs were
mapped from the template back to each individual participant in the present analysis. For
each subject, mean cortical thickness within each ROI was calculated by deriving an
average of all of the thickness estimates at vertices that fell within the labeled ROI. For
each subject, the resultant ROI measures of cortical thickness were averaged across the
two hemispheres, and these values were used for further statistical analysis. In addition,
for each subject, a single summary measure was derived from the average thickness
across all 9 AD-related ROIs, the “AD-signature” ROI summary measure.
Results
In the MGH sample, CDR Sum-of-Boxes scores for the NC-AD Converter group at the
time of diagnosis of dementia were 6.9 (S.D. = 2.4) with the overall CDR ratings being
1.2 (S.D. = 0.5). CDR scores were not available for the Rush sample.
References
1.
Albert MS, Moss MB, Tanzi R, Jones K. Preclinical prediction of AD using
neuropsychological tests. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2001;7:631-639.
2.
Hughes CP, Berg L, Danziger WL, Coben LA, Martin RL. A new clinical scale
for the staging of dementia. Br J Psychiatry 1982;140:566-572.
3.
Morris JC. The Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR): current version and scoring
rules. Neurology 1993;43:2412-2414.
4.
McKhann G, Drachman D, Folstein M, Katzman R, Price D, Stadlam M. Clinical
diagnosis of Alzheimer's disease: Report of the NINCDS-ADRDA Work Group under
the auspices of Department of Health and Human Services Task Force on
Alzheimer's disease. Neurology 1984;34:939-944.
5.
Roman GC, Tatemichi TK, Erkinjuntti T, et al. Vascular dementia: diagnostic
criteria for research studies. Report of the NINDS-AIREN International Workshop.
Neurology 1993;43:250-260.
6.
Neary D, Snowden JS, Gustafson L, et al. Frontotemporal lobar degeneration:
a consensus on clinical diagnostic criteria. Neurology 1998;51:1546-1554.
7.
Delis DC, Kramer JH, Kaplan E, Ober BA. California verbal learning test,
research edition, manual. San Antonio: The Psychological Corporation, Harcourt
Brace Jovanovich, 1987.
8.
Grober E, Buschke H. Genuine memory deficits in dementia. Developmental
Neuropsychology 1987;3:13–36.
9.
Rothman KJ, Greenland S. Modern Epidemiology, 2nd ed. Philadelphia:
Lippincott, Williams, and Wilkins, 1998.
10.
deToledo-Morrell L, Stoub TR, Bulgakova M, et al. MRI-derived entorhinal
volume is a good predictor of conversion from MCI to AD. Neurobiol Aging
2004;25:1197-1203.
11.
Bennett DA, Wilson RS, Schneider JA, et al. Natural history of mild cognitive
impairment in older persons. Neurology 2002;59:198-205.
12.
Kordower JH, Chu Y, Stebbins GT, et al. Loss and atrophy of layer II entorhinal
cortex neurons in elderly people with mild cognitive impairment. Ann Neurol
2001;49:202-213.
13.
Mufson EJ, Chen EY, Cochran EJ, Beckett LA, Bennett DA, Kordower JH.
Entorhinal cortex beta-amyloid load in individuals with mild cognitive impairment.
Exp Neurol 1999;158:469-490.
14.
Bennett DA, Schneider JA, Buchman AS, Mendes de Leon C, Bienias JL, Wilson
RS. The Rush Memory and Aging Project: study design and baseline characteristics
of the study cohort. Neuroepidemiology 2005;25:163-175.
15.
Stoub TR, Bulgakova M, Leurgans S, et al. MRI predictors of risk of incident
Alzheimer disease: a longitudinal study. Neurology 2005;64:1520-1524.
16.
Morris JC, Heyman A, Mohs RC, et al. The Consortium to Establish a Registry
for Alzheimer's Disease (CERAD). Part I. Clinical and neuropsychological assessment
of Alzheimer's disease. Neurology 1989;39:1159-1165.
17.
Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR. "Mini-mental state". A practical method
for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res
1975;12:189-198.
18.
Albert M, Smith LA, Scherr PA, Taylor JO, Evans DA, Funkenstein HH. Use of
brief cognitive tests to identify individuals in the community with clinically
diagnosed Alzheimer's disease. Int J Neurosci 1991;57:167-178.
19.
Wechsler D. WMS-R Wechsler Memory Scale - Revisd Manual New York: The
Psychological Corporation, Harcourt Brace Jovanovich, Inc., 1987.
20.
Stoub TR, Rogalski EJ, Leurgans S, Bennett DA, deToledo-Morrell L. Rate of
entorhinal and hippocampal atrophy in incipient and mild AD: Relation to memory
function. Neurobiol Aging;31:1089-1098.
21.
Kuperberg GR, Broome MR, McGuire PK, et al. Regionally localized thinning
of the cerebral cortex in schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2003;60:878-888.
22.
Salat DH, Buckner RL, Snyder AZ, et al. Thinning of the cerebral cortex in
aging. Cereb Cortex 2004;14:721-730.
23.
Rosas HD, Liu AK, Hersch S, et al. Regional and progressive thinning of the
cortical ribbon in Huntington's disease. Neurology 2002;58:695-701.
24.
Fischl B, Sereno MI, Dale AM. Cortical surface-based analysis. II: Inflation,
flattening, and a surface-based coordinate system. Neuroimage 1999;9:195-207.
25.
Dale AM, Fischl B, Sereno MI. Cortical surface-based analysis. I. Segmentation
and surface reconstruction. Neuroimage 1999;9:179-194.
26.
Fischl B, Dale AM. Measuring the thickness of the human cerebral cortex from
magnetic resonance images. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the
United States of America 2000;97:11050-11055.
27.
Fischl B, Salat DH, Busa E, et al. Whole brain segmentation: automated
labeling of neuroanatomical structures in the human brain. Neuron 2002;33:341355.
28.
Fischl B, Liu A, Dale AM. Automated manifold surgery: constructing
geometrically accurate and topologically correct models of the human cerebral
cortex. IEEE Trans Med Imaging 2001;20:70-80.
29.
Han X, Jovicich J, Salat D, et al. Reliability of MRI-derived measurements of
human cerebral cortical thickness: The effects of field strength, scanner upgrade and
manufacturer. Neuroimage 2006;32:180-194.
30.
Dickerson BC, Fenstermacher E, Salat DH, et al. Detection of cortical thickness
correlates of cognitive performance: Reliability across MRI scan sessions, scanners,
and field strengths. Neuroimage 2008;39:10-18.
31.
Wonderlick JS, Ziegler DA, Hosseini-Varnamkhasti P, et al. Reliability of MRIderived cortical and subcortical morphometric measures: effects of pulse sequence,
voxel geometry, and parallel imaging. Neuroimage 2009;44:1324-1333.
32.
Fischl B, Sereno MI, Tootell RB, Dale AM. High-resolution intersubject
averaging and a coordinate system for the cortical surface. Hum Brain Mapp
1999;8:272-284.
33.
Dickerson BC, Bakkour A, Salat DH, et al. The cortical signature of
Alzheimer's disease: regionally specific cortical thinning relates to symptom
severity in very mild to mild AD dementia and is detectable in asymptomatic
amyloid-positive individuals. Cereb Cortex 2009;19:497-510.
34.
Bakkour A, Morris JC, Dickerson BC. The cortical signature of prodromal AD:
regional thinning predicts mild AD dementia. Neurology 2009;72:1048-1055.
Download