1 HOW MANAGEMENT PRACTICES INFLUENCE ORGANIZATIONAL CULTURE AND BEHAVIORS. A MULTILEVEL ANALYSIS OF CULTURAL PROPERTIES AND THEIR EFFECTS Bruno Bolognini Università di Genova Dipartimento di Scienze politiche e sociali Largo Zecca 8/16 16121 Genova ITALY Tel: 0039102099015 Fax: 0039102099027 e-mail: bologni@unige.it CeSMO Working paper 2007-05-03 This research has benefited from financing by Università di Genova, ex 60% 2 HOW MANAGEMENT PRACTICES INFLUENCE ORGANIZATIONAL CULTURE AND BEHAVIORS. A MULTILEVEL ANALYSIS OF CULTURAL PROPERTIES AND THEIR EFFECTS Abstract Practices of organizational governance influence results and performance, but it is not always clear how they do so. The study analyses practices through the perceptions of organizational members in order to verify hypotheses concerning the connection between these perceptions and collective behaviors. On the basis of data collected in 61 organizations in diverse sectors by means of questionnaires administered to circa 4000 employees, the multilevel analysis reveals significant connections at both the individual and collective level. According to Adam Smith, the wealth of nations results from equilibrium in economic exchanges. Also organizations produce wealth, doing so by establishing equilibrium among their members. The invisible hand is the metaphor for the regulation of economic equilibria, but the relations among organizational members respond to other criteria (Simon, 1957; Williamson, 1975; Ouchi, 1980). These relations and their effects are a recurrent theme in the literature. The economy of incentives (Barnard, 1938), the exchange between contributions and incentives (Simon, 1957), and the power relations model (Crozier, 1963) are some of the explanations put forward for these complex mechanisms. Interpreting work relations according to economic exchange (Arrow, 1985) or alternatively social exchange (Blau, 1964), or the psychological contract (Rousseau, 1995), has substantial practical significance because it suggests managerial solutions that help establish organizational 3 equilibria, influencing behaviors and the results that ensue from them. If people are managed with attention paid to social exchanges, and not solely to economic exchanges, the results seem to be better: Tsui, Pearce, Porter and Tripoli (1997) have shown that social exchange based on mutual investment is more effective than purely economic exchange based on quasi-spot contracts. The impact of human resources management and of high performance practices on organizational results has been examined by numerous studies (Arthur, 1994; Huselid, 1995; Macduffie, 1995; Becker & Gerhart, 1996), but there is no consensus on the effect of these practices or on how they influence results (Cappelli & Neumark, 2001; Preuss, 2003; Gittell, Von Nordenflycht, and Kochan, 2004). The same practices do not always produce the same effects. In the majority of cases, analyses have dealt with the relation between management practices and results; less attention has been paid to the intervening variables in the intermediate dimension providing entry to the black box that helps explain the linkage between human resources systems and firm performance (Becker & Gerhart, 1996). One difficulty in developing tools with which to describe organizational relations arises from the ‘competencies trap’. Organization psychologists concentrate on micro phenomena and the features of individuals, while sociologists tend to focus on macro phenomena. The consequent misunderstandings have been stressed (Granovetter, 1985), and so too has the need to develop a meso level of analysis (Cappelli & Sherer, 1991), or a meso paradigm (House, Rousseau, & Thomas-Hunt, 1995). On other hand, there is a need for multilevel studies which shed light on organizational phenomena which manifest themselves at several levels (Rousseau, 1985; Kozlowski & Klein, 2000). 4 The aim of this study is to analyse management practices from the point of view of employees’ perceptions of them, the purpose being to show their implications at both the individual and the collective levels. The idea is that the set of these perceptions within an organization gives rise to diverse equilibria, and that the latter have significant effects on collective behaviors and therefore on organizational results. The study has two distinctive characteristics. The first is that it introduces the concept of ‘code’ as a means with which to describe organizations in terms of the equilibria generated by managerial practices, with the consequent possibility of identifying these equilibria as distinctive cultural properties of organizations. The second innovative feature of the study is that it focuses on the effects of different contextual settings on organizational behavior; effects which have significant implications for governance practices. ORGANIZATIONAL PROPERTIES The idea that the functioning of organizations can be explained in terms of certain properties which distinguish among them has prompted discussion on the difference between obtrusive and unobtrusive properties (Webb & Weick, 1979), on the organizational climate and its dimensions (Sims & LaFollette, 1975; Reichers & Schneider, 1990; James, James, & Ashe, 1990), on an organization’s being or having a culture (Smircich, 1983). The organizational properties of interest to me here concern the relations among organizational members, and the institutionalization of those relations in the form of behavior-regulating cultural codes (Louis, 1980). 5 The action of people in organizations produces relations whose complexity differs according to the activities performed and the technologies used, and according to environmental conditions. Behaviors must comply with rules, for which reason the institutional nature of organizations has been frequently stressed (Selznick, 1957; March & Olsen, 1989; Scott, 1995). According to March and Olsen (1989), rules and meanings are the essential terms for understanding behavior in organizations. Crawford and Ostrom (1995:582) define institutions as “enduring regularities of human action in situations structured by rules, norms and shared strategies”. For North (1990), the function of institutions is to reduce uncertainty and to create regularity. The normative character of institutional ties is therefore widely recognized. The problem that I shall address here is whether it is possible to describe these ties empirically and to measure their effect on behaviors. However, I shall analyse only certain forms of relation, my purpose being to verify whether, and to what extent, they help establish rules and behaviorregulating criteria. Organizational Codes The concept of ‘code’ relates to a structure which produces linguistic expressions, cultural meanings, or regularities of behavior. In this sense one talks of codes of conduct. The notion can also be used to define institutional mechanisms which stabilize patterns of behavior (Douglas, 1986). Various disciplines employ the concept to construct their theories. It has been used in organization studies in relation to organizational communication (Louis, 1980; Broms & Gamberg, 1983; Manning; 1979; Donellon, Gray, & Bougon, 1986). Gagliardi (1986) describes 6 the cultural code of an organization as a system of rules which govern the attribution of meaning, March and Olsen (1989) consider codes to be variants of rules. The notion of ‘code’ can be considered from another point of view. Cognitive psychology has developed the idea that the human mind functions like a computer which processes information by means of scripts (Schank & Abelson, 1977). The term ‘schema’ or ‘script’, and in particular of higher-order schemata, has been proposed in order to denote abstract mental structures which assign meanings to environmental features (James, James, & Ashe, 1990). They act as evaluation or support criteria for value judgements constructed at individual level. For James, James, and Ashe (1990), measuring the psychological climate is equivalent to measuring these schemata. A code can be related to the notion of schema in the sense that it is a criterion for judgement. Codes are criteria which guide decisions and actions, but because they regulate relations among several subjects, they have a social connotation. Sensemaking is a complex phenomenon which occurs at both the individual and social level (Weick, 1995). Examples of sensemaking processes are those by which individuals define their identities by attributing a meaning to their selves in relation to others (Dutton & Dukerick, 1991). Organizational codes are consolidated through sensemaking processes which transform perceptions of interpersonal relations into expectations on the basis of reciprocities which tend to become stabilized. As Weick (1995) pointed out, sensemaking processes are rooted, inter alia, in the formation of expectations which may be confirmed or disproved but which in general tend to consolidate until they influence perceptions and alter reality itself. Also Luhmann (1985) gave great importance to the mechanisms which consolidate reciprocal expectations as explaining how behavior-regulating rules are formed. 7 My hypotheses are based on the idea that the action of organizational governance produces perceptions among employees in regard to their relations with the organization and those who run it. These perceptions tend to change into expectations which, as they consolidate, generate codes of behavior by interconnecting perceptions, expectations and codes, these being understood as properties of organizational collectives. March and Olsen (1989) cite sensemaking processes to explain, in terms of cognitive congruence mechanisms, how meanings are defined and consolidated in institutions via expectations. Organizational codes have also been related to organizational principles, which Zander and Kugat (1995) define as ways in which relations among individuals and groups are structured and coordinated (McEvery, Perrone, & Zaheer, 2003). In empirical terms, organizational codes can be defined as emergent relational properties of a cultural nature and describable as latent non-observable constructs whose effects can be analysed. Emergent properties are collective phenomena: they originate in individuals but they are manifest at a higher level. Kozlowski and Klein (2000) relate these emergent phenomena to formal and informal factors which act as concurrent forces. Some Fundamental Organizational Relations The relations best suited to defining social exchange in organization have been studied for some time. Two main perspectives have been adopted in their analysis: the point of view of employers, and that of employees. Contrary to the approach used by Tsui et al. (1997), here I shall assume the employees’ point of view to study their perceptions of organizational practices 8 and the expectations that derive from those perceptions. It will thus be possible to analyse how these perceptions and these expectations contribute to the consolidation of codes of behavior in organizations. I shall concentrate on three forms of relation in particular: empowerment, identity, and trust. Empowerment. When employees perceive that the organization to which they belong sets value on them, the bases are laid for empowerment relations. Empowerment is principally an interpersonal relation connected with the conferring of power to those of subordinate positions: in this sense it is connected to the traditional idea of delegation. The concept of empowerment has attracted a great deal of attention. In the majority of cases, study has been made of its psychological features by measuring subjective dimensions such as meaning, competence, selfdetermination, or impact (Spreitzer, 1995; Conger & Kanungo, 1988; Thomas & Velthouse, 1990). In a recent study, Seibert, Silver and Randolph (2004) distinguish between psychological empowerment and empowerment climate, by which they mean in the former case the motivation to work, and in the latter shared perceptions on the extent to which an organization uses policies and practices to empower its employees. The dimensions in this case are information sharing, autonomy through boundaries, team responsibility, and accountability. Empowerment is therefore viewed as an attribute of both individuals and organizations. Moreover, the concept is associated with a wide variety of meanings, although all of them link with the idea of valorising employees. I adopt this general idea by proposing a measure of empowerment which concerns the extent to which employees feel that their organization sets value on them by implementing certain management practices. 9 The empowerment perceived in organizations stems from explicit managerial choices, from the way in which managerial practices are implemented. But there are other relations, like identity and trust, the perception of which is less directly relatable to explicit practices but which is just as much tied to governance and to the way in which this develops through communication, leadership styles, and the decisions that mark organizational activity. Identity. The multiple identities of individuals, and of organizations, are always constructed through relations (Albert & Whetten, 1986; Pratt & Foreman, 2000). Burke & Reitzes (1991) associate the identity of individuals with commitment and therefore view identity as mediating the relation between individual and organization. However, to explain organizational identity one must go beyond this individual level of analysis and focus on the relations among the various actors who help determine organizational action. Scott and Lane (2000) explain the formation of identity as action by top management to construct the organization’s image by means of a mechanism based on the reciprocity of relationships. From this perspective, attention focuses on the relationship between the management and employees, examining the way in which the organization is able to confer identity on its employees and analysing it in terms of the perceptions of the employees themselves. All individuals define their identities by associating a fundamental oppositional system consisting of “Ego versus non-Ego” with the elements of another oppositional system variously recognized in social groupings or diverse fields of experience. Identity is a property of organizations which thus assume institutional significance. As ‘legitimated social groupings’, institutions codify expectations and enable uncertainties to be curbed by defining a shared 10 identity (Douglas, 1986). It is therefore important to describe organizations as institutions because of their ability to confer identity. Trust. Trust can be considered an asymmetrical relation between two or more subjects who perform different roles as trusters and trustees. Given its relational character, trust can be defined in many different ways (Bigley & Pearce, 1998): as willingness to accept that one is vulnerable (Mayer, Davis, & Schoorman, 1995), as a psychological state (Rousseau, Sitkin, Burt, & Camerer, 1998; Dirks & Ferrin, 2001), as expectation and intention or volition (McEvery, Perrone, & Zaheer, 2003; Luhmann, 1979); or as an attitude characterized by risk or hazard (Luhmann, 1979; Coleman, 1990). Empirical as well as theoretical analysis of trust relations has concentrated on interpersonal relationships (Colemann, 1990; Granovetter, 1985). Luhmann (1979), however, distinguished between familiarity-based trust and system trust. In fact, Luhmann evidenced a sort of continuity between these two forms of trust, which he considered concepts able to explain different levels of complexity. On the other hand, the different ways of considering trust depend on the disciplinary approaches taken by researchers. Whatever the case may be, the distinction between these two types of trust may correspond empirically to different levels of analysis. Hence if one looks at individual perceptions and how people manifest their trust expectations towards others, one may speak of interpersonal trust, whereas if one looks at the aggregate perceptions of several organizational members towards their organization, then one can speak of system trust. In what follows, I shall study organizational properties. I shall therefore consider trust also from the structural and cultural point of view, bearing in mind its function as a governance mechanism (Bradach & Eccles, 1989; Williamson, 1993) and therefore as an organizing 11 principle which enables solution of problems of interdependence and uncertainty (McEvery, Perrone, & Zaheer, 2003). Empowerment, identity and trust define a number of important social relations which arise among members of organizations and produce different equilibrium systems. Analysing them according to the manner in which they are perceived may shed light on organizational governance actions and on their effects. Organizational Performance The question of performance in organizations involves a certain ambiguity as to its definition and measurement: indeed, some authors have talked of the paradox of performance (Meyer & Gupta, 1994; Rousseau, 1997). Moreover, one need only consider the literature on high performance practices to see how many measures of performance have been proposed (Becker & Geherart, 1996). In order to analyse the effect of organizational codes, I use the term ‘performance’ in a particular sense. Weick and Roberts (1993) put forward the concept of a collective mind as a model for appropriate organizational action when high reliability is necessary, as in the case of aircraft carriers. What matters is the quality or style of the performance. In fact, performance may not only be high or low, productive or unproductive, satisfactory or unsatisfactory, but also heedful or heedless. Attentive performance is achieved when actions involving reciprocal adaptations are modelled so that they form a ‘collective mind’. Rather than the reification of a 12 social entity, according to Weick and Roberts this is the representation of a system produced by interrelated individual actions. The concept of performance associated with that of collective mind mainly applies to organizations which pursue reliability rather than efficiency. However, Weick and Roberts (1993) point out that high-performance organizations need to integrate their processes in order to obtain high levels of quality in much the same way as high-reliability organizations are required to do so. In effect, although in these cases there is much less risk of catastrophic events in the short term, the possibility of not surviving in highly competitive situations should not be neglected. The dependent variable used in this study is related to the concept of heedful performance in order to define the collective behavior prevalent in an organization and to determine its connection with the forms of relation described above. I shall call this variable ‘collective involvement’. Summary and hypotheses Organizational governance practices are decisive for results because they influence people’s behavior. But in their turn they are influenced by how people perceive their relations with others and with the organization as a whole. In fact, it is on the basis of these perceptions that expectations of behavior and patterns of action are formed. The perceptions of practices are consequently an important object of analysis because they enable definition of practices that have consolidated in the form of meanings and rules of behavior. The hypotheses of this study 13 concern the way in which these regularities come about and the likelihood that they will be transformed into behavior-regulating codes. When endeavouring to measure organizational culture, Hofstede, Neuijen, Ohayv, and Sanders (1990) found that the dimensions termed ‘organizational practices’ had a good capacity to differentiate among organizations, while those termed ‘values’ did not discriminate among them equally well. Subsequent studies by Hofstede (1998) have confirmed this finding: ‘practices’ are cultural properties of organizations, while ‘values’ and ‘attitudes’ are individual properties. Bearing these considerations in mind, I shall seek to verify whether the way in which people perceive organizational relations is a purely subjective phenomenon or whether it is influenced by significant differences among organizations. Hypothesis 1. Measuring individual perceptions of organizational relations – empowerment, identity, and trust – aggregated at organizational level enables organizations to be distinguished in a statistically significant manner. The second hypothesis is intended to test, at the individual level, the effect of how intraorganizational relations are perceived on how collective behavior is perceived. Hypothesis 2. There is a positive correlation between perceptions of empowerment, identity and trust measured at the individual level and perceptions, again at the individual level, of collective involvement: those who more intensely perceive the presence of these relations also perceive a greater collective involvement of the organization’s members. 14 The third hypothesis concerns the transformation of perceptions of relations into emergent organizational properties. The idea is that the way in which the relations among people, and between these people and the organization, consolidated by everyday routine, give rise to particular emergent properties. Hypothesis 3. Individual perceptions of organizational relations manifest themselves as emergent properties and, when measured at the organizational level, display significant correlations with individual perceptions of collective involvement. These three hypotheses are illustrated by Figure 1. -------------------------------Insert Figure 1 about here -------------------------------METHODS The sample Subject to analysis were 71 Italian organizations for a total of 4568 persons. Due to the homogeneity of data collected I use in the study responses from 61 organizations and 4077 persons. The organizations in the sample were operating in diverse industrial sectors which can be classified into four categories: 27 private manufacturing firms, 14 private service firms, 10 public administration organizations, and 10 social cooperatives. This differentiation takes account of Glick’s (1985) recommendation that a diversified sample of organizations should be used in order to obtain sufficient variance. The sample of interviewees was on average, for the organizations in the four categories, respectively, 77, 57, 98, and 22. 15 Collecting the Data A questionnaire, created ad hoc, was administered to a representative sample of employees belonging to all organizational areas whose replies testified to how current practices are perceived. They were asked to indicate the frequency with which these practices were apparent: for example, “pay policies in my organization encourage individual initiative”. The reply options consisted of a scale from 0 to 10, with 0 = never and 10 = always. This method tended to blur the distinction between ‘respondents’ and ‘informants’ (Seidler, 1974; Rousseau, 1990). Replies to the questionnaire depicted situations which characterized the everyday experience of the respondents, and they concerned subjective perceptions and interpretations of the facts reported. The aim was to measure the way in which people perceive organizational relations and, consequently, their reciprocal expectations. The focus of the items was descriptive rather than evaluative. Moreover, they referred to the organization as a whole. Measures The hypotheses required one dependent variable and three independent variables. Collective involvement. The dependent variable measured the prevalent forms of behavior in regard to the degree of collective involvement that the respondents perceived in their organizations. Concepts like that of ‘commitment’ (Mowday, Steers, & Porter 1979; Mowday & Sutton, 1993; Mowday, 1998) or ‘organizational citizenship behavior’ (Organ 1988) measure 16 attitudes, psychological states, or attributes of individual behavior connected with dispositional factors and motivations (Organ, 1990). My intention was instead to measure collective behavior. The testimonies of the respondents furnished a representation of collective behavior. Each respondent belonged to the collective examined, but s/he was simultaneously an observer: s/he observed everyday behaviors and evaluated the degree of collective involvement. Used to measure this dimension was a single item1 which asked the respondent how frequently the organization’s personnel showed maximum commitment. The three independent variables were measured by means of scales constructed ad hoc. The measures used were derived from a preliminary study by means of exploratory factor analysis. In order to adopt more rigorous validation criteria, then performed were a number of tests using confirmatory factor analysis. These procedures reduced the initial number of items: some were eliminated because the analysis revealed their ambiguity in that they were correlated with more than one dimension; others because they were used on a relatively small number of organizations compared to the standards required for this type of analysis (Anderson & Gerbig, 1984; Bollen, 1989; Hu & Bentler, 1995). A total of nine items, three for each dimension, were considered for the measures. The confirmatory factor analysis, for a total of 61 organizations, yielded the following overall fit: chi squared = 24.22 with pr > 0.34 (22 df); GFI = 0.92; RMSEA = 0.04; CFI = 0.99; NFI = 0.96. The r-squared values for the individual indicators 1 This proxy links with the concept of collective mind by virtue of its connection with another proxy which reports the respondents’ perception of the degree of carefulness with which the members of his/her organization worked (Pearson correlation coefficient 0.94 for 36 cases). I did not use this latter proxy in my model because of the relatively small number of cases in which it was used. 17 ranged from a minimum of 0.57 to a maximum of 0.91. Thus defined were the following measures. Empowerment. The empowerment that the organization conferred on its employees was measured with three items regarding pay policies, the organization’s concern with selection, the organization’s concern with training. The alpha coefficient for the three items was 0.85. Identity. Three items were used to ascertain the extent to which the organization was able to confer identity: the first verified whether threats to the organization originated internally, the second whether the subjects felt proud at belonging to their organizations, the third the degree of attention paid by the organization to it employees’ sense of belonging. The alpha coefficient for the three items was 0.88. Trust. Three items were used to ascertain the level of trust present in the organization. They concerned the sharing of goals, the sharing of strategies, and trust in senior management. The alpha coefficient of the three items was 0.93. Table 1 presents descriptive statistics and correlations for all study variables. ------------------------------Insert Table 1 about here -------------------------------- Levels of Analysis The explanatory variables were measured at two levels of analysis: individual and organizational. That the levels of analysis should be clearly defined has been frequently 18 recommended (Klein, Dansereau, & Hall, 1994; Kozlowski & Klein, 2000; Bliese, 2000; Morgeson & Hofmann, 1999; Gavin & Hofmann, 2002). At individual level, the data measured the respondents’ perceptions of the organizations to which they belonged and of the relations that they had experienced. The data were then aggregated at the level of the organization as a whole, because this was the object to which the individual items referred. The aggregation was not compositional in the strict sense (Bliese, 2000). In itself, it is not necessary to think of isomorphism between the meaning of the variable at individual level and aggregate level. The aggregate indicates the strength of reciprocal expectations and their capacity to transform themselves into collective behavior-regulating criteria. We may accordingly talk of partial isomorphism or of bottom-up fuzzy composition processes (Bliese, 2000). Finally, I used the average at organizational level in order to measure the intensity of these aggregate values, not because this is based on the sharing of perceptions but because the average is an indicator that enables one to graduate the strength with which organizational properties operate, according to the measurement scale. RESULTS Tests of the hypotheses Hypothesis 1. The first hypothesis was that it is possible to differentiate among organizations with the measures used to define their cultural properties. Calculation of the intraclass correlation coefficients (ICC(1)) produced the following values for empowerment, identity and 19 trust respectively: 0.23, 0.24 and 0.29. Around one quarter of the variance among the replies from the circa 4000 subjects in the sample can therefore be explained by their membership of their respective organizations. The ICC(1) for perception of collective involvement – the dependent variable for hypotheses 2 and 3 – was 0.11: a value lower than that obtained for the other variables but nevertheless significant because it showed the non-independence of the individual observations and suggested that multilevel analysis was required to reduce the probability of statistical errors in the estimates of the parameters describing the relations among the variables (Kreft & de Leeuw, 1998). Hypothesis 2. In order to test the second hypothesis, I first conducted multilevel analysis on the equation: collective involvementij = interceptj + empowermentij. This produced the following estimate of the parameters (data centered at the organization means; standard errors in brackets): collective involvement = 6.61 (0.11) + 0.29 (0.02) empowerment The perception of empowerment practices was associated with the perception of collective involvement: the increase was 0.29 for every point of empowerment perceived. The equations estimating the relationship between the other two properties and the perception of collective involvement were the following: collective involvement = 6.62 (0.11) + 0.30 (0.02) identity collective involvement = 6.62 (0.11) + 0.22 (0.02) trust 20 The parameters of the equations were average values which differed among the organizations. These differences were defined by the estimates of the residuals2 reported in Table 2. -----------------------------Insert Table 2 about here -----------------------------The estimates of the fixed effects for the three factors considered did not differ greatly. The same applies for the random parameters. In particular, comparisons among these latter for the three models showed that introducing the explanatory variable into the single intercept model there was a significant reduction of deviance, which is a good indicator of goodness of fit (Kreft & de Leeuw, 1998). These models were the first step in verifying hypothesis 2. Multiple regression with the three explanatory variables produced the following estimates: collective involvement = 6.61 (0.11) + 0.20 (0.02) empowerment + 0.16 (0.02) identity + 0.06 (0.02) trust The standard errors of the coefficients remained at a level such that hypothesis 2 was not rejected: each respondent’s perception of organizational relations, in the three forms of empowerment, sense of identity and trust, enabled prediction of how s/he would perceive collective involvement in his/her organization. Moreover, as we can see from table 2, the model 2 Only the intercepts were assumed as random parameters in the analysis described. Analysis of a more complex model in which also the slopes were random did not reveal significant changes in the fixed parameters. As Kreft and de Leeuw (1998) point out, the trade-off with multilevel procedures is between the advantage of yielding more reliable estimates of the parameters and the disadvantage of increasing the complexity of models and making them more unstable, the more they are complex. 21 with tree explanatory variables produces a reduction of residuals and a considerable improvement of fitting. Yet the prediction is not much accurate. The explained variances in the models are R2W 0.08 for empowerment, R2W 0.06 for identity, and R2W 0.04 for trust. For the multiple regression the explained variance is R2W 0.10. Hypothesis 3. The third hypothesis was that individual perceptions of relations aggregated at organizational level may manifest themselves as emergent cultural properties. The presence of these properties was determined by their correlation with individual perceptions of collective involvement. In order to test the hypothesis it was therefore necessary to consider the independent variables both at the individual level and when aggregated at the level of the organization. Analysis of the date yielded the following three equations: collective involvement = 6.45 (0.08) + 0.29 (0.02) empowerment + 0.55 (0.07) mean_empowerment collective involvement = 6.50 (0.09) + 0.30 (0.02) identity + 0.59 (0.08) mean_identity collective involvement = 6.54 (0.09) + 0.22 (0.02) trust + 0.45 (0.07) mean_trust The random parameters relative to these models are set out in Table 3. -----------------------------Insert Table 3 about here -----------------------------Taken individually, therefore, the three variables revealed their contextual effect. When evaluating the fixed parameters it had to be borne in mind that the variables at individual level were centered at organization means and variables at level 2 were centered at grand mean. Thus, 22 the regression coefficients at level 2 represented the group-level relation between the level 2 predictor and the dependent variable plus the influence of the level 1 predictor (Hofmann & Gavin, 1998). The texts of the differences between the two parameters in the three models give the following estimates: 0.25 (0.08) for menan_empowerment, 0.29 (0.09) for mean_identity, and 0.23 (0.07) for mean_trust. Moreover the addition of the organization-level explanatory variable reduces the between-organizations variance, so that we have the following R2B: 0.55 for mean_empowerment, 0.50 for mean_identity, and 0.44 for mean_trust. The model describing the combined effect of these models was the following: collective involvement = 6.44 (0.09) + 0.20 (0.02) empowerment + 0.16 (0.02) identity + 0.06 (0.02) trust + 0.55 (0.07) mean_empowerment In this model, the only aggregate-level effect that maintains statistical reliability is the effect for empowerment. The reason for the disappearance of the other effects at aggregate level is probably the collinearity among these variables. Whatever the case may be, the results for the fixed effects show an incremental prediction on the outcome over and above individual level predictors (Hofmann & Gavin, 1998). Besides the advantage of yielding more reliable estimates of the parameters when the data are not independent, multilevel analysis can be used to identify components of variance due to the context. Table 3 relative to the random parameters shows that there were differences among the organizations due to variance of the intercepts. Interpretation of these random effects is of great interest: when a person reports a certain degree of empowerment in his/her organization, it is of importance to know the value of the intercept in the equation tying the perception of empowerment to the perception of collective 23 involvement. In fact, the lower the intercept, the more likely it is that, for that person, corresponding to the level of perception of empowerment there will be a perception of a level of collective involvement lower than the level which s/he would perceive in an organization with a higher intercept. To say that another way: if two persons report the same value of empowerment in two organizations, s/he whose organisation has a lower intercept will probably perceive a lower level of collective involvement; The sensemaking effect distinguished by the relation yielded by the fixed parameters must therefore be interpreted in each individual organization because, as the random parameters show, the values of the intercepts differ significantly. It is consequently the organization that makes the difference. A high intercept signifies widely shared organizational codes, behavior expectations characterized by reciprocity, and a greater likelihood of involvement-based behavior. A low intercept signifies highly differentiated perceptions of relations (and of collective involvement), less reciprocity-based expectations, and a likelihood of behavior at odds with what Weick and Roberts (1993) call the ‘collective mind’. Overall, one may conclude that the contextual effects due to the aggregate values pass the test of statistical significance when these properties are considered individually. When these same variables are considered jointly, the only one of them which exerts an effect as an emergent property is empowerment. DISCUSSION 24 The analysis of data collected from a sample of 61 organizations and more than 4000 employees has shown that organizations differentiate the ways in which people perceive both organizational relations and collective involvement. The data evince the presence, at individual level, of cognitive congruence mechanisms in perceptions of organizational relations and collective behaviors that can be interpreted as sensemaking processes. Finally, the analysis has highlighted that individual perceptions are influenced by contextual effects: some relations present to a marked extent in people’s perceptions seemingly generate reciprocal expectations of involvement. The emergence of these phenomena at collective level can be interpreted as resulting from an equilibrium of forces which, once it has reached a certain intensity, produces contextual effects: that is to say, it significantly strengthens the impact of effects due to cognitive congruence at individual level. The idea of organizational codes as resulting from emergent processes and as regulating collective behavior relates to certain ways of interpreting organizational culture both as the effect of collective sensemaking (Weick, 1995) and as an expression of ‘basic assumptions’ (Schein, 1984), modes of thought, not always conscious, which guide people’s choices. Moreover, the definition of codes in quantitative as well as qualitative terms derives from the study by Hofstede et al. (1990) on the measurement of organizational culture. This approach enables contribution to be made to an area of inquiry that has attracted particular in recent years regarding the effects of organizational practices oriented to commitment (Arthur, 1994) or high involvement (Lawler, 1992) and, more generally, the use of high performance work practices. This study has proposed a number of variables which may shed clearer light on the connection between management practices and organizational performance. 25 More generally, the study helps resolve the problem of how to handle the effects of cultural properties on behavior in empirical terms. These properties derive from the way in which organizational members perceive relations among themselves and with the organization. The data on these perceptions thus acquire importance at both the individual and collective levels. Considered at individual level they have been used to test the hypothesis of congruence between the perception of intra-organizational relations and the perception of behaviors. Considered at organizational level they have been used to test the hypothesis of emergent processes such to transform the perceptions of organizational members into properties of organizations themselves. Managerial Implications These results have a number of practical implications. Firstly, they suggest a way to develop diagnoses of an organization’s cultural state by measuring indices which define cultural properties. Knowledge of these indices is necessary if suitable choice is to be made of managerial solutions and coherent governance actions. If it is true that a person’s perception of collective involvement in his/her organization is connected to his/her mode of perceiving the fundamental relations of identity, trust and empowerment, then it is necessary to develop management practices which foster those perceptions. For example, among the various professional characteristics of managers, priority should be given to the ability to inspire trust, to create a sense of identity, and to foster empowerment. Empirical analysis may enable assessment of these abilities and adjustment of governance actions to the pursuit of these goals. 26 To be stressed, however, is the risks that arise if human resources management policies are devised in individualistic terms and based mainly on actions ad personam. The emergent processes identified by this study suggest that the focus should be on team building, seeking to consolidate common perceptions of relational properties. Discussion of the third hypothesis highlighted the importance of contextual effects: the perception of collective involvement springs not only from how each organizational member perceives fundamental relations but also from how these relations are perceived on average in the organization. Improving the equilibrium of organizational forces deriving from the greater diffusion of a mode of perceiving relations entails strengthening the effect of emergent properties and increasing the likelihood that a more widespread perception of collective involvement will generate reciprocal expectations of committed behavior. These conclusions correspond to those reached by Weick and Roberts (1993) concerning the features of the collective mind and the need to develop not only new ways to measure performance but also to implement governance practices which foster social integration. Limitations and Future Research One limitation of the research reported by this study concerns the sample used, which was relatively small. Nevertheless, in view of the difficulty of collecting data on numerous organizations in a reasonable space of time, I have decided to propose this model in exploratory rather than confirmatory terms (Jöreskog, 1993). 27 Another limitation is due to the cross-sectional design of the research. The causal relation hypothesised between perceptions of organizational relations and perceptions of collective involvement is obviously not demonstrated by the correlation yielded by the equations. The effect may also operate the other way round. Moreover, there may be reciprocal causality relations. Considering perceptions of organizational relations as the basis for consolidating expectations, and considering these latter as necessary for generating committed behavior, is the theoretical core of this study. Only further research based on longitudinal analysis or experiments could reinforce this theoretical structure. Despite these limitations, attempts to operationalize the above concepts would, I believe, furnish comparative and cumulative analyses which improve understanding of the relations among management practices, organizational behavior and corporate performance. Developing this line of inquiry will require its extension to a larger sample of organizations which also covers different national and cultural contexts. Also required are longitudinal studies and experimental projects which substantiate the hypotheses of a causal relation between the variables considered. Moreover, the concept of organizational code should not be restricted to the relational equilibria defined by the independent variables examined by this study. Another codes, for example, control, procedure or supervision could be examined to determine the effect of bureaucratic cultural attitudes on perceptions of collective involvement. 28 REFERENCES Arrow, K. J., 1985. The economics of agency. In J. W. Pratt & R. Zeckhauser (Eds.), Principals and agents: The structure of business: 37-51. Bonston, MA: Harvard Business School Press. Albert, S., & Whetten, D. A. 1985. Organizational identity. In L. L. Cummings & B. M. Staw (Eds.), Research in organizational behavior, vol. 7: 263-295. Greenwich: CT: JAI Press. Anderson J. & Gerbig, D. W. 1984. The effects of sampling error on convergence, improper solutions and goodness-of-fit indices for maximum likelihood confirmatory factor analysis. Psychometrika, 49: 155-173. Arthur, J. B. 1994. Effects of human resource systems on manufacturing performance and turnover. Academy of Management Journal, 37: 670-687. Barnard , Ch. J. 1938. The Functions of the Executive. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Becker, B., & Gerhard, B. 1996. The impact of human resource management on organizational performance: Progress and prospects. Academy of Management Journal, 39: 779-801. Bigley, G. A., & Pearce, J. L. 1998. Straining for shared meaning in organization science: Problems of trust and distrust. Academy of Management Review, 23: 405-421. Blau , P. M. 1964. Exchange and power in social life. New York: John Wiley. Bliese, P. D. 2000. Within-group agreement, non-independence, and reliability: Implications for data aggregation and analysis. In K. J. Klein, & S. W. J. Kozlowski (Eds.), Multilevel 29 theory, research, and methods in organizations: foundations, extensions, and new directions: 349-381. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Bollen, K. A. 1989. Structural equations with latent variables. New York: Wiley. Bradach J. L. & Eccles R. G. 1989. Price, authority, and trust: From ideal types to plural forms. Annual Review of Sociology, 15: 97-118. Broms, H., & Gahmberg, H. 1983. Communication to self in organizations and cultures. Administrative Science Quarterly, 28: 482-495. Burke, P. J., & Reitzes, D. C. 1991. An identity theory approach to commitment. Social Psychology Quarterly, 54: 239-251. Cappelli, P., & Neumark, D. 2001. Do high-performance work practices improve establishement-level outcomes? Industrial and Labor Relations Review, 54: 737-775. Cappelli, P., & Sherer, P. D. 1991. The missing role of context in OB: The need for a mesolevel approach. In L. L. Cummings & B. M. Staw (Eds.), Research in organizational behavior, vol. 13: 55-110. Greenwich, CT: JAI Press. Coleman, J. S. 1990. The foundations of social theory. Cambridge MA: Harvard University Press. Conger, J. A., & Kanungo, R. N. 1988. The empowerment process: Integrating theory and practice. Academy of Management Review, 13: 471-482. Crawford, S. E. S., & Ostrom, E. 1995. A grammar of institutions. American Political Science Review, 89: 582-600. Crozier, M. 1964. The bureaucratic phenomenon. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. 30 Dirks, K. T., & Ferrin, D. L. 2001. The role of trust in organizational settings. Organization Science, 12(4): 450-467. Donnellon, A., Gray B., & Bougon, M. G. 1986. Communication, meaning, and organized action. Administrative Science Quarterly, 31: 43-55. Douglas, M. 1986. How institutions think. Syracuse, NY: Syracuse University Press. Dutton, J. E. & Dukerick J. M. 1991. Keeping an eye on the mirror: image and identity in organizational adaptation. Academy of Management Journal, 34: 517-554. Gagliardi, P. 1986. The creation and change of organizational culture: Conceptual framework. Organization Studies, 7: 117-134. Gavin, M. B., & Hofmann, D. A. 2002. Using hierarchical linear modelling to investigating the moderating influence of leadership climate. The Leadership Quarterly, 13: 15-33. Gittell, J. H., Von Nordenflycht, A., & Kochan, T. A. 2004. Mutual gain or zero sum? Labor relations and firm performance in the airline industry. Industrial and Labor Relations Review, 57(2): 163-180. Glick, W. H. 1985. Conceptualizing and measuring organizational and psychological climate: Pitfalls in multilevel research. Academy of Management Review, 10: 601-616. Granovetter, M. 1985. Economic action and social structure: The problem of embeddedness. American Journal of Sociology, 91: 481-510. Hofmann, D. A., & Gavin, M. B. 1998. Centering decisions in hierarchical linear models: Implications for research in organizations. Journal of Management, 24: 623-641. Hofstede, G. 1998. Attitudes, values and organizational culture: Disentangling the concepts. Organization Studies, 19: 477-492. 31 Hofstede, G., Neuijen, B., Ohayi, D. D., & Sanders, G. 1990. Measuring organizational cultures: A qualitative and quantitative study across twenty cases. Administrative Science Quarterly, 35: 286-316. House, R., Rousseau, D. M., & Thomas-Hunt, M. 1995. The meso paradigm: A framework for the integration of micro and macro organizational behavior. In L. L. Cummings & B. M. Staw (Eds.), Research in organizational behavior, vol. 17: 71-114. Greenwich, CT: JAI Press. Hu, L., & Bentler, P. M. 1995. Evaluating model fit. In R. H. Hoyle (Ed.), Structural equation modelling. Concepts, issues, and applications. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications. Huselid, M. 1995. The impact of human resource management practices on turnover, productivity and corporate financial performance. Academy of Management Journal, 38: 635-672. James, L. R., James, L. A., & Ashe, D. K. 1990, The meaning of organizations: the role of cognition and values. In B. Schneider (Ed.), Organizational climate and culture: 40-84. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Jöreskog, K. G. 1993. Testing structural equation models. In K. A. Bollen, & J. S. Long (Eds.), Testing structural equation models: 294-316. Newbury Park, London: Sage Publications. Klein, K. J., Dansereau, F., & Hall, R. J. 1994. Levels issues in theory development, data collection, and analysis. Academy of Management Review, 19:195-229. Kozlowski, S. W. J., Klein, K. J. 2000. A multilevel approach to theory and research in organizations. Contextual, temporal, and emergent processes. In K. J. Klein, & S. W. J. 32 Kozlowski (Eds.), Multilevel theory, research, and methods in organizations: 3-90. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Kreft, I., & de Leeuw, J. 1998. Introducing multilevel modeling, London, Sage. Lawler, E. E. III. 1992. The ultimate advantage: Creating the high-involvement organization. San Francisco: Jossey Bass. Lewin, K., 1951. Field theory in social science, New York: Harper & Row. Louis, M. R. 1980. Organizations as culture bearing milieux. In L. R. Pondy, P. J. Frost, G. Morgan, & T. C. Dandridge (Eds.), Organizational symbolism: 39-54. San Francisco: Jossey Bass. Luhmann, N. 1979. Trust and power. New York: J. Wiley & Sons. Luhmann, N. 1985. A sociological theory of law. London: Routledge. Macduffie, J. P. 1995. Human resource bundles and manufacturing performance: Organizational logic and flexible production systems in the world auto industry. Industrial and Labor Relations Review, 48: 197-221. Manning, P. K. 1979. Metaphor of the field: Varieties of organizational discourse. Administrative Science Quarterly, 24: 660-671. March, J. G., & Olsen, J. P. 1989. Rediscovering institutions. The organizational basis of politics. New York: The Free Press. Mayer, R. C., Davis, J. H., & Schoorman, F. D. 1995. An integrative model of organizational trust. Academy of Management Review, 20: 709-734. McEvery, B., Perrone, V, & Zeheer, A. 2003. Trust as an organizing principle. Organization Science, 14: 91-193. 33 Meyer, M. W., & Gupta, V., 1994. The performance paradox. In B. M. Staw & L. L. Cummings (Eds.), Research in organizational behavior, vol. 16: 309-369. Greenwich, CT: JAY Press. Miller, G. J., 1992. Managerial dilemmas: the political economy in hierarchies. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Morgeson, F. P., & Hofmann, D. A. 1999. The structure and function of collective constructs: Implications for multilevel research and theory development. Acandemy of Management Review, 24: 249-265. Mowday, R. T. 1998. Reflections on the study and relevance of organizational commitment. Human Resource Management Review, 8(4): 387-401. Mowday, R. T., Steers, R. M., & Porter, L. W. 1979. The measurement of organizational commitment. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 14: 224-247. Mowday, R. T., & Sutton, R. I. 1993. Organizational behavior: Linking individuals and groups to organizational contexts. Annual Review of Psychology, 44: 195-229. North, D. C. 1990. Institutions, institutional change and economic performance. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press. Organ, D. W. 1988. Organizational citizenship behavior: The “good soldier” syndrome. Lexington, MA: Lexington Books. Organ, D. W. 1990. The motivational basis of organizational citizenship behavior. In B. M. Staw & L. L. Cummings (Eds.), Research in organizational behavior, vol. 12: 43-72. Greenwich, CT: JAI Press. 34 Ouchi, W. G. 1980. Markets, bureaucracies, and clans. Administrative Science Quarterly, 25: 129-141. Pratt, M. G., & Foreman, P. O. 2000. Classifying managerial responses to multiple organizational identities. Academy of Management Review, 25: 18-42. Preuss, G. A. 2003. High performance work systems and organizational outcomes: The mediating role of information quality, Industrial and Labor Relations Review, 56(4): 590605. Reichers, A. E., & Schneider, B. 1990. Climate and culture: An evolution constructs. In B. Schneider (Ed.), Organizational climate and culture: 5-39. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Rousseau, D. M. 1985. Issues of level in organizational research: Multi-level and cross-level perspectives. In L. L. Cummings & B. M. Staw (Eds.), Research in organizational behavior, vol. 7: 1-37. Greenwich, CT: JAI Press. Rousseau, D. M.. 1990. Assessing organizatinal culture: the case for multiple methods. In B. Schneider (Ed.), Organizational climate and culture. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Rousseau, D. M. 1995. Psychological contracts in organizations: Understanding written and unwritten agreement, Newbury Park, CA: Sage Rousseau, D. M. 1997. Organizational behavior in the new organizational era. Annual Review of Psychology, 48: 515-546. Rousseau, D. M., & Parks, J. M. 1993. The contracts of individuals and organizations, In L. L. Cummings & B. M. Staw (Eds.), Research in organizational behavior, vol. 15: 1-43. Greenwich: CT: JAI Press. 35 Rousseau, D. M., Sitkin, S. B., Burt, R. S., & Camerer, C. 1998. Not so different after all: A cross-discipline view of trust. Academy of Management Review, 23: 393-404. Schank, R. C., & Abelson, R. P. 1977. Scripts, plants, goals and understanding: An inquiry into human knowledge structures. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Elbaum. Schein, E. H. 1984. Coming to a new awareness of organizational culture. Sloan Management review, 25: 3-16. Scott, W. R. 1995. Institutions and organizations. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Scott, S. G., & Lane, V. R. 2000. A stakeholder approach to organizational identity. Academy of Management Review, 25: 43-62. Seibert, S. E., Silver, S. R., & Randolph, W. A. 2004. Taking empowerment to the next level: A multiple-level model of empowerment, performance, and satisfaction. Academy of Management Journal, 47: 332-349. Seidler, J. 1974. On using informants: A technique for collecting quantitative data and controlling for measurement error in organizational analysis. American Sociological Review, 39: 816-831. Selznick, P. 1957. Leadership in administration. A sociological interpretation. New York: Harper & Row. Simon, H. A. 1957. Administrative behaviour (2nd ed.). New York: Macmillan. Sims, H. P., & LaFollette, W. 1975. An assessment of the Litwin and Stringer Organization Climate Questionnaire. Personnel Psychology, 28: 19-38. Smircich, L. 1983. Concepts of culture and organizational analysis. Administrative Science Quarterly, 28: 339-358. 36 Spreitzer, G. M. 1995. Psychological empowerment in the workplace: Dimensions, measurement, and validation. Academy of Management Journal, 38: 1442-1465. Thomas, K.W., & Velthouse, B. A. 1990. Cognitive elements of empowerment: An interpretative model of intrinsic task motivation. Academy of Management Review, 15: 666-681. Tsui, A. S., Pearce, J. L., Porter, L. W., & Tripoli, A. M. 1997. Alternative approaches to the employee-organization relationship: Does investment in employees pay off? Academy of Management Journal, 40: 1089-1121. Webb, E., & Weick, K. E. 1979. Unobtrusive measures in organizational theory: A reminder. Administrative Science Quarterly, 24: 650-659. Weick, K. E. 1979. The social psychology of organizing (2end ed.). Redading, MA: AddisonWesley. Weick, K. E. 1995. Sensemaking in organizations. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Weick, K. E., & Roberts, K. H. 1993. Collective mind in organizations: Heedful interrelating on flight decks. Administrative Science Quarterly, 38: 357-381. Williamson, O. E. 1975. Markets and hierarchies: Analysis and antitrust implications. A study in economics of internal organization. New York: The Free Press. Williamson, O. E. 1993. Calculativeness, Trust, and Economic Organization. Journal of Law and Economics, 36: 453-486. Zander, U., & Kogut, B. 1995. Knowledge and the speed of the transfer and imitation of organizational capability: An empirical test. Organization Science, 6(1): 76-92. 37 FIGURE 1 A Multilevel Model: Management Practices and Perceptions of Collective Involvement Organizational Management Aggregate level Practices Perceptions of collective Expectations Involvement H3 Perceptions Codes H1 Individual Perceptions level Of relations of collective Empowerment involvement Identity Trust H2 Perceptions 38 TABLE 1 Descriptive Statistics and Correlationsa Variables a Mean s.d. 1 2 3 4 Mean s.d. 1. Collective involvement 6.53 2.37 .75** .73** .67** 6.67 0.97 2. Empowerment 4.91 2.22 .30** .89** .80** 5.28 1.21 3. Identity 5.59 1.99 .28** .52** .87** 5.84 1.12 4. Trust 5.84 2.27 .23** .49** 6.08 1.37 .54** Organization-level means, standard deviations, and correlations (n = 61) are above the diagonal; individual-level means, standard deviation, and correlations (n = 4077) are below the diagonal. ** p < .01 39 TABLE 2 Multilevel Models of Organizational Relation Perceptions. Individual level. Dependent variable: collective involvement. Estimates of random effects (s.e.)a Models a intercept residuals deviance I = intercept only 0.66 (0.15) 5.14 (0.12) 18197.5 II = I + empowerment 0.65 (0.15) 4.75 (0.11) 17463.1 III = I + identity. 0.66 (0.15) 4.84 (0.11) 17382.7 IV = I + trust 0.66 (0.15) 4.94 (0.11) 17525.6 V = I + II + IV + VI 0.67 (0.16) 4.63 (0.11) 16623.1 I estimated these parameters with data centred on the organization mean. 40 TABLE 3 Multilevel Models of Organizational Relation Perceptions. Individual level and organizational level Dependent variable: collective involvement. Estimates of random effects (s.e.)a Models intercepts residuals deviance 0.66 (0.15) 5.14 (0.12) 18197.5 0.30 (0.07) 4.74 (0.11) 17423.1 III = I + identity + mean_identity 0.33 (0.08) 4.84 (0.11) 17345.7 IV = I + trust + mean_trust 0.37 (0.09) 4.93 (0.11) 17496.2 0.31 (0.08) 4.63 (0.11) 16583.4 I = intercept only II = I + empowerment + mean_empowerment V = I + empowerment + identity + trust + mean_empowerment a I estimated these parameters with data centred at individual-level on the organization mean, and at organization-level at the general mean.