Problem Solving Deteriorates Following Mood Challenge in Formerly Depressed Patients With a History of Suicidal Ideation J. Mark G. Williams, Thorsten Barnhofer, and A. T. Beck Catherine Crane University of Pennsylvania University of Oxford The authors divided 34 participants who had a history of depression into 2 groups, those having previous suicidal ideation or behavior (n � 19) and those having no such symptoms (n � 15), then compared the 2 groups with a group of participants who had no history of depression (n � 22). Assessment of interpersonal problem-solving performance using the Means-Ends Problem-Solving (MEPS) task before and after a mood-induction procedure showed that only those formerly depressed people with a history of suicidal ideation shifted in MEPS performance, producing significantly less effective problem solutions following mood challenge, consistent with a differential activation account of vulnerability for recurrence of suicidal ideation and behavior. The deterioration in effectiveness following mood challenge was moderated by lack of specificity in autobiographical memory. Keywords: depression, suicide, differential activation, problem solving, autobiographical memory We are interested in the factors that determine vulnerability to suicide and suicidal behavior. Suicidal behavior occurs most frequently in the context of an episode of psychiatric disorder, most commonly depression (Molnar, Berkman, & Buka, 2001), and the population attributable ratio (PAR) for depression in serious but nonfatal suicidal behavior (that proportion of suicidal behavior that would be removed if depression were removed) is 80% (Beautrais et al., 1996). However, because the vast majority of depressed patients do not attempt or commit suicide, research has focused on finding the additional risk factors that predict suicidal ideation and behavior. In our previous work, we have suggested that a major determinant of whether depressed mood escalates into suicidal thoughts and behavior is the unavailability of alternative coping options. This lack of options can mean that a person, when a situation becomes stressful, may feel trapped (the “arrested flight” model, Williams & Pollock, 2000). Many studies suggest that deficits in interpersonal problem solving in suicidal patients may be a critical factor in reducing coping options. These deficits then feed back in a vicious circle to reinforce suicide ideation. In one half of the vicious circle, problem-solving deficits increase helplessness, hopelessness, and feelings of being trapped, all of which intensifies suicidal ideation. In the next half of the circle, ideation—itself one of the most severe forms of self-focused rumination—further impairs problem solving (Watkins & Baracaia, 2002). J. Mark G. Williams, Thorsten Barnhofer, and Catherine Crane, Department of Psychiatry, University of Oxford, Oxford, United Kingdom; A. T. Beck, Psychopathology Research Unit, Department of Psychiatry, University of Pennsylvania. This research was supported by Wellcome Trust Grant GR067797. We thank Danielle Duggan, Melanie Fennell, Silvia Garnsey, and Wendy Swift for help with and discussion of this article. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to J. Mark G. Williams, University Department of Psychiatry, Warneford Hospital, Oxford OX3 7JX, United Kingdom. E-mail: mark.williams@psych.ox .ac.uk Typically, studies have used the Means-Ends Problem-Solving (MEPS) procedure (Platt, Spivack, & Bloom, 1975) because deficits on this measure are closely associated with a patient’s difficulty in finding ways to solve real-life problems (Marx, Williams, & Claridge, 1992; Rotheram-Borus, Trautman, Dopkins, & Shrout, 1990). Research has consistently found that suicidal patients are poor at generating relevant and effective means to deal with their interpersonal problems (in fact, they may be poorer problem solvers in general, though most research has focused on the interpersonal domain; Pollock & Williams, 1998). Differences in problem-solving ability between these groups are not attributable to differences in general intelligence (Biggam & Power, 1999). Importantly, problem-solving deficits are more severe in suicidal patients even if these patients are compared with other psychiatric patients matched for severity of illness and with depressed mood covaried out (Pollock & Williams, 2001). It appears that poor problem solving, a general feature of people who suffer psychiatric disturbance, is particularly associated with suicidality. However, there is a problem in interpreting these results: Problem solving has usually been measured soon after the suicidal episode. It is therefore not possible to determine to what extent these deficits are long-term traits that render patients vulnerable or how much they are state-dependent—that is, simply a feature of short-term suicidal crises. The data on this point remain ambiguous. Despite early studies showing that MEPS performance was a trait feature, or diathesis (Schotte & Clum, 1987), there is also evidence that problem solving is state-dependent: Performance that is poor in a crisis rapidly recovers (Biggam & Power, 1999; Schotte, Cools, & Payvar, 1990) and the relationship between problem solving and repetition of suicidal behavior weakens when current level of depression is partialed out (Hawton, Kingsbury, Steinhardt, James, & Fagg, 1999). The conclusion from available studies is that a “simple, diathesis stress model appears to be inadequate in explaining the relationship between problem-solving skills and suicidal behavior . . . the direction of the relationship has not yet been adequately resolved” (Biggam & Power, 1999, p. 38). 421 This theoretical issue has important clinical implications. If, once the suicidal crisis is past, problem solving returns to normal, then it is difficult to know which patient remains vulnerable, or whether any therapy is succeeding in dealing with the underlying vulnerability. How can we determine which individuals remain vulnerable to future suicidal crises even when they appear to have completely recovered? Recently we have suggested that this question may best be answered in the context of differential activation theory (Lau, Segal, & Williams, 2004), which was developed to explain why people remain vulnerable to depressive recurrence even when the usual cognitive vulnerability markers (e.g., dysfunctional attitudes) appear to have normalized on recovery. The theory states that it is not the “resting level” of such attitudes when euthymic that determines vulnerability, but how easily these attitudes are reactivated by small changes in mood. Why might suicidality be particularly explicable by differential activation theory, which has heretofore been applied only to depression in general? The original theory stated that during a person’s learning history—and particularly during episodes of depression—low mood becomes associated with patterns of negative information processing (biases in memory, interpretations, and attitudes). Any return of the mood reactivates the pattern, and if the content of what is reactivated is global, negative, and self-referent (e.g., “I am a failure; worthless and unlovable”), then relapse and recurrence of depression is highly likely (Teasdale, 1988). Of all the noncore symptoms of depression, suicidal ideation (as well as guilt, with which it is closely associated) is the one that represents the most severe type of negative global self-referent thinking pattern. Although other symptoms of depression (e.g., appetite and sleep changes, fatigue, agitation or retardation, and concentration difficulties) are important features of the depressive picture, they are not themselves patterns of self-referent thoughts. Thus we might expect that suicidal ideation, once initiated, may become linked with other negative global self-referent thought (Malone et al., 2000). Vulnerability to recurrence of suicidal ideation and behavior consists of the ease with which these specific cognitive patterns can be reactivated by small amounts of negative mood. How can we test whether differential activation theory applies to suicidality? The usual procedure to test the theory as it applies to depression is to use mood challenge (Ingram, Miranda, & Segal, 1998; Segal & Ingram, 1994). In these experiments, people who have recovered from depression are compared with people who have never been depressed. Dysfunctional attitudes, memory bias, attentional bias, or self-referent descriptions are measured before and after the mood challenge. There have been some discrepant results, but the majority of studies report that those who have been depressed in the past, although not distinguishable before the mood challenge, show a reactivated pattern of negative cognitions following the mood challenge (Lau et al., 2004). In order to investigate whether this model is applicable to suicidal vulnerability, we needed to show that patients assessed during recovery would show no evidence of suicidal vulnerability but that, following a mood challenge, the vulnerability would become clear. Furthermore, because our previous research had shown that problem-solving deficits are a signature characteristic of suicidality, we hypothesized that mood challenge would differentially affect problem-solving ability in those patients who had been suicidal in the past. To test this hypothesis, we followed previous studies by recruiting a sample of people who had an episode of major depression in the past and a comparison sample of people who had never been depressed, then asking both groups to undergo a mood challenge. However, we added two features that were different from previous studies. First, using both clinical interviews and questionnaires, we divided the previously depressed group into those who either had or had not experienced suicidal ideation and behavior when depressed. Second, rather than using a test of dysfunctional attitudes as our outcome variable (as in most previous studies of differential activation), we had participants complete the MEPS test before and after the mood challenge. We report data showing that, whereas all participants showed significantly greater mood disturbance as a result of the mood challenge, only those participants with suicidality in their past were affected in their problem-solving performances (i.e., they generated solutions that were significantly less effective). This study also gave us the opportunity to examine the relation between differential activation processes and autobiographical memory. Research has found that depressed and suicidal patients tend to respond to cue words in an Autobiographical Memory Test with memories that are less specific; that is, their responses do not single out a particular event (e.g., happy 7 “going for a long walk three weeks ago”) but instead are overgeneral (e.g., happy 7 “going for long walks”) (Pollock & Williams, 2001; Williams, 2004; Williams & Broadbent, 1986; Williams et al., 1996). Moreover, research shows that patients’ difficulties in being specific about the past are associated with difficulties in problem solving: Failure to retrieve specific memories prevents patients from accessing previous occasions when they have solved analogous problems in the past (Evans, Williams, O’Loughlin, & Howells, 1992; Pollock & Williams, 2001). How might overgeneral memory interact, then, with differential activation theory? If reactivation of suicidal cognition affects problem solving as we predict, then those people who have difficulty in retrieving specific memories will be more vulnerable to the effects of mood challenge. They will have fewer adequate representations of past coping behavior to help generate alternative solutions to current problems as their problem-solving process comes under stress from mood disturbance. We therefore administered a test of autobiographical memory before and after the mood induction. It was not clear whether specificity would change with mood, inasmuch as the evidence is ambiguous with respect to this question (MacCallum, McConkey, Bryant, & Barnier, 2000; McBride & Cappeliez, 2004; Svaldi & Mackinger, 2003). We found no effect of mood on overgenerality in memory, so memory could not be a mediator of change in problem-solving performance. Nevertheless, we report data showing that level of memory specificity prior to the mood challenge moderated the extent to which problem solving deteriorated. Method Participants We recruited participants by contacting people who had taken part in previous studies in the University of Oxford’s Department of Psychiatry and by advertising on the website of a local newspaper. Participants from previous studies had indicated their willingness to be contacted again for further research and, as part of the previous studies, had completed questionnaires about prior history of depression. This factor allowed us to conduct purposive sampling, contacting an equal number of those who had indicated a history of major depression, with or without suicidal ideation, and a similar number of those who had indicated that they had never been depressed. We contacted 185 (108 female, 77 male, age: M �47.03, SD �7.97) potential participants in the Oxford region by letter; 57 (35 female, 22 male, age: M �47.82, SD �7.6) responded. Age and sex distribution did not differ between those who volunteered and those who did not: sex, � (1, N �145) �.07, ns; age, t(143) ��.62, ns. Another 14 participants (10 female, 4 male; 10 formerly depressed, 4 never depressed; age M �31.2, SD �11.1) were recruited via the advertisements on the local newspaper website. This group did not differ in sex distribution, � (71) �.48, p �.10, but was significantly younger than those participants who had taken part in previous research, t(69) �6.7, p �.01. Current and past diagnostic status was assessed using the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM–IV Axis I (SCID-I; First, Spitzer, Gibbon, & Williams, 1996) with the addition of the Borderline Personality Disorder section from the SCID-II (First, Gibbon, Spitzer, & Williams, 1997). All interviews were administered by a postdoctoral clinical or research psychologist. To be included in the study, participants had to meet the National Institute of Mental Health criterion for remission of depression: being symptom-free for at least 8 weeks (Frank et al., 1991; Keller, Shapiro, Lavori, & Wolfe, 1982). In addition, participants had to be from 18 to 65 years of age. Five participants were excluded because they failed to meet criteria for recovery and 7 more participants were excluded because of current active or passive suicidal ideation (assessed during the clinical interview and on the basis of responses to the Beck Scale for Suicide Ideation [BSS], as described in the Measures section). Also, 1 participant was excluded because of current mania, 1 because of current psychotic symptoms, and 1 of because of age exceeding upper limit.on the We defined three groups participants according the to responses SCIDs and the BSS—“Worst Ever” (BSSW) questionnaire described in the Measures section. The first group (Control; n �22) comprised individuals with no history of major depressive disorder (MDD) or suicidality. The second group (MDD-suic; n �19) comprised individuals with a history of MDD who also had reported past suicidal ideation either during the clinical interview or on the BSSW questionnaire. The third group (MDD-no suic; n �15) comprised individuals with a history of MDD who did not report past suicidal ideation during the interview or on the BSSW. Table 1 shows the median ages and the sex distributions of the groups; there was no significant difference groups terms of thesedepressed demographics. Analysis was carried outbetween to compare the in two previously groups 2 2 in terms of the presence of other core symptoms of MDD, in order to ensure that the groups were matched in this regard. Only symptoms rated Table 1 Demographics and Symptom Scores for Control Group and Previously Depressed Participants MDD-no suicb Controla Variable Demographic Age Symptom score BDI BHS BSSw MDD-suicc M SD M SD M SD 49.6 4.1 2.8 0.0 7.3 4.2 2.7 0.0 43.6 8.4 4.7 0.6 9.2 4.7 4.2 1.1 42.9 12.0 7.0 13.3 12. 0 8.0 3.8 7.4 Note. BDI �Beck Depression Inventory; BHS �Beck Hopelessness Scale; BSSw �Beck Scale for Suicide Ideation—Worst ever. MDD � major depressive disorder; suic �suicidality history. Control group, n �22 (13 female, 9 male). Participants with history of MDD but no suic, n �15 (9 female, 6 male). Participants with both history of MDD and suic, n �19 (14 female, 5 male). a b c as definitely present during the participants’ worst episode of MDD, as assessed by the SCID interviews, were analyzed. Fisher’s exact test (twotailed) was used to examine possible differences between the MDD-suic and MDD-no suic groups. Table 2 shows the percentages of participants in the MDD-suic and MDD-no suic groups reporting each core symptom of MDD. No significant differences between the groups were identified regarding the presence of any of the core symptoms of MDD. Table 2 also shows that the two groups did not differ significantly regarding either the number of prior episodes of MDD reported or the age at first onset of MDD/dysthymia. Differences in mean length of the longest past episode also were not significant, although episodes were numerically longer in those with previous suicidal ideation than in those without such ideation, F(1, 32) �2.06, p �.16. Past episodes with a duration of 2 years or longer were reported by 6 of the participants in the group with previous suicidal ideation and 1 participant in the group without suicidal ideation, Fisher’s exact test (one-sided) p � .09. Criteria for a past history of dysthymic disorder were met by 1 participant in the group with previous suicidal ideation and 2 participants in the group without suicidal ideation. A past episode of mania was diagnosed in 1 participant in the previously depressed group without suicidal ideation and 1 participant in the previously depressed group with suicidal ideation. Additionally, 1 of the participants in the previously suicidal group reported having suffered a single (antidepressant-induced) episode of SCID interviews revealedfrom current diagnoses for psychiatric disorders other hypomania at theinage than depression 18 of 16. the participants. Criteria for either a full diagnosis or partial remission were met in 3 of the 22 control participants (2 specific phobia, 1 social phobia); 7 of the 15 participants in the MDD-no suic group (2 generalized anxiety disorder [1 in partial remission], 1 social phobia and obsessive–compulsive disorder, 1 specific phobia and obsessive–compulsive disorder, 1 alcohol dependence in early full remission, 1 alcohol dependence in full remission with current mild problems, and 1 panic disorder in partial remission); and 8 of the 19 participants in the MDD-suic group (4 specific phobia, 1 bulimia nervosa in partial remission, 1 panic disorder in partial remission, 1 panic disorder with agoraphobia, and 1 borderline personality disorder and alcohol dependence in early full remission). Severity ratings of the disorder did not exceed a mild-tomoderate degree in any of the cases. Measures Visual Analog Scale (VAS) mood rating. Two VASs, each 10 cm in length, were used to rate the participant’s mood (from despondency to happiness) on four occasions during the testing session: once immediately prior to commencing the experimental tasks, once immediately prior to the mood induction, once immediately following the mood induction, and once at the end of the experimental tasks. For each rating the statement “At this moment I feel . . . ” was printed above the line and either “happy” or “despondent” was printed below the line, anchored on a scale of not at all Beck Depression Inventory (BDI-II). The BDI-II is a well-established to extremely. measure of depressive symptomatology that contains 21 groups of statements, referring to the presence of symptoms of depression over the preceding 2 weeks (Beck, Steer, & Brown, 1996). BSS. The BSS scale contains 21 groups of statements referring to the presence or absence of various forms of suicidal ideation (thoughts, plans, or wishes to commit suicide) during the preceding week (Beck & Steer, 1993b; Beck, Steer, & Ranieri, 1988). BSSW. This questionnaire, a modified version of the BSS developed for this study, is based on the clinician-rated Scale for Suicidal Ideation worst-point suicidality interview, which has been used in several studies (e.g., Joiner et al., 2003). Research suggests that worst-point suicidality represents a significant predictor of an individual’s future risk of death by suicide (Beck, Brown, Steer, Dahlsgaard, & Grisham, 1999). The BSSW questionnaire contains the same items as the BSS but is worded in the past tense, requiring respondents to indicate at what time in their life they felt the “most down about life, the most depressed you have been,” and then report on their suicidal ideation at that time. Table 2 Worst-Episode Symptoms, Treatment Experience, and Symptom History of Previously Depressed Participants MDD-no suic MDD-suic Variable n %of Nn %of N Fisher’s exact test p Worst-episode symptoms (assessed) Loss of interest 15 100 17 89 .49 Appetite or weight change 11 73 14 74 1.00 Sleep disturbance 10 73 17 89 .19 Agitation/retardation 10 67 13 68 1.00 Fatigue 13 87 15 79 .67 Worthlessness/guilt 14 93 15 79 .35 Indecisiveness/concentration 15 100 17 89 .49 Treatment experience Antidepressants (last episode) 6 40 12 63 .16 Counseling/psychotherapy (ever) 6 40 11 58 .46 Antidepressants (current) 3 20 2 11 .63 M SD M SD Analysis of variance p Symptom history Age at onset 25.2 9.6 21.5 11.0 .32 No. of prior episodes 2.5 1.9 3.0 2.4 .51 Longest episode (months) 7.8 12.09 15.1 16.47 .16 a Note. MDD � major depressive disorder; suic � suicidality history; SCID � Structured Clinical Interview for DSM–IV. Age at onset refers to onset of any depressive disorder (i.e., includes dysthymia for 2 participants in MDD-no suic group and 1 in MDD-suic group). a Beck Hopelessness Scale (BHS). The BHS contains 20 statements describing negative and positive attitudes toward the future (Beck & Steer, 1993a). MEPS task. Participants were presented with problem scenarios on cards that were simultaneously read aloud by the experimenter. Each scenario outlined an initial situation in which there was a problem to be solved and a desired end point (Marx, Williams, & Claridge, 1992; Platt et al., 1975). In line with previous studies in suicidal and depressed populations, rather than presenting the MEPS as a test of imagination, a clear problem-solving set was introduced, with participants given 60 s to describe the most effective strategy for solving the problem (Marx, Williams & Claridge, 1992; Watkins & Baracaia, 2002). In the current study, participants were given an additional 60 s to describe alternative strategies for solving the problem. For each problem scenario, ratings of effectiveness and number of relevant means were based on the total response of the participant, across both Two sets ofsections. MEPS items, each comprising 3 of the original 10 scenarios described by Platt et al., were prepared. Set A contained scenarios describing (a) relationship difficulties with a boyfriend/girlfriend, (b) losing a watch, and (c) making friends in a new neighborhood. Set B contained scenarios describing (a) difficulties with a supervisor at work, (b) starting a relationship with an attractive boy/girl, and (c) friends avoiding you. Participants were randomly allocated to order A/B or B/A. Scenarios were presented in the third person, with the protagonist of each story presented as the same sex as the participant. Participants’ responses were recorded on audiocassette and transcribed before rating. For each MEPS scenario, two dependent variables were derived: the overall effectiveness of the participant’s response, which was rated blind on a 7-point scale (1 � not at all effective to 7 � extremely effective), and the number of relevant means (active problem-solving steps) the participant produced. As described above, in both cases these ratings were derived on the basis of the total response of participants to each problem item. Transcripts were rated by two independent raters, both of whom were blind to group status. Consistency between raters was established on a random sample of 15% of the cases yielding coefficients of � (54) � .84 (excellent) for number of relevant means and � (55) � .68 (substantial) for ratings of effectiveness. Participants’ effectiveness ratings and relevant means scores were summed across items administered at the same stage of the experiment to create overall scores for effectiveness and total number relevant means pre-and post-mood induction. Autobiographical Memory Test (AMT). Participants were presented one at a time with a series of cue words, which were read to them by the experimenter and also presented written on cards. For each cue word, participants were asked to report an event that had happened to them that the word reminded them of—a specific event, defined as an event that had lasted less than a day and occurred at a particular time and place. Participants were additionally asked not to include memories from the preceding week and to avoid repeating events described in response to previous cue words. Three practice words were given to participants, with practice continuing until specific memories had been recalled for at least two of these three words. Participants were given 30 s to respond to each cue word. Individuals’ responses to each cue word were recorded verbatim by the and also recorded6on audiotape for laterinrating. Twoexperimenter sets of 18 words (6 positive; negative; 6 neutral each) were used as cues. In each set the positive, negative, and neutral words were matched for frequency. Additionally, the positive and negative words were matched in (high) emotionality, with the neutral words chosen to be low in emotionality. Sets A and B were matched to one another in overall frequency and emotionality of words. Following previous studies, responses were later rated by the experimenter as specific (events lasting less than a day); categoric (repeated events); extended (events lasting longer than one day); semantic associates of the cue word, and omissions (no response; details of scoring and procedures for the AMT are available from the authors). Procedure After an initial introduction and opportunity for questions, participants gave their written informed consent to participate in the study. The study received ethical approval from the Oxfordshire Psychiatric Research Ethics Committee. group, F(2, 52) � 6.00, p � .005, partial � � .19, and a significant interaction between time and group, F(4.38, 113.98) � 2.95, p � .02, partial � � .10. For happiness there was again a main effect of time, F(2.39, 124.14) � 38.67, p � .001, partial � � .43, a main effect of group, F(2, 52) � 6.27, p � .004, partial � � .19, and an interaction between time and group that was at trend level after applying the Greenhouse-Geisser correction to adjust the degrees of freedom, F(4.78, 124.14) � 2.23, p � .06, partial � � .08. Post hoc pairwise comparisons (Bonferroni) were used to further examine the interactions between time and group for both despondency and happiness ratings. Differences in the pre-mood induction phase. There were no significant differences between participants in levels of despondency or happiness at the beginning of the experimental session. However, by Time 2 (pre-mood induction) participants in the MDD-suic group reported significantly higher levels of despondency and lower levels of happiness than controls (both ps � .01) and those in the MDD-no suic group (both ps � .05). Effects of mood induction. In all three participant groups there was a significant worsening of mood from Time 2 (pre-mood induction) to Time 3 (post-mood induction), indicated by significant reductions in happiness (all ps � .002) and significant increases in despondency (all ps � .001). Immediately after the induction, participants in the MDD-suic group were significantly more despondent and less happy than individuals in the control group (both ps � .008), but did not differ from individuals in the MDD-no suic group. There were also differences at trend level between the controls and the MDD-no suic group in both despondency and happiness post-mood induction. Post-induction test phase. None of the groups changed significantly from Time 3 to Time 4 in either happiness or despondency ratings, indicating that induced sad mood was largely maintained. 2 Measures. The study began with the SCID interview. Following this, participants completed the MEPS and AMT on two occasions, once prior to and once following a mood-induction procedure. The tasks were administered in the same order pre-and post-mood induction, with the MEPS completed prior to the AMT. Following completion of all tasks, participants filled in a questionnaire booklet, including the BDI-II, BHS, BSS, and BSSW. At the end of the experimental session, participants were debriefed. Researchers ensured that any participants who had become upset during the mood-induction procedure had returned to their normal mood before departing. Mood induction. Participants were informed that the purpose of the mood-induction procedure was to induce a sad mood and that in order to do this they would be asked to listen to music and to read cards containing sad statements (40 Velten negative statements such as, “There are things about me that I do not like.”). The mood-induction music (“Russia Under the Mongolian Yoke” by Prokofiev, remastered at half-speed) was played to the participant through loudspeakers for 8 min. Participants were asked to read through the Velten statements and to identify those that were most helpful to them in inducing a sad mood while trying to evoke the thoughts and feelings described by the cards. Two mood-induction boosters were given to sustain the sad mood during completion of the post-mood induction tasks, one immediately prior to the post-mood induction MEPS task and one immediately prior to the post-mood induction AMT. Results Participant Characteristics Depression. BDI-II scores for each group are shown in Table 1. Univariate analysis of variance (ANOVA) indicated a significant main effect of group on current BDI-II score, F(2, 53) � 9.05, p � .001. Post hoc tests (Bonferroni) indicated that although there was no significant difference between the two formerly depressed groups, the BDI-II score in the MDD-suic group was significantly higher than in controls, MI-J � 7.87, SE � 1.86, p � .001. Hopelessness. Current levels of hopelessness, as measured by the 20-item BHS, were compared across the three groups (see Table 1). Univariate ANOVA indicated a main effect of group on current levels of hopelessness, F(2, 53) � 6.98, p � .002, with post hoc tests (Bonferroni) revealing that although there were no significant differences between the two formerly depressed groups, the MDD-suic group was significantly more hopeless than controls (MI-J � 4.17, SE � 1.12, p � .001). Worst-ever suicidality. Participants’ scores on the BSSW are shown in Table 1. Univariate ANOVA indicated a main effect of group, F(2, 53) � 56.28, p � .001, due to the fact that, as expected, participants in the MDD-suic group scored significantly higher than those in both the control group (MI-J � 13.32, SE � 1.37, p � .001) and in the MDD-no suic group (MI-J � 12.72, SE � 1.51, p � .001). 2 2 2 2 2 MEPS Task Number of relevant means. The number of means produced by participants prior to the mood-induction procedure was similar in each group (Controls: M � 24.9, SD � 6.56; MDD-no suic: M � 26.33, SD � 10.31; MDD-suic: M � 23.21, SD � 5.65). Following the mood induction, the number of means produced appeared to be somewhat lower in all groups (Controls: M � 22.64, SD � 6.03; MDD-no suic: M � 22.20, SD � 8.00; MDD-suic: M � 19.11, SD � 6.68). A repeated measures ANOVA conducted to examine the effect of the negative mood induction on the number of relevant means indeed revealed a main effect of time, F(1, 53) � 26.51, p � .001, partial � � .33, as a result of participants producing fewer relevant 2 1 Visual Analog Mood Ratings Figure 1A shows changes in participants’ ratings of happiness and Figure 1B shows changes in participants’ ratings of despondency across the experimental session. Changes in despondency and happiness ratings were examined separately using repeated measures ANOVAs with time as the within-subjects factors and group as the between-subjects factor. Effect sizes were calculated using the partial eta squared statistic (partial � ), with .01 representing a small effect, .06 a moderate effect, and .14 a large effect. Analysis of despondency revealed a main effect of time, F(2.19, 113.98) � 54.83, p � .001, partial � � .51, a main effect of 1 2 2 Mauchly’s test indicated that the data for both happiness and despondency were significantly nonspherical, so the Greenhouse-Geisser correction was applied to adjust the degrees of freedom where appropriate. After controlling for BDI in the analyses of despondency change, the main effect of time, F(2.21, 112.80) � 33.95, p � .001, partial � � .40, and interaction between time and group F(4.42, 112.81) � 4.24, p � .002, partial � � .14, remained significant. In the case of happiness ratings, the main effect of time, F(2.38, 121.41) � 14.77, p � .001, partial � � .23, remained significant, whereas the interaction between time and group, F(4.76, 121.41) � 1.95, p � .095, was a trend. Significant decreases in happiness from Time 2 to Time 3 were observed in all groups (all ps � .003). However, there were no significant differences between groups at any time point. 2 2 2 2 Figure 1. Mean sum scores of Visual Analogu e Scale (VAS) happines s (A) and VAS despond ency (B) ratings at pre-and post-mo od inductio nassess ments in controls, previous ly depresse d individu als without suicidal ideation (MDD-n o suic), and previous ly depresse d individu als with suicidali deation (MDD-s uic). MDD � major depressi ve disorder. Figure 2. Mean Means-End Problem Solving task problem-solving effectiveness pre-and post-mood induction in controls, previously depressed individuals without suicidal ideation (MDD-no suic), and previously depressed individuals with suicidal ideation (MDD-suic). MDD �major depressive disorder. means post-mood induction. However, there was no significant interaction between time and group, and no between-subjects effect of group, indicating that all participant groups responded similarly to the negative mood-induction procedure in this regard. Problem-solving effectiveness. A repeated measures ANOVA with group as the between-subjects factor and time as the within-subjects factor revealed a main effect of time, F(1, 53) �21.81, p �.001, partial � �.29, and a significant interaction between time and group, F(2, 52) �5.67, p �.006, partial � �.18. There was no between-subjects effect of group. 2 2 Post hoc pairwise comparisons (Bonferroni), used to examine the interaction between group and time (shown in Figure 2), indicated that the MDD-suic participants were significantly less effective in problem solving post-mood induction, relative to their pre-mood induction scores (Mpre �12.58, SD �2.22 vs. Mpost � 10.16, SD �2.63; MI-J ��2.42, SE �.44, p �.001), whereas the never-depressed controls (Mpre �12.50, SD �2.18 vs. Mpost � 11.77, SD �2.79 and MDD-no suic group (Mpre �12.33, SD � 2.58 vs. Mpost �11.87, SD �2.13) showed no significant change. Thus, individuals in the MDD-suic group experienced a greater deterioration in problem solving than controls or nonsuicidal previously depressed participants. Next, we examined the difference in effectiveness between the groups at Time 2 covarying out Time 1 effectiveness scores. In addition to the main effect of pre-mood induction effectiveness, F(1, 52) �50.21, p �.001, partial � � .49, there was a main effect of group, F(2, 52) �5.75, p �.006, resulting from the fact that the MDD-suic group showed less effective post-mood induction problem solving than both the MDD-no suic group (MI-J ��1.90, SE �.64, p �.05) and the control group (MI-J ��1.68, SE �.58, p �.02), which did not differ from each other (MI-J ��.22, SE �.62, p �.1). 2 As discussed in the preceding sections, significant differences between groups were identified in current BDI-II and BHS scores, and in levels of happy and despondent mood during the pre-and post-mood induction test phases. Pearson’s product moment correlations were computed between each of these variables and the measure of pre-to post-mood induction effectiveness change, separately in each participant group. There were no significant correlations (all ps �.1), with the exception of a trend toward an association between BHS score and effectiveness change in the control group (r �.41, p �.06). 3 Autobiographical Memory Exploration of the autobiographical memory data showed that in most of the cases in which participants had not been able to come up with a specific memory, responses took the form of omissions; mean numbers of memories classified as extended, categorical, or semantic associate were comparably small (mean numbers within groups �1). This prevented comparison of different forms of 3 As an added precaution, we conducted repeated measures ANCOVAs with group and order as between-subjects factors and time (pre-, post-mood induction) as a within-subjects factor to examine the effect of the mood induction on problem-solving effectiveness, controlling separately for BDI, BHS, and the VAS measures. An ANCOVA including only the previously depressed groups was also conducted to control for length of longest episode of depression. In each case the interaction between time and group remained significant (BDI: F(2, 49) �4.13, p �.02, partial � �.14; BHS: F(2, 49) �4.04, p �.05, partial � �.14; VAS measures: F(2, 45) �3.79, p �.03, partial � �.14; length of episode: F(1, 31) �6.09, p �.02, partial � �.164. 2 2 2 2 overgeneral memories and further analyses were, consequently, based on total numbers of specific memories retrieved. Means and standard deviations of numbers of specific memories before and after the mood induction are depicted in Table 3. Inspection of the table shows that memory performance remained largely unaffected by the mood induction in all three groups. A 2 � 3 repeated measures ANOVA with time (pre-vs. post-mood induction) as within-subjects and group (control vs. MDD-no suic vs. MDDsuic) yielded no significant main or interaction effects (all ps � .1). Autobiographical Memory and MEPS Effectiveness In order to investigate possible moderating effects of memory specificity, changes in problem-solving effectiveness were further analyzed by dividing participants into those with baseline specificity scores greater than 14 or less than or equal to 14 (the overall group median). A 2 � 2 � 2 repeated measures ANOVA with time (effectiveness pre-vs. post-mood induction) as within-subjects and specificity (high vs. low) and group (control vs. MDD-no suic vs. MDD-suic) as between-subjects factors showed a significant time � specificity interaction, F(1, 50) � 5.07, p � .05, partial � � .09. This interaction was due to significant decreases in problem-solving effectiveness in individuals with low memory specificity (MI-J � 1.73, SE � .31, p � .001); there were no significant differences in those with high memory specificity (MI-J � .57, SE � .40, p � .16). The three-way interaction failed to reach significance, F(2, 50) � 1.66, p �the .20. Because differential activation of suicidality hypothesis related specifically to the group with a history of suicidal ideation, we examined effectiveness decreases within each group. This confirmed that differences were significant only in the previously suicidal group. Although effectiveness of problem solving was significantly impaired in previously suicidal individuals whether they were high (MI-J � 1.40, SE � .57, p � .02) or low in memory specificity (MI-J � 3.56, SE � .61, p � .001), the impairment was more pronounced in the latter group. Effectiveness changes in both of these subgroups are depicted in Figure 3. A 2 (effectiveness: pre-vs. post-mood induction) � 2 (memory specificity: high vs. low) repeated measures ANOVA yielded a significant interaction, F(1, 17) � 7.39, p � .01, partial � � .30, confirming the moderating effect of overgenerality on decreases in problem-solving effectiveness in previously suicidal individuals. Results of the above analyses remained unchanged when change in despondency and happiness were entered as covariates, suggesting that 2 2 the moderating effect of memory specificity was not attributable to possible influences on actual degree of mood change. Discussion The aim of this study was to investigate whether the differential activation processes found in people who are vulnerable to depressive relapse can be extended to suicidal ideation and behavior. The study was motivated by the observation that not all depressed patients are suicidal; hence, the need to specify in greater detail what renders patients specifically vulnerable to suicidality. We chose to examine interpersonal problem solving, a feature of psychological performance that has consistently been found to be associated with suicidality, and to examine it following a mood challenge in formerly depressed participants and never-depressed controls. The results showed that, whereas the number of relevant means produced in MEPS tasks was reduced following mood challenge in all groups, the effectiveness of these solutions was selectively more impaired in those who had been depressed and suicidal in the past. Such suicidality was not merely a marker of having had more episodes of depression, or of having had more serious or a different subtype of depression, because we were able to check that other symptoms did not distinguish the groups. This is consistent with data from Mann et al.’s work showing that patients with a diagnosis of MDD with a history of suicidal behavior were indistinguishable on clinician ratings of severity of depression, but were different on more cognitively focused self-ratings (Beck’s Depression and Hopelessness scales, and Linehan’s Reasons for Living inventory; Malone et al., 2000). The present study extends such findings to patients in remission, showing that when mood has returned to normal, cognitive variables may return to normal, but those who have been depressed and suicidal in the past are vulnerable to react differentially to changes in mood—with greater deterioration in problem-solving ability. The current study extends differential activation theory in a number of ways. First, it extends it from a general theory about depressive relapse to the important specific problem of suicidal ideation and vulnerability. Second, it extends the range of relevance of the theory from purely cognitive measures (attitudes, memory, and attentional bias) to a behavioral measure: problem solving. To our knowledge this is the first study to apply differential activation theory to a behavioral deficit, a deficit that has been found to be an important signature of suicidality. Third, it extends differential activation by suggesting that, in some cases, Table 3 Specific Memories Pre-and Post-Mood Induction in Control Group and Previously Depressed Participants a b c Control MDD-no suic MDD-suic Specific memories M SD M SD M SD Pre-mood induction 13.6 2.4 12.9 2.9 14.5 2.0 Post-mood induction 13.6 3.2 12.6 2.9 13.6 3.0 Note. MDD � major depressive disorder; suic � suicidality history. Control group, n � 22. Participants with history of MDD but no suic, n � 15. Participants with both history of MDD and suic, n � 19. a c b Figure 3. Mean Means-End Problem Solving task problem-solving effectiveness pre-and post-mood induction in formerly depressed subjects with suicidal ideation scoring high (n � 10) or low in memory specificity (n � 9). the extent to which mood affects behavior can be moderated by other variables—in this case, by specificity in autobiographical memory. We predicted this on the basis that inability to retrieve event-specific knowledge would prevent a person from bringing evidence to bear of effective solutions from the past. But there remains a puzzle. Why did the previously suicidal people not also show impairments on the number of relevant means they produced in response to the MEPS items? Interestingly, our previous research with suicidal patients has also found that memory specificity is correlated more highly with effectiveness than with the number of relevant means (Pollock & Williams, 2001, p. 393). However, memory alone cannot explain these results, since the groups did not differ in their specificity of memory. What then can account for this pattern of results? Recall that the differential activation model suggests that suicidality represents one of the most severe forms of negative self-referent global patterns of thinking. Impaired problem solving is one behavioral outcome that becomes associatively linked with this negative processing pattern. In those with a history of suicidal ideation, therefore, the mood challenge begins to reactivate the entire pattern of negative thinking. It is possible that the “effectiveness” component of the MEPS is more sensitive to subtle shifts in the quality of problem solving, a shift from external task-focused problem orientation toward more internally focused, ruminative, or affect-regulating strategies. According to this account, those with a suicidal history remain able to produce some means toward a problem solution, but they tend to recast the problem in terms of coping with themselves and their emotions. Indeed, there is evidence that inducing a ruminative response style impairs problem solving in its own right (Lyubomirsky & Nolen-Hoeksema, 1995; Watkins & Baracaia, 2002). Only further research will show us if reactivation by mood challenge is equivalent (in terms of under lying process and observable outcome) to reactivation by rumination induction in previously suicidal individuals. What are the clinical implications? The results show that problem solving deteriorates in response to low mood in those who have been suicidal in the past even if it appears, before the mood challenge, that their problem solving is intact. First, it implies that psychological treatments that focus on remedying behavioral deficiencies may have a hard time engaging suicidal patients once the crisis is past not only because they may be unwilling to engage in therapy in general, but also because their problem solving, which is the focus of most psychological approaches (Hawton et al., 1998), has returned to normal or near normal levels. Second, it implies that problem solving may be impaired not only when a person comes up against a “real” problem in daily living, but also when their mood is disturbed for any reason. This implies that clinicians should be on the lookout for endogenous changes (such as sleep disturbance) that may not themselves be the result of life events, but which may exacerbate mood and lead to impaired problem solving—especially in patients who have been suicidal previously when in an episode of depression. It is important to realize the limitations of the study we have reported. First, the numbers were relatively small, so it requires replication. Small numbers introduce the possibility of Type II errors, so that we may have missed some findings that would have become apparent with larger numbers. Second, the pattern of autobiographical memory data showing no difference between previously depressed and never-depressed groups contradicts some previous research (Williams, 2004). This may represent population differences between the studies that only further research can resolve. Third, we followed previous studies that compare previously depressed with never-depressed people, and did not use “neutral mood induction” control groups. Thus, it remains possible that the observed effects on problem solving were not specifically due to reactivity to mood per se, but to other factors such as fatigue, loss of motivation, or preoccupation with the pretest assessment material. Indeed, it is interesting to note that the MDDsuic group showed mood change before the other two groups and prior to the experimental mood induction, although covarying out pre-mood-induction mood did not alter the results. Fourth, although we focused on the fact that specific memory protected against the differential activation effects in those with a history of suicidal ideation, this protective effect was shown across all groups, so this finding should be treated cautiously. Finally, our findings appear to be directly relevant to vulnerability for further suicidal behavior, but this claim would require a prospective study in which people at risk for suicidal behavior were followed up to see if their future suicidality was predicted by their response to mood challenge. However, a first test of whether differential activation theory applies in any new context requires the demonstration of a vulnerability factor that is latent in the absence of disturbed mood, but becomes manifest in the context of such disturbance. This study shows that this may indeed be the case for problem-solving effectiveness in previously suicidal patients; that such latent vulnerability is not shown by formerly depressed patients who have not been suicidal; and that the effect is partly moderated by the trait specificity in autobiographical memory. References Beautrais, A. L., Joyce, P. R., Mulder, R. T., Fergusson, D. M., Deavoll, B. J., & Nightingale, S. K. (1996). Prevalence and comorbidity of mental disorders in persons making serious suicide attempts: A case-control study. American Journal of Psychiatry, 153, 1009–1014. Beck, A. T., Brown, G. K., Steer, R. A., Dahlsgaard, K. K., & Grisham, J. R. (1999). Suicide ideation at its worst point: A predictor of eventual suicide in psychiatric outpatients. Suicide and Life-Threatening Behavior, 29, 1–9. Beck, A. T., & Steer, R. A. (1993a). Manual for the Beck Hopelessness Scale. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation. Beck, A. T., & Steer, R. A. (1993b). Manual for the Beck Scale for Suicide Ideation. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation. Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., & Brown, G. K. (1996). Manual for the BDI-II. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation. Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., & Ranieri, W. F. (1988). Scale for Suicide Ideation: Psychometric properties of a self-report version. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 44, 505. Biggam, F. H., & Power, K. G. (1999). Suicidality and the state-trait debate on problem-solving deficits: A re-examination with incarcerated young offenders. Archives of Suicide Research, 5, 27–42. Evans, J., Williams, J. M. G., O’Loughlin, S., & Howells, K. (1992). Autobiographical memory and problem-solving strategies of parasuicide patients. Psychological Medicine, 22, 399–405. First, M. B., Gibbon, M., Spitzer, R. L., & Williams, J. B. W. (1997). Structured Clinical Interview for DSM–IV Axis II Personality Disorders (SCID-II). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press. First, M. B., Spitzer, R. L., Gibbon, M., & Williams, J. B. W. (1996). User’s guide for the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM–IV Axis I Disorders. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press. Frank, E., Prien R. F., Jarrett, R. B., Keller, M. B., Kupfer, D. J., Lavori, P. W., et al. (1991). Conceptualisation and rationale for consensus definitions of terms in major depressive disorder: response, remission, recovery, relapse, and recurrence. Archives of General Psychiatry, 48, 851–855. Hawton, K., Arensman, E., Townsend, E., Bremner, S., Feldman, E., Goldney, R., et al. (1998). Deliberate self-harm: Systematic review of efficacy of psychosocial and pharmacological treatments in preventing repetition. British Medical Journal, 317, 441–447. Hawton, K., Kingsbury, S., Steinhardt, K., James, A., & Fagg, J. (1999). Repetition of deliberate self-harm by adolescents: The role of psychological factors. Journal of Adolescence, 22, 369–378. Ingram, R. E., Miranda, J., & Segal, Z. V. (1998). Cognitive vulnerability to depression. New York: Guilford Press. Joiner, T. E., Steer, R. A., Brown, G., Beck, A. T., Pettit, J. W., & Rudd, M. D. (2003). Worst-point suicidal plans: A dimension of suicidality predictive of past suicide attempts and eventual death by suicide. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 41, 1469–1480. Keller, M. B., Shapiro, R. W., Lavori, P. W., & Wolfe, N. (1982). Recovery in major depressive disorder–analysis with life table and regression models. Archives of General Psychiatry, 39, 905–910. Lau, M. A., Segal, Z. V., & Williams, J. M. G. (2004). Teasdale’s differential activation hypothesis: Implications for mechanisms of depressive relapse and suicidal behavior. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 42, 1001–1017. Lyubomirsky, S., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1995). Effects of self-focused rumination on negative thinking and interpersonal problem-solving. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 176–190. MacCallum, F., McConkey, K. M., Bryant, R. A., & Barnier, A. J. (2000). Specific autobiographical memory following hypnotically induced mood state. International Journal of Clinical & Experimental Hypnosis, 48, 361–373. Malone, K. M., Oquendo, M. A., Haas, G. L., Ellis, S. P., Li, S. H., & Mann, J. J. (2000). Protective factors against suicidal acts in major depression: Reasons for living. American Journal of Psychiatry, 157, 1084–1088. Marx, E. M., Williams, J. M. G., & Claridge, G. C. (1992). Depression and social problem solving. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 101, 78–86. McBride, C., & Cappeliez, P. (2004). Effects of manipulating valence and arousal components of mood on specificity of autobiographical memory. Psychological Reports, 95, 615–630. Molnar, B. E., Berkman, L. F., & Buka, S. L. (2001). Psychopathology, childhood sexual abuse and other childhood adversities: Relative links to subsequential suicidal behavior in the US. Psychological Medicine, 31, 965–977. Platt, J. J., Spivack, G., & Bloom, W. (Eds.). (1975). Manual for the Means-Ends Problem-Solving (MEPS) procedure: A measure of interpersonal problem-solving skill. Philadelphia: Hahnemann Medical College Hospital. Pollock, L. R., & Williams, J. M. G. (1998). Problem solving and suicidal behavior. Suicide and Life-Threatening Behavior, 28, 375–387. Pollock, L. R., & Williams, J. M. G. (2001). Effective problem solving in suicide attempters depends on specific autobiographical recall. Suicide and Life-Threatening Behavior, 31, 386–396. Rotheram-Borus, M. J., Trautman, P. D., Dopkins, S. C., & Shrout, P. E. (1990). Cognitive style and pleasant activities among female adolescent suicide attempters. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 58, 554–561. Schotte, D. E., & Clum, G. A. (1987). Problem-solving skills in suicidal psychiatric patients. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 55, 49–54. Schotte, D. E., Cools, J., & Payvar, S. (1990). Problem-solving deficits in suicidal patients — trait vulnerability or state phenomenon. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 58, 562–564. Segal, Z. V., & Ingram, R. E. (1994). Mood priming and construct activation in tests of cognitive vulnerability to unipolar depression. Clinical Psychology Review, 14, 663–695. Svaldi, J. J., & Mackinger, H. F. (2003). Mood-related changes in specificity of autobiographical recollections. Zeitschrift fur Klinische Psychologie Psychiatrie und Psychotherapie, 51, 144–157. Teasdale, J. D. (1988). Cognitive vulnerability to persistent depression. Cognition and Emotion, 2, 247–274. Watkins, E., & Baracaia, S. (2002). Rumination and social problem solving in depression. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 40, 1179–1189. Williams, J. M. G. (2004). Experimental cognitive psychology and clinical practice: Autobiographical memory as a paradigm case. In J. Yiend (Ed.), Cognition, emotion and psychopathology (pp. 251–269). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press. Williams, J. M. G., & Broadbent, K. (1986). Autobiographical memory in suicide attempters. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 95, 144–149. Williams, J. M. G., Ellis, N. C., Tyers, C., Healy, H., Rose, G., & MacLeod, A. K. (1996). The specificity of autobiographical memory and imageability of the future. Memory & Cognition, 24, 116–125. Williams, J. M. G., & Pollock, L. R. (2000). The psychology of suicidal behavior. In K. Hawton & K. van Heeringen (Eds.), International handbook of suicide and attempted suicide (pp. 79–90). New York: Wiley. Received July 17, 2004 Revision received November 10, 2004 Accepted November 18, 2004 �