Title: An ERP study for investigating the semantic combinability effect and phonetic consistency effect in reading Chinese Names: Chun-Hsien Hsu1, Jie-Li Tsai1, Chia-Ying Lee1, 2, Daisy L. Hung1 and Ovid J.L. Tzeng1, 2 Affiliation: 1. Laboratory of Cognitive Neuroscience, National Yang-Ming University, Taiwan 2. The Institute of Linguistics, Academia Sinica, Taiwan Abstract The majority of Chinese characters are phonograms, which usually contain a phonetic radical on the right and a semantic radical on the left. According to the split-fovea theory, the semantic and phonetic radicals of a central fixated phonogram may be initially projected to and processed in right and left hemispheres, respectively. Recently, a repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) study by Hsiao et al. (2006) showed that rTMS over the left occipital cortex impaired the facilitation of semantic radicals with large combinability, whereas right occipital rTMS did not. Their results suggested that for a character with a large combinability semantic radical, the reliance is more skewed to the information on the right of the character. The presented event-related potential (ERPs) study aimed to examine this hypothesis by manipulating character consistency and semantic combinability in a homophone judgment task. Character consistency was defined as whether a group of characters containing the same phonetic radical have same pronunciation. Semantic combinability was defined as the number of characters sharing the same semantic radical. If the hypothesis is true, the consistency effect shall be more salient in reading characters with high semantic combinability. The results showed significant semantic combinability effect and consistency effect on P200. Meanwhile, there was a significant consistency-by-semantic combinability interaction in N400. High consistency characters revealed greater negative of N400 than low consistency characters for reading characters with high semantic combinability, but not for those with low semantic combinability. These findings support the split-fovea theory for lexical processing. Laboratory of Cognitive Neuroscience, Library and Information Building, National Yang-Ming University N0. 155, Sec. 2, Linong st. Pei-Tou District, Taipei City, Taiwan Introduction In Chinese, approximately 80% of the characters are phonetic compounds that are made up of a semantic radical (usually on the left) and a phonetic radical (usually on the right). Furthermore, these sub-character units could contribute in recognizing Chinese phonograms by means of phonological consistency and radical combinability. The phonological relationship between phonogram and phonetic radical can be addressed by consistency. This refers to whether the pronunciation of a character agrees with those of its orthographic neighbors which, by definition, contain the same phonetic radical. While manipulating the character frequency and consistency, robust previous studies have demonstrated the frequency by consistent interaction in the naming of Chinese phonograms (Fang et al., 1986; Lee et al., 2004; Lee et al., 2005). That is, naming low consistency characters revealed longer reaction time and lower accuracy than naming high consistency characters, especially when those are low frequency characters. The radical combinability refers to the number of characters sharing same phonetic or semantic radical, therefore it can be further divided into the semantic combinability and the semantic combinability. By manipulating the radical combinability (for phonetic and semantic radical respectively) and the radical position in the character decision task, Feldman and Siok (1997) found facilitative combinability effects for both semantic and phonetic radicals, but not reliably within their positions. For phonetic radicals, the combinability effect was significant on both left and right positions. For semantic radicals, the combinability effect was significant only when the semantic radical was on the left. These findings suggested that radical function should be considered while investigating the radical processing. Recently, some studies suggested the anatomical constrain of the split-fovea phenomena, which assumes a vertical meridian split in the foveal representation and 2 the consequent contralateral projection of information in the two hemifields to the two hemispheres, should be considered in reading (see Lavidor and Walsh, 2004 for review). Therefore, the presented study aimed to further verify the rising question that how the two radicals interact in retrieving phonological and semantic knowledge. Recently, Hsiao, Shillcock and Lavidor (in press) suggested while recognizing high semantic combinability characters the impact of the phonetic radical on the semantic judgment would be increased due to a less informative semantic radical. However, their investigation was relied on the reaction time measurement that reflects the summation of all the steps prior to the behavioral response. By measuring the event-related potentials (ERPs), different cognitive operations in the brain could be separated at each millisecond from the onset of the language stimuli. For example, N170 has been associated with word form analysis; P200 has been used to index mechanisms related to feature detection (Luck and Hillyard, 1994), and the N400 is well known for being associated with lexical semantic processing (Curran et al., 1993). Current studies have found that low frequency words elicit greater negativity of N400 in comparison with high frequency words. In contrast, frequency of the first syllables of a word elicits a reversed pattern (Barber et al., 2004; Barnea and Breznitz, 1998). Furthermore, they found the onset of syllable frequency effect (around 150mse, words containing high frequency syllables produced less positive P200) was earlier than that of lexical frequency effect (around 350mse). Moreover, Lee et al (in press) and Hsu et al. (in preparation) also suggested these three components could reflect radical processing in reading Chinese also. Both consistency and semantic combinability were manipulated in the presented experiment. According to the hypothesis of split-fovea theory, the usage of phonetic radical would depend on the combinability of the semantic radical; hence there shall 3 be a consistency-by-semantic combinability interaction. Specifically, the consistency effect shall be more salient in reading characters with high semantic combinability than in reading those with low semantic combinability. Experimental Procedure Participants Thirty-one native Chinese speakers were paid for NT500 for their participation. All participants were native Chinese speakere and right-handed with normal or corrected vision. Experimental Design and materials A list of 120 Chinese phonograms, configured horizontally with a semantic radical on the left and a phonetic radical on the right, were selected from the Academia Sinica balanced corpus (1998). These phonograms were divided into four conditions by manipulating their phonological consistency (high versus low) and semantic combinability (large versus small). The index for semantic combinability and consistency was calculated based on 3697 phonograms. The semantic combinability is defined as the number of phonograms that shared a semantic radical. On the other hand, within a group of phonograms which share a same phonetic radical, let’s say characters with the same pronunciation were classified as ‘friends’. Character consistency was indexed by the relative proportion of the numbers of friends and semantic combinability. Table 1 illustrates the examples of the material and the characteristics of each condition. Each target character was paired by a homophone for the homophone judgment task. This task was aimed to ensure that the phonology of target was being processed without an overt naming response which might cause muscular noise for the EEG recording. To ensure that participants knew the correct pronunciation of the target character, every target character was followed by a 4 homophone as a “yes” trial. For the balance of response, another 120 non-homophone pairs were created for “no” trials. Procedure Participants were seated in front of a monitor at a distance of approximately 60cm in an acoustically shielded room. Each participant received 35 trials for practice and 240 randomized experimental trials in three test sessions. Participants could take a break between test sessions for as long as they needed. Total time for the experiment took about thirty minutes. Each trial started with two short vertical lines for 500msec and it was replaced by a target character for 150msec. Participants were asked to fixate the middle of the space between the two short lines all the time during the experiment and name the target character silently. The target character was replaced by a cross for 850 ms and then a probe character was presented. Participants were asked to decide whether the target character and probe character were homophones by pressing a button on the mouse as quickly and as accurately as possible. The index finger indicated “yes” and the middle finger “no”. The correctness and reaction time were recorded. After the disappearance of the probe character, a blank screen was presented for 1800 ms the next trial. EEG Recording and ERPs pre-processing The electroencephalogram (EEG) was recorded from 64 Ag/AgCl electrodes (QuickCap, Neuromedical Supplies, Sterling, USA) with a common vertex reference located between Cz and CPz. The data was re-referenced off-line to the average of the right and the left mastoids for further analysis. Vertical eye movements (VEOG) were recorded by a pair of electrodes placed on the supra- and infra-orbital ridges of the left eye. Horizontal eye movements (HEOG) were recorded by a pair of electrodes placed lateral to the outer canthus of the right and left eyes. A ground electrode was placed on 5 the forehead anterior to Fz. Electrode impedances was kept below 5 KΩ. EEG signal was continuously recorded and digitized at a rate of 1000Hz. Signal was amplified by SYNAMPS2 ® (Neuroscan, Inc.) amplifiers with the low-pass filter at DC-100Hz for off-line analysis. For the off-line analysis, the continuous wave was epoched with 100msec pre-stimulus interval and 922msec post-stimulus interval. The pre-stimulus interval was used for baseline correction. Trials contaminated by eye movement or with voltage variations larger than 60 V were rejected. The data was then band-pass filtered at 0.01~30 Hz (zero phase shift mode, 12 dB). ERPs of four conditions were computed for every participant at every electrode site by averaging over corresponding trials with correct response. 6 Result Figure 1 shows the grand averaged ERPs of four conditions at 9 representative electrodes. As shown in the figure, two major components were identified for further analysis. The first component is P200, which is a positive going wave distributed at frontal-central sites, reached its peak around 220msec. The third one is N400, which is a slowly negative going wave, peaked around 350msecand appeared at almost every electrode. Effects of consistency and combinability were assessed via comparisons of mean amplitudes in two temporal time windows of interest: P200 (210-240 ms) and N400 (300-450 ms). Differently repeated measure ANOVA was performed on these components, including factors such as consistency, semantic combinability, and the electrode in the region of interest. Both P200 and N400 were divided into the midline analysis and the lateral analysis since these components were distributed on the entire scalp. For midline analysis, the dependant variable was the mean amplitude during the latency from 300msec to 450msec; and five electrodes (FZ, FCZ, CZ, CPZ, PZ) were chosen as the electrode variable. For lateral analysis, the same dependant variable of midline analysis was used and eight electrodes (F3/4, FC3/4, C3/4, CP3/4) were chosen as the electrode variable. Accordingly, midline analysis were tested by a three-way ANOVA, which included the character consistency, the semantic combinability, and electrode as within-subject factors; and lateral analysis was tested by a four-way ANOVA, which included the character consistency, the semantic combinability, hemisphere and electrode as within-subject factors. For each ANONA, the Greenhouse-Geisser adjustment to the degrees of freedom was applied to correct for violations of sphericity associated with repeated measures. Accordingly, for all F tests with more than 1 degree of freedom in the numerator, the corrected p-value is reported. Post hoc 7 comparisons were carried out by using Tukey’s procedure. P200 The data showed a non-significant consistency effect (midline: F(1,25) = 2.821, p > 0.1; lateral: F(1,25) = 2.735, p > 0.1) and a significant semantic combinability effect (midline: F(1,25) = 9.211, p < 0.01; lateral: F(1,25) = 12.219, p < 0.01). Low combinability characters revealed more positive P200 than high combinability characters. The interaction between consistency and semantic combinability effect was not significant (midline: F(1,25) = 0.117, p > 0.1; lateral: F(1,25) = 0.188, p > 0.1). None of the interaction effect was significant (p > 0.1). The result of consistency effect here didn’t replicate previous studies (Lee et al, in press; Hsu et al., in preparation). According to these studies, the consistency effect may be localized at frontal site. Therefore, an additional ANOVA was performed by including mean amplitude of three frontal electrodes only (F3, FZ and F4). The results showed both main effect of consistency (F(1,25) = 4.815, p < 0.05) and semantic combinability (F(1,25) = 11.813, p < 0.01) were significant. Low consistency characters revealed more positive P200 than high consistency characters. However, the interaction between consistency and semantic combinability effect was still not significant (F(1,25) = 0.131, p > 0.1). N400 The data showed a non-significant consistency effect (midline: F(1,25) = 0.244, p > 0.1; lateral: F(1,25) = 0.946, p > 0.1) and a non-significant semantic combinability effect (midline: F(1,25) = 0.396, p > 0.1; lateral: F(1,25) = 1.326, p > 0.1). Interestingly, the interaction between consistency and semantic combinability effect was significant (midline: F(1,25) = 6.858, p < 0.05; lateral: F(1,25) = 6.575, p < 0.05). Post hoc test revealed that high consistency characters revealed more negative N400 8 than low consistency characters only in characters with high semantic combinability (midline: F(1,50) = 5.509, p < 0.05; lateral: F(1,50) = 6.699, p < 0.05). However, the consistency effect in low semantic combinability characters was not significant (midline: F(1,25) = 2.977, p = 0.08; lateral: F(1,25) = 1.785, p > 0.1). Discussion This study showed that low combinability characters produce more positive amplitude at the P200 time window and less negative amplitude at the N400 time window than high combinability characters. This finding is congruent with Hsu et al’s (in preparation) finding and supports that the two-stage framework for lexical access use P200 and N400 to index the early and late stage of lexical processing. According to this framework, characters with high combinability (larger orthographic neighbors) will facilitate their processing at orthographic level due to the larger orthographic activation and show less positivity in P200. On the other hand, the larger neighborhood also led to greater semantic competition and thus shows greater N400 (Holcomb et al., 2002). This is also congruent with the syllable frequency effect found in P200 and N400 (Barber et al., 2004; Barnea and Breznitz, 1998), which showed words containing high frequency syllables produced less positive P200 and more negative N400 than words containing low frequency syllables. This study also demonstrates that low consistency characters produce more positive amplitude at the P200 time window and less negative amplitude at the N400 time window than high consistency characters. This finding is congruent with our previous studies (Lee et al., in press; Hsu et al., in preparation). Characters associate with more phonological alternatives and show greater P200 positivity. The N400 indexes the semantic competition at lexical level. Given the semantic combinability was matched between high and low consistency conditions, there were more 9 homophones within a selected phonological subgroup for high consistency characters than there were for low consistency characters. Therefore, characters with high consistency would involve greater semantic competition and thus showed greater N400 than low consistency characters. Furthermore, the consistency effect was especially true of those with high semantic combinability characters. This pattern of interaction was congruent with the prediction by the theory of fovea-splitting, which is the processing on characters with less informative semantic radicals (high semantic combinability) may rely on the phonetic radicals more than those characters whose semantic combinability are small. In conclusion, this study suggests that reading Chinese phonograms involve an early selection of the phonological alternatives associated with a given phonetic radical and the late competition among the candidate within a selected phonological group. Most importantly, the lexical processing of character recognition is accomplished by the interaction of statistical mapping among orthography, phonology, and semantics. Moreover, these mapping may induce different type of cross-hemisphere processing in reading Chinese characters. Future studies may further examine the relationship between semantic and phonetic radicals and also how their function interacts with the neighborhood properties in the domain of orthography, phonology, and semantics. 10 References 1998. Academia Sinica balanced corpus (version 3). Vol., ed.^eds. Academia Sinica, Taipei, Taiwan. Barber, H., Vergara, M., Carreiras, M., 2004. Syllable-frequency effects in visual word recognition: evidence from ERPs. NeuroReport. 15, 0959-4965. Barnea, A., Breznitz, Z., 1998. Phonological and orthographic processing of Hebrew words: electrophysiological aspects. J. Genet. Psychol. 159, 492-504. Curran, T., Tucker, D.M., Kutas, M., Posner, M.I., 1993. Topography of the N400: brain electrical activity reflecting semantic expectancy. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 88, 188-209. Fang, S.P., Horng, R.Y., Tzeng, O.J.L., 1986. Consistency effects in the Chinese character and pseudo-character naming tasks. In: Linguistics, Psychology, and the Chinese Language. Vol., H.S.R. Kao, R. Hoosain, ed.^eds. Centre of Asian Studies, University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong, pp. 11-21. Feldman, L.B., Siok, W.W.T., 1997. The Role of Component Function in Visual Recognition of Chinese Characters. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 23, 776-781. Hsu, C-H, Lee, C-Y, Hung, D.L., Tzeng, O. J.L., in preparation. The Dynamic Manifestation of Orthographic Neighbors and Phonological Friends in recognizing Chinese phonograms: an ERP study. Lavidor, M, Walsh V., 2004. The nature of foveal representation. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 5, 729-35. Lee, C.-Y., Tsai, J.-L., Kuo, W.-J., Yeh, T.-C., Wu, Y.-T., Ho, L.-T., Hung, D.L., Tzeng, O.J.L., Hsieh, J.-C., 2004. Neuronal correlates of consistency and frequency effects on Chinese character naming: an event-related fMRI study. Neuroimage. 23, 1235-1245. Lee, C.-Y., Tsai, J.-L., Su, E.C.-I., Tzeng, O.J.L., Hung, D.L., 2005. Consistency, Regularity, and Frequency Effects in Naming Chinese Characters. Lang. Linguist. 6, 75-107. Lee, C.-Y., Tsai, J.-L., Chan, W.-H., Hsu, C.-H., in press. The temporal dynamics of the consistency effect in reading Chinese: An ERP study. NeuroReport. Luck, S.J., Hillyard, S.A., 1994. Electrophysiological correlates of feature analysis during visual search. Psychophysiology. 31, 291-308. 11 Table 1. Table 1.—Mean consistency index, semantic combinability, character frequency, number of strokes, semantic combinability and represented characters of four conditions. High Consistency Conditions Low Consistency High Low High Low Combinability Combinability Combinability Combinability Number of Strokes 13.37 15.10 13.30 13.20 Frequency 29.03 25.53 25.23 22.67 Consistency 0.87 0.86 0.29 0.27 Semantic Combinability 82.17 11.70 90.53 9.17 Phonetic combinability 7.17 6.87 7.87 8.40 12 Figure 1. Grand average ERPs of four conditions across 26 participants at 9 represented electrodes. 13