Supplemental methods

advertisement
1
Daytime micro-naps in a nocturnal migrant: An EEG analysis.
2
Fuchs, T., Maury, D., Moore, F. R. & Bingman, V. P.
3
4
Supplemental methods, references and figure captions
5
6
7
Animals
Adult Swainson’s thrushes (Catharus ustulatus) of mixed gender were mist-netted
8
on the Mississippi gulf coast in the fall of 2002 and 2003. Birds were temporarily housed
9
at the University of Southern Mississippi, and later transported to Bowling Green State
10
University (BGSU). Transport and housing conditions were in accordance with BGSU
11
animal care and use regulations. The thrushes were individually housed in wire cages (60
12
x 40 x 40 cm) containing 2 perches. All animals were maintained on a 12:12 light-dark
13
cycle throughout the experiments. Birds were provided with an ad libitum diet of meal
14
worms, mixed fruit, moistened monkey biscuits (Mazuri) and a vitamin supplement
15
(Eight in One Pet Products) with water available at all times.
16
Electrophysiology
17
Thrushes were bilaterally implanted with stainless steel screw electrodes over the
18
hyperpallium accessorium (dorsal “Wulst” region; 3 mm lateral to the midline) to record
19
Electro-Encephalogram (EEG). Bare wires (100μm stainless steel) were threaded under
20
the skin, above the orbital bones, to detect eye movements (EOG). A reference electrode
21
used for differential recordings was implanted along the posterior midline. Wires were
22
gathered into a small connector board (Microsystems) and the array was fixed to the skull
1
1
with dental acrylic. Surgical procedures were conducted under isoflurane anesthesia, in
2
accordance with Bowling Green State University animal care and use regulations.
3
4
After surgery animals were allowed to recover for 2 weeks. During the second
5
week of recovery, birds were introduced to the recording environment; a glass terrarium
6
(50x50x30 cm) that allowed camera access from every angle. The recording cage was
7
furnished with a single perch to make a bird’s position in the cage more predictable.
8
Birds were connected to a movable lightweight cable (Dragonfly Inc.) through their head
9
plug/connector board. Animals were allowed to adjust to the recording cable and
10
recording environment for a minimum of 3 nights before any recordings were conducted.
11
The presented data are based on daytime recordings during episodes of migratory
12
restlessness and nighttime recordings in the same birds when non-migratory. Continuous
13
daytime and nighttime recordings during the migratory season were not employed
14
because the birds, while otherwise behaving normally (posture, movement, grooming,
15
drinking and feeding), failed to show signs of nocturnal migratory restlessness under the
16
restraint of the recording cable. Consequently, under recording conditions, birds initially
17
failed to show the sleep-like, daytime behaviour, initially detected during behavioural
18
observations (Fuchs et al. 2006), presumably because of an absence of nocturnal sleep
19
loss. To address this problem, we resorted to ‘daytime only’ recordings. Birds remained
20
in their home cages at night and were moved to the recording cage by day. Birds treated
21
in this manner displayed migratory restlessness at night in their home cages and sleep-
22
related, daytime behaviour during EEG recordings.
2
1
Differentially recorded signals (EEG, EOG) first passed through a head-stage
2
amplifier constructed of JFETs, were then amplified 2000-4000 times, bandpass filtered
3
between 1 and 50 Hz (Neuralynx, Tucson, AZ), and sampled at 200 Hz (DataWave
4
Technologies, Longmont, CO). All EEG recordings were accompanied by video
5
recordings using 4 Sony camcorders and a screen splitter. While a bird’s behaviour was
6
monitored with up to 3 camcorders, one camera recorded the EEG-timer to allow for
7
accurate temporal alignment of behaviour and EEG samples. Between recording sessions
8
birds were housed in their home cages.
9
10
11
Behaviour
Captive migratory birds display the behavioural changes associated with the
12
migratory season in the form of nocturnal restlessness or “zugunruhe” (Berthold et al.
13
2000). Home cage activity was recorded continually with infrared motion detectors. A
14
bird was classified as migratory if it showed nocturnal activity on every night for at least
15
one week, and non-migratory if it did not show nocturnal activity for at least seven days.
16
Daytime behaviour was categorized according to supplemental Tab. 1 employing the
17
same behavioural criteria as in Fuchs et al. (2006). Behaviour shorter than 4 seconds was
18
not included in the analysis.
19
20
Analysis
21
The EEG analysis of mammalian sleep rests on the assumption that the intensity
22
of Δ-activity (average EEG slow-wave activity typically in the 1-4 Hz frequency range,
23
assessed as ‘power’ in the ‘Δ-band’ by power spectrum analysis; see below) accurately
3
1
reflects sleep quality (i.e. depth of sleep). The validity of this assumption is based on
2
observations in mammals showing that increased delta power corresponds to higher
3
arousal thresholds (Neckelmann & Ursin 1993) and that sleep deprivation results in
4
increased Δ-power ( the so-called ‘Δ-rebound’) during recovery sleep (reviewed in
5
Borbely & Acherman 2000). Mammals, including humans, show the most intense slow
6
wave activity at the beginning of their subjective night when ‘sleep pressure’ is
7
presumably highest. Several studies in birds indicate that this aspect of slow-wave sleep
8
is shared with mammals (Martinez-Gonzalez et al. 2008, Rattenborg et al. 2004,
9
Szymczak et al.1996, Van Luijtelaar et al. 1987). Consequently, spectral power in the Δ-
10
band was employed as a measure of sleep quality in the present study.
11
12
The video recordings were manually scored for behaviour (daytime sleep,
13
unilateral eye closure, drowsiness, alert wakefulness) according to the criteria outlined in
14
Tab. 1 and Fig. 1 of the electronic supplemental material and the corresponding sections
15
of the time stamped EEG recordings were subjected to further analysis (also see Fig. 2 of
16
the electronic supplemental material). All scoring was conducted blind to condition; i.e.,
17
behavioural state was recorded without knowledge of EEG activity. Episodes of daytime
18
drowsiness typically last several minutes, with episodes of daytime sleep and unilateral
19
eye closure nested within (Fuchs et al. 2006). Consequently the analyzed EEG samples of
20
drowsiness, unlike the samples of unilateral eye closure and daytime sleep, do not
21
correspond in length to entire bouts of drowsiness (10-20s samples, frequently repeated
22
samples within a single period of drowsiness, were taken). Nocturnal slow-wave sleep
23
samples consisted of 5 min episodes of uninterrupted sleep behaviourally identified on
4
1
the video recordings. Swainson’s thrushes display 2 nocturnal sleeping postures, front
2
sleep and back sleep (Fig. 1; Tab. 1 of the electronic supplemental material). 3 of the
3
tested birds exclusively displayed a back sleep posture during the nighttime sleep
4
samples. 2 animals exclusively showed front sleep and one animal showed front sleep
5
during the early (2h) sample and switched to a back sleep posture for the late (10 h)
6
sample (1 of the 7 animals had to be eliminated from this comparison due to a corrupted
7
nighttime video file). A similar preference of birds for either sleeping posture was also
8
observed in Swainson’s thrushes that were not implanted or restrained by recording
9
equipment (Fuchs et al. 2006).
10
EEG files were visually inspected for artifact (movement, cutting/mending) and
11
episodes of rapid eye movement (REM) sleep were removed from the night-time sleep
12
samples (characterized by clusters of eye movements on the EOG traces and a decline of
13
slow wave activity in the EEG while birds remained behaviourally asleep). The
14
processed EEG was then subjected to power spectrum analysis (Fast Fourier Transform
15
(FFT), Hanning window, 0.2 Hz frequency resolution) and average EEG power was
16
computed for frequency values between 1.4 and 4 Hz (Δ-power).
17
The power values corresponding to the analyzed behaviour (daytime sleep,
18
unilateral eye closure, drowsiness) were standardized and expressed as a percentage of Δ-
19
power during alert wakefulness for each electrode/recording session, before they were
20
subjected to statistical analysis. The alert wakefulness EEG consisted of a one minute
21
sample of alert wakefulness from the beginning and a one minute sample from the end of
22
each recording session.
23
5
1
2
Fast Fourier Transform (FFT) and Power Spectrum
FFT is a mathematical procedure that allows to describe a complex signal (an
3
EEG trace for example) by breaking it down into sine and cosine components of discrete
4
frequencies. A Power spectrum assigns a power value to each frequency component
5
(Armitage et al. 1995) which is a measure of how much energy a certain frequency
6
component contributes to the original signal or how prominent a certain frequency
7
component is in the signal. The frequency resolution of an FFT is determined by the
8
number of data-points entered into the equation (i.e. the size of the “window”) and the
9
sampling frequency of the analyzed signal. In this study a 1024 point window at a
10
sampling frequency of 200 Hz resulted in a 0.2 Hz resolution. The final power value for a
11
certain frequency represents the average of all windows applied to a given signal for that
12
frequency. FFT algorithms have a tendency to generate leakage if the signal amplitude on
13
both ends of the applied window does not equal zero (frequencies that are not
14
contributing to the original signal are detected). Consequently, windowing functions are
15
used to artificially reduce the signal amplitude to zero at a window’s edges. A variety of
16
different windows (e.g. Hanning, Hamming, Koenig) are available. In the present study a
17
Hanning window was employed. The analysis was conducted with DataWave, Neuro
18
Explorer and EEG Lab (Delorme & Makeig 2004) software. For an introduction to FFT
19
see Ramirez (1985).
20
21
22
23
Statistics
The data were tested for normality using the Shapiro-Wilk test. Normaly
distributed data were subjected to paired t-tests or within subjects repeated measures
6
1
analysis of variance (ANOVA/GLM/univariate). The distribution of the difference scores
2
for paired comparisons was also tested for normality with the Shapiro-Wilk test.
3
Violations of sphericity were compensated with the Greenhouse-Geyser correction. If
4
significant main effects were found with ANOVAs, paired t-tests were used for follow up
5
comparisons. Multiple comparisons were controlled for Type I error with the Holm
6
Simultaneous Testing Procedure (a step down procedure derived from the Bonferroni
7
method; Netter et al. 1996). Wilcoxon signed ranks tests were used to compare sleep-like
8
daytime behaviour to alert wakefulness (the 100% line in Fig. 2 A of the manuscript
9
proper). Because of the conservative nature and low power of non-parametric tests, these
10
3 comparisons were not corrected for type 1 error.
11
12
Supplemental References
13
14
Armitage, R., Hoffmann, R., Fitch, T., Morel, C.,& Bonato, R. 1995 A comparison of
15
period amplitude and power spectral analysisof sleep EEG in normal adults and
16
depressed outpatients. Psychiat. Res. 56, 245-256.
17
Berthold, P., Fiedler, W. & Querner, U. 2000 Migratory restlessness or zugunruhe in
18
birds: A description based on video recordings under infrared illumination. J. Ornithol.
19
141, 285-299.
20
Borbély, A. A. & Acherman, P. A. 2000 Sleep homeostasis and models of sleep
21
regulation. In Principles and practice of sleep medicine (eds M. H. Kryger, T. Roth,
22
W. C. Dement), pp 377 -390. Philadelphia: Saunders.
7
1
2
3
4
5
6
Delorme, A., & Makeig, S. 2004 EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of
single-trial EEG dynamics. J. Neurosci. Meth. 134, 9-21.
Neckelmann, D., Ursin, R. 1993 Sleep stages and EEG power spectrum in relation to
acoustical stimulus arousal threshold in the rat. Sleep 16, 467-77.
Netter, J., Kutner, M. H., Nachtsheim, C. J., Wasserman, W. 1996 Applied Linear
Statistical Models. USA: Irwin.
7
Ramirez, R. W. 1985 The FFT. Fundamentals and Concepts. USA: Prentice-Hall.
8
Van Luijtelaar, E. L. J. M.,van der Grinten, C. P. M., Blokhuis, H. J., & Coenen, A. M. L.
9
1987 Sleep in the domestic hen (Gallus domesticus). Physiol. Behav. 41, 409-414.
10
11
Supplemental Figure Captions
12
13
Fig. 1: Representative examples of behavioural postures. Daytime sleep and night-time
14
front sleep are behaviourally identical. Note that in the night-time back sleep posture the
15
bird turns its head and buries its beak and eyes in the scapula feathers. The back sleep
16
posture was not observed at daytime and not every bird studied showed this posture at
17
night.
18
19
Fig. 2: Analysis of sleep-like daytime behaviour. Episodes of each behaviour (here:
20
daytime sleep) were identified behaviourally in the video recordings. The corresponding
21
EEG was then selectively removed from the recordings, combined into a single file for
22
each recording session and subjected to spectral analysis (images do not correspond to
23
the EEG sample and are only intended to illustrate behavioural state).
8
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
15
16
17
18
19
20
21
22
23
9
1
2
3
4
5
10
Download