Antecedents and Outcomes of Adolescents’ Involvement with Romantic Partners Melanie Zimmer-Gembeck, Ph.D. Contact: Melanie Zimmer-Gembeck University of Minnesota Institute of Child Development 51 E River Rd Minneapolis, MN 55455 zimm0225@tc.umn.edu Poster presented at the annual meeting of the Western Psychological Association, Portland, Oregon, April 2000 Zimmer-Gembeck Page 2 Antecedents and Outcomes of Adolescents’ Involvement with Romantic Partners One developmental task of adolescence is the establishment of romantic interests, but the onset of this task (e.g., dating) varies among adolescents. Although there is evidence that these individual differences are influenced by biological and social factors (Collins & Repinski, 1995), onset does not always accurately represent patterns of involvement with romantic partners during subsequent years. Therefore, we know less about antecedents and outcomes of patterns of involvement with romantic partners across adolescence. Antecedents and outcomes of patterns of romantic involvement should be identified because this developmental task is accompanied by benefits such as friendship, social support, and peer status, but might also be accompanied by difficulties for some adolescents such as rejection, suicidal thoughts, conflict and aggression. For example, a lack of peer companionship has been associated with serious social and emotional difficulties, and low quality relationships, particularly relationships marked by aggression and maltreatment, can also be quite challenging for adolescents (Baumeister & Leary, 1995; Goodman, et al, 1993; Hartup, 1983, 1989; Leary, 1990; Parker & Asher, 1987; PirogGood & Stets, 1989; Walker, 1984, 1987). Yet, the establishment of high quality romantic relationships provides an important base of support during adulthood that has been linked to physical and emotional health. In this study, the goals were to determine some of the antecedents and outcomes of patterns of female adolescents’ involvement with romantic partners during high school. Zimmer-Gembeck Page 3 Hypotheses Predictors of Trajectories of Involvement with Romantic Partners Hypothesis 1. Early relationships with males and individual characteristics will speed up the rate with which females become involved with romantic partners Greater popularity with males, a greater number of male friends, being more attractive, and having a more mature appearance in grade 9, as well as having older romantic partners will be associated with more involvement with romantic partners throughout high school. Hypothesis 2. The level of involvement with female friends and family will slow the rate with which females become involved with romantic partners. Greater popularity with females, spending more time with female friends, a greater number of female friends, and spending more time with family in grade 9 will be associated with reduced rates of involvement with romantic partners throughout high school. Outcomes of Trajectories of Involvement with Romantic Partners Hypothesis 3. Trajectories reflecting greater involvement with romantic partners during high school will have both positive and negative outcomes. Greater involvement with romantic partners during high school will be associated with positive outcomes of reduced depression, loneliness and higher quality romantic relationships in late adolescence. Greater involvement with romantic partners during high school will be associated with the negative outcome of having lower quality female friendships in late adolescence. Zimmer-Gembeck Page 4 Method Participants Participants were 102 white females between the ages of 17 and 19 who had recently graduated from high school (M age = 18; SD = 0.7). Recruitment took place at two sites in the Northwestern United States including a large urban high school and a large urban university. Almost all participants were from low to middle income families. Self-Report Measures Self-Report Measures Dating. Dating was defined as a planned encounter that involved spending time with a person you were romantically interested in with the understanding that you were together as a dyad whether or not you were alone. A steady romantic relationship was defined as a mutual relationship that lasted longer than 2 weeks and that the interviewee felt had been a relationship that she was emotionally connected to or invested in at the time. These definitions were discussed during the interview and participants were encouraged to ask questions when unsure of who to include as romantic partners. Participants listed all of the steady romantic relationships they had before or during high school, the age of these romantic partners, when they began going steady with each person (month and year), when each relationship ended (month and year), and the length of time each relationship continued. For this study, this information was used to determine the average age of romantic partners. Friendships. Participants were asked to report the number of best female friends and very good or best male friends they had at 8 times during high school (in the fall and spring of grade Zimmer-Gembeck Page 5 9, 10, 11, and 12). For this study, this information was used to determine the number of female best friends (M = 3.0, SD = 2.4) and very close or best male friends (M = 2.3, SD = 3.5) each individual had in the ninth grade. Mature Appearance. To measure an aspect of maturity that is recognized by others, participants were also asked to report how they "looked" in ninth grade compared to other females who were the same age. This was reported on a 5-point Likert-type scale with choices ranging from “much younger than other females in the ninth grade” to “much older than other females in the ninth grade” (M = 3.1, SD = 1.1) Popularity. Popularity has been found to be only somewhat stable across time. However, the correlation between popularity with males and popularity with females has been found to be low (Franzoi, et al., 1994; Miller, 1990). Therefore, each participant in this study was asked to give ratings of her popularity with males in the ninth grade (M = 2.7, SD = 1.3), and to give ratings of her popularity with females in the ninth grade (M = 2.8, SD = 1.0). Five possible responses ranged from: “much more popular with boy (girls) than other girls in the ninth grade” to “much less popular with boys (girls) than other girls in the ninth grade.” Attractiveness. One item measured participants’ self-perceived attractiveness in the ninth grade in comparison to other females that age. Five possible responses were options ranged from “much more attractive than other girls in ninth grade” to “much less attractive than other girls in ninth grade” (M = 2.6, SD = 0.9). Emotional Tone and Peer Relations. Emotional tone and peer relations were measured with two subscales of the Self-Image Questionnaire for Young Adolescents (SIQYA; Peterson, et al., 1984). Emotional tone was the average of 11 items that measured lack of anxiety, sadness, and Zimmer-Gembeck Page 6 loneliness (alpha = .88, M = 4.6, SD = 0.9). The peer relation score was an average of 10 items that measured sociability and ease of making friends (alpha = .86, M = 4.6, SD = 0.9). Loneliness. Loneliness was measured with the UCLA loneliness scale (Russell, et al., 1980). This scale is a sum of 20 items that reflect feelings of being remote from others, and lacking in social companionship and meaningful relationships (alpha = 90, M = 37.8, SD = 10.0). Positive qualities of friendships and romantic relationships. The Networks of Relationships Inventory (NRI; Furman & Buhrmester, 1985; Furman, not dated) was used to assess twelve qualities of current (or most recent past) peer relationships including: (a) reliable alliance (a lasting dependable bond), (b) admiration (enhancement of worth), (c) affection, (d) companionship, (e) instrumental aid, (f) intimacy, (g) nurturance, (h) satisfaction, (i) conflict, (j) punishment, and (k) antagonism, and (l) relative power in the relationship. All subscales were completed about the current “very best” female friend and current (or most recent past) romantic partner. Seven of the subscales (in italics) were combined to form a measure of positive qualities of peer relationships (alpha = .89 when reporting about best female friendships and alpha = .94 when reporting about romantic partner). M positive qualities in current female friendships = 3.6 (0.6) M positive qualities in current (or most recent past) romantic relationship = 3.4 (0.9). M Current romantic partner only (n = 58) positive qualities = 3.7 (0.7) Interview Eight estimates of time spent (in the fall and spring of the grade 9, 10, 11, and 12) with best female friends, romantic partners, and family were collected retrospectively. After focusing on each point in time, adolescents were asked to "estimate on a scale of 0 to 100, where 0 is no time Zimmer-Gembeck Page 7 at all, 50 is about one-half of your free-time, and 100 is every minute of free-time you had, how much time you spent with your best female friends?" Participants were given a visual-analog scale and asked to write a number to indicate the time they select. This procedure was repeated asking about time spent with the steady boyfriend (romantic partner) and family. Numbers selected were summarized and any discrepancies were discussed with the participant. Fall and spring ratings from grade 9 were averaged to form the rating of time spent with social partners in grade 9 used in this study. Results Correlations between Time Spent with Romantic Partners, Antecedents and Outcomes For reference, Table 1 shows the correlations between antecedents and outcomes. Table 2 shows correlations of the amount of time spent with romantic partners in each grade of high school with all hypothesized antecedents and outcomes. Females who were more popular with males in grade 9 and had more male friends in grade 9 also spent more time with romantic partners in each grade in high school. Physical attractiveness, mature appearance, and popularity with females in grade 9 had stronger positive correlations with the amount of time spent with partners in the early years of high school, while having older partners predicted spending more time with partners in the later years of high school. Spending more time with romantic partners in the later years of high school predicted a more positive emotional tone (less sadness and nervousness), reduced loneliness, and higher quality romantic relationships in late adolescence. Zimmer-Gembeck Page 8 Identifications of Trajectories of Time Spent with Romantic Partners A k-means clustering algorithm (Hartigan, 1975) was used to classify participants based upon repeated measures of the amount of leisure time spent with romantic partners during high school and differences between patterns of time spent with partners. K-means clustering was used to minimize within cluster variance on criterion variables and between cluster differences, while allowing all profiles to be considered. This algorithm makes no attempt to obtain groups of similar size. The criterion variables used to identify clusters were average time spent with romantic partners in 9th, 10th, 11th, and 12th grades. The mean square ratio statistic (R; Hartigan, 1975) was used to guide the selection of the appropriate number of clusters. To justify selecting a larger number of clusters, R was required to be at least 10. A 3-cluster solution was chosen that identified a “low involvement” cluster of 59 females (58%), an “increasing involvement” cluster of 21 females (21%), and a “high continuous involvement” cluster of 22 participants (22%). Average trajectories of time spent with romantic partners in each cluster are shown in Figure 1. Hypothesis 1. Early relationships with males and individual characteristics will speed up the rate with which females become involved with romantic partners As predicted, greater popularity with males, a greater number of male friends, and older male partners sped up involvement with romantic partners in high school (see Table 3). Females in the high continuous involvement cluster were the most popular with males in grade 9 and had more male friends in grade 9 when compared to the low involvement and the increasing involvement clusters. Both the high continuous involvement and the increasing involvement clusters dated older males when compared to the low involvement cluster. However, Zimmer-Gembeck Page 9 attractiveness and maturity of appearance did not significantly differ when comparing these three clusters. Hypothesis 2. Involvement with female friends and family will slow the rate with which females become involved with romantic partners On the contrary, greater popularity with females and spending more time with female friends in grade 9 actually sped up involvement with romantic partners during high school (see Table 3). However, it did appear that the group who increased their involvement with partners during high school spent the greatest amount of time with female friends in grade 9, while the low involvement and high continuous involvement clusters spent less time with friends. Number of female friends and the amount of time spent with family in grade 9 did not differ when comparing the clusters. Hypothesis 3. Trajectories reflecting greater involvement with romantic partners during high school will have both positive and negative outcomes Positive outcomes Average positive emotional tone (reduced sadness and nervousness), loneliness and quality of romantic relationships in late adolescence differed when comparing the three clusters (see Table 3). Females in the increasing and high continuous involvement clusters had more positive emotional tone, reported higher quality romantic relationships in late adolescence, and were less lonely than the low involvement cluster. In addition, females in the increasing and high continuous involvement clusters reported that they were more sociable in late adolescence. Zimmer-Gembeck Page 10 Negative outcomes No negative outcomes of high continuous and increasing involvement with romantic partners were identified (see Table 3). Contrary to prediction, the high continuous involvement cluster did not report lower quality female friendships in late adolescence. Summary and Conclusions Lower attractiveness and popularity early in high school, as well as having fewer close male and female friends results in a pathway of reduced involvement with romantic partners that is associated with reduced emotional and social functioning later in life when compared to other females. Females who had low involvement with romantic partners throughout high school were less attractive, looked younger, were less popular with males and females, spent less time with female friends, and had fewer male friends in grade 9 when compared to females who increased time with partners or had high continuous involvement with partners during high school. After high school, the low involvement group was more sad, nervous, and lonely, less sociable and competent with peers, and reported lower positive qualities in romantic relationships. Although no negative outcomes of early initiation of romantic relationships were identified in this study, it is still likely that those who are involved in romantic relationships early and continuously may experience some difficulties along the way. For example, in other research, individuals who began dating early (before age 14) were more likely to use alcohol and drugs, and to participate in delinquent behavior (Thomas & Hsiu, 1993; Wright, 1982), and others report that females who date early engage in problem behavior because they date older males Zimmer-Gembeck Page 11 and become associated with an older peer group (Stattin et al., 1989). This study also found that females more involved in romantic relationships dated older males, on average. Future research should extend this study to include a wider range of outcomes such as problem behaviors, dating aggression, identity development, and vocational development. Focusing on girls, Douvan and Adelson (1966) wrote that, “once a girl begins to date, her interests change and she finds little to share with girlfriends who have not yet entered the dating phase” (p.215). However, this study indicates that it is the females who have low involvement with romantic partners as other are forming these relationships that are experiencing the most emotional and social difficulties. In this study, popularity with females in grade 9, but not the number of best female friends, was higher among females with more involvement with romantic partners during high school. Some researchers have suggested that friendships are a context in which adolescents learn about and practice intimacy, reciprocity, nurturance, conflict resolution, and power (Douvan and Adelson, 1966; Furman & Wehner, 1997; Sullivan, 1953). In other words, friendships may be a template for the qualities young people will search for in romantic relationships. Additionally, friends may provide concrete help with the specifics of the task of romantic relationships, such as advice on selection, feedback about choices, support during conflict and break-ups, and encouragement of dissolution of unhealthy relationships. In this study is appears that relations with females are influences on trajectories of involvement with romantic partners. Future research is needed to understand these changes and the variety of strategies adolescents use to accomplish the task of developing high quality romantic relationships, as well as to understand the ways to accomplish this task that result in optimal identity development, minimize Zimmer-Gembeck Page 12 internalizing and externalizing disorders, and promote a variety of healthy, high quality social relationships. References Baumeister, R.F., & Leary, M.R. (1995). The need to belong: Desire for interpersonal attachments as a fundamental human motivation. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 497-529. Collins, W.A., & Repinski, D.J. (1995). Relationships during adolescence: Continuity and change in interpersonal perspective. In R. Montemayor, G.R. Adams, & T.P. Gullotta. Personal relationships during adolescence, Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Douvan, E. & Adelson, J. (1966). The adolescent experience. New York: Wiley & Sons. Franzoi, S.L., Davis, M.H., & Vasquez-Suson, K.A. (1994). Two social worlds: Social correlates and stability of adolescent status groups. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 462-473. Furman, W., & Buhrmester, D. (1985). Children's perceptions of the personal relationships in their social networks. Developmental Psychology, 21, 1016-1024. Furman, W. (not dated). Instructions for scoring Network of Relationships Inventory. Denver, CO: University of Denver. Furman, W., & Wehner, E.A.. (1997). Adolescent romantic relationships: A developmental perspective. . In S. Shulman & W.A. Collins (Eds.). Romantic relationships in adolescence: Developmental perspectives (pp. 21-36), San Francisco,: Jossey-Bass. Zimmer-Gembeck Page 13 Goodman, L.A., Koss, M.P., Fitzgerald, L.F., Russo, N.F., & Keita, G.P. (1993). Male violence against women: Current research and future directions. American Psychologist, 48, 1054-1058. Hartigan, J. (1975). Clustering algorithms. Wiley: New York. Hartup, W.W. (1983). Peer relations. In E,M, Hetherington (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology (Vol. 4): Socialization, personality, and social development (pp. 103-198). New York: Wiley. Hartup, W.W. (1989). Social relationships and their developmental significance. American Psychologist, 44, 120-126. Leary, M.R. (1990). Responses to social exclusion: Social anxiety, jealousy, loneliness, depression, and low self-esteem. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 9, 221-229. Miller, K.E. (1990). Adolescents' same-sex and opposite-sex peer relations: Sex differences in popularity, perceived social-competence, and social cognitive skills. Journal of Adolescent Research, 5, 222-241. Parker, J.G., & Asher, S.R. (1987). Peer relations and later personal adjustment: Are low-accepted children at risk? Psychological Bulletin, 102, 357-389. Peterson, A.C., Schulenberg, J.E., Abramowitz, R.H., Offer, D., & Jarcho, H.D. (1984). A self-image questionnaire for young adolescents (SIQYA): Reliability and validity studies. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 13, 93-111. Pirog-Good, M.A., & Stets, J.E. (1989). Violence in dating relationships: Emerging social issues. New York: Praeger. Zimmer-Gembeck Page 14 Russell, D., Peplau, L.A., & Cutrona, C.E. (1980). The revised UCLA loneliness scale: Concurrent and discriminant validity evidence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 472-480. Stattin, H., Gustafson, S.B., & Magnusson, D. (1989). Peer influences on adolescent drinking: A social transition perspective. Journal of Early Adolescence, 9, 227-246. Sullivan, H.S. (1953). The interpersonal theory of psychiatry. New York: Norton. Thomas, B.S., & Hsiu, L.T. (1993). The role of selected risk factors in predicting adolescent drug use and its adverse consequences. The International Journal of Addictions, 28, 1549-1563. Walker, L.E. (1984). The Battered Woman Syndrome. New York: Springer. Walker, L.E. (1987). Assessment and intervention with battered women. In P.A. Keller & S.R. Heyman (Eds.), Innovations in clinical practice: A sourcebook, Vol. 6. Sarasota, FL: Professional Research Exchange. Wright, L.S. (1982). Parental permission to date and its relationship to drug use and suicidal thoughts among adolescents. Adolescence, 17, 409-418.