Reading Psychology, 27:263–311, 2006 Copyright © 2006 Taylor & Francis Group, LLC ISSN: 0270-2711 print / 1521-0685 online ^"^ D^i i^arlr^d s^ ^7 mm" P DOI: 10.1080/02702710600846803 WRITTEN LANGUAGE IS AS NATURAL AS SPOKEN LANGUAGE: A BIOLINGUISTIC PERSPECTIVE P. G. AARON Indiana State University, Terre Haute, Indiana, USA R. MALATESHAJOSHI Texas A&M University, College Station, Texas, USA A commonly held belief is that language is an aspect of the biological system since the capacity to acquire language is innate and evolved along Darwinian lines. Written language, on the other hand, is thought to be an artifact and a surrogate of speech; it is, therefore, neither natural nor biological. This disparaging view of written language, even though propounded by some renowned linguists and biologists, has not gained universal acceptance. Dissenters such as linguists from the Prague circle who claim that written language is an independent system that deserves a status equivalent to that of spoken language have developed their argument along linguistic parameters. The present article also endeavors to show that written language is as natural as spoken language but does so from a biolinguistic perspective. Biolinguistics defines language as a product of biological adaptation in the Darwinian sense (Givon, 2002) and considers language to be innate and species specific (Jenkins, 2000). The present article presents evidence to show that, similar to spoken language, written language has adaptive value, evolved over time, and is relatively independent of spoken language. The Egyptian hieroglyphic writing, which has a history of about 4,000 years, is used for examining the proposition that written language evolved along Darwinian lines as much as spoken language did. It is concluded that written language is yet another manifestation of the natural endowment of the human mind and may not be treated as a proxy for speech. The educational implication is that, in literacy instruction, written language should be given as much importance in today’s schools as elements of spoken language, such as phoneme awareness and phonological awareness. The present article proposes the thesis that language is a human endowment that can express itself in more than one form and that spoken language and written language are two manifestations of the human mind. This view is somewhat unorthodox and is not Address correspondence to P. G. Aaron, Department of Educational and School Psychology, College of Education, Indiana State University, Terre Haute, IN 47809. E-mail: paaron@isugw.indstate.edu 263 264 P. G. Aaron and R. M. Joshi endorsed by many linguists who consider that writing is not a natural endowment but is a human invention and a cultural artifact. Such a view has historical roots. Darwin, for instance, wrote in his The Descent of Man (1871) that man has an instinctive tendency to speak, as we see in the babble of young children, while no child has instinctive tendency to write. Expressing his opinion about written language, the linguist Saussure (1916) declared that the sole reason for the existence of writing is to represent speech. Drawing a distinction between spoken and written languages, Bloomfield (1933) asserted “writingisnot language, but merelyawayofrecord-ing speech by visible marks” (p. 21). More recently, Pinker and Bloom (1990), after remarking that the use of natural language belongs more to the study of human biology than human culture, stressed the contrast between the two modes of communication by stating that “language is not like writing or the wheel” (p. 707). The belief that written language is an artifact and invention is so old and buttressed by the weight of authority and tradition that it has come to be accepted as dogma without critical examination. In spite of the impressive array of authorities who have relegated written language to a lower station, the view that written language is there to merely garnish spoken language is not agreed upon by all contemporary scholars. An occasional dissenting voice could be heard even from the distant past. Samuel Johnson (1755), for instance, collected samples for his dictionary from writers but not from everyday English speakersbecause the speechofthe working and merchant classes, as he put it, is casual and mutable, is a fugitive cant which is always in a state of increase or decay and therefore must be suffered to perish with other things unworthy of preservation (Stubbs, 1980). During recent times, the view that spoken and written languages are distinct representations of inner language has been advocated by linguists such as Pulgram (1965) and Vachek (1973, 1989) of the Prague Linguistic Circle. Vachek (1973, 1989) has argued in favor of the independent status of written language on the grounds that changes in the writing system do not always parallel those in the spoken language and that the influence of the two systems on each other is mutual. The kinship between spoken and written languages is depicted by Goody (1977) in the form of a triangular connection between the three elements, namely language, speech, and writing. He further notes that recognition of this relationship has been impeded by a neglect Written Language Is Natural 265 of the study of developmental and evolutionary factors that are associated with written language, an oversight that the present article attempts to amend. The long-held belief that written language is subservient to oral language and that written language is parasitic upon oral language is finally being questioned, and the “great divide” between literacy and oracy is becoming increasingly less convincing once context and environmental factors are taken into account (Pontecorvo & Orsolini, 1996; Roberts & Street, 1997) The field of linguistics is divided over the issue of how exactly to characterize the nature of language and in what terms it should be defined (Christiansen & Kirby, 2003). Many linguists have adopted Saussure’s (1916) langue and parole distinction and view the abstract inner language faculty to be distinct from behav-iorally observable spoken language. Notwithstanding this theoretical distinction, linguists have utilized observations made on spoken language to draw inferences regarding the nature of the inner language. Examples of linguists who have relied on spoken language to examine inner language are Humboldt (1931), Givon (1979), Bickerton (1995), Hurford and Kirby (1995), Miller and Weinert (1998), Aiello (1998), Pinker (1998), and Kalmar (1985). These citations make it apparent that spoken language is the window through which observations about inner language are made. In contrast, written language has been seldom exploited as a tool for exploring the nature of inner language because of the belief that it is an invention and an artifact. Based on studies of spoken language, linguists have relied on two sets of observations to support their assertion that language as manifest in its spoken form is a natural endowment of human beings. First, language is claimed to be a biological system since it meets certain criteria such as heritability, innateness, and modularity. In addition, the observation that almost every member of the society can acquire adequate linguistic capability without strenuous effort and that there appears to be a critical period for acquiring language has also been used to support the notion that language is a biological phenomenon. The second set of observations that supports the proposition that languageisnaturalisthat the evolutionaryhistoryoflanguages is believed to reveal certain features propounded by Darwinian theory such as variation, adaptation, and natural selection. After examining language on the basis of these criteria, several linguists 266 P. G. Aaron and R. M. Joshi (e.g., Lieberman, 1984; Muller, 1996; Pinker & Bloom, 1990) have concluded that language satisfies these evolutionary requirements and, therefore, should be considered to have followed the course of organic evolution. The purpose of the present article is to present a case for treating written language as a biolinguistic system that is as natural as spoken language and to submit evidence to show that written language has a status of its own. An answer to the question “To what extent is written form of language as natural as language in its spoken form?” depends on the degree to which features of written language match the reported biological and evolutionary features of spoken language. For this reason, a description of the theoretical framework that has been used to study the hypothetical nature of linguistic evolution is presented first; subsequently, this framework is used for evaluating the status of written language as a natural system. Theories of Language Evolution Evolutionary psychologists and linguists may be united in their claim that language is a natural system, but they are not all agreed as to which theoretical framework should be utilized for explaining the origin and evolution of language. The leading theoretical candidates are (a) a theory of grand change which resulted in neural structures capable of language production, which, in turn, created a disjunction between human language and animal communication (e.g., Language Acquisition Device, Chomsky, 1975); (b) a theory of gradual emergence of phenotypic changes over a period of thousands of years caused by small but numerous mutations leading to adaptation through natural selection (e.g., neo-Darwinism, Lieberman, 1984; Muller, 1996; Pinker & Bloom, 1990); (c) a theory that considers language as the product of interaction between genes and systems outside the genome including sociocultural environment (e.g., Neophenogenesis, Johnston & Gottlieb, 1990; Gottlieb, 1991; Velichkovsky, 1996); and (d) a theory of “neoneo-Darwinism,” which proposes that of the countless potential genetic features available in the genome, the ones that have adaptive environmental advantages are selectively expressed (e.g., Exapta-tion, Piattelli-Palmarini, 1989; Jenkins, 2000). Written Language Is Natural 267 Theory of Grand Change Chomsky (1975, 1988), after defining universal grammar as the system of principles and rules that are properties of all human languages, proposes that language is innate, a biological necessity, and species specific, with implications for genetic determination. According to Gould and Marler (1987), such a view implies that language is an evolutionary accident, and if it happened, it might have been a mutation resulting in a major neurologic upheaval, an event comparable to the “Big Bang” theory of the origins of the cosmos. Such an orientation explains evolution in terms of a putative major mutation or mutations that might have occurred in spurts after long periods of quiescence (punctuated equilibria, Eldredge, 1989; Gould, 1984). In the present article, we discuss this “macro-mutation” view of the origin of language within the framework of macro-evolution. Theory of Gradual Change Approaching phylogeny of language from a neo-Darwinian perspective, Pinker and Bloom (1990) propose that language has been shaped by innumerable but gradual gene-based pheno-typic changes acted on by natural selection. Such a process would involve random mutations, adaptation, selection, and genetic transmission of these changes. Authors such as Maryanski (1996) and Rumbaugh and Savage-Rumbaugh (1996), who are sympathetic to this view, argue that language is the product of evolutionary trends because discernible traces of language acquisition capacities could be seen in nonhuman primates, notably, the chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes). There are, however, critics of this adaptationist view too (e.g., Gould and Lewontin, 1979). The theory that explains evolution in terms of phenotypic variation, minute mutations, gradual changes, and the associated adaptive advantages that lead to the survival of such changes, and their genetic transmission to the next generation is described within the microevolutionary perspective in the present article. 268 P. G. Aaron and R. M. Joshi Theory of Interaction between Genes and the Environment The gene-environment interaction view posits that social demands and cultural factors might have played a crucial role in providing an environment for the innate language capacity to express itself and subsequently propelled language evolution to an unparalleled level of sophistication (Velichkovsky, 1996; Deacon, 1996). This perspective is described as “neophenogenesis,” a term borrowed from biology. “Neophenogenesis” refers to the genesis or emergence of new phenotypes that persist over time and are brought about by genetic variation and extra-genetic factors present in the environment (Johnston & Gottlieb, 1990). This is in stark contrast to neoDarwinian theory, which views phenotypic change to be strictly due to changes in genes, which, being insulated, are impervious to environmental agents. Whenappliedtolanguage,neophenogeneticprinciplemeans that evolution of language has been made possible by a combination of genetic potential and environmental demands. According to Foley and Van Valin (1984), whose theory is functionalist in nature, language is a product of socio-linguistic competence because pragmatic principles, discourse competence, and social interactions all shape language. Evidence for the importance of social experience in language development comes from the observation that some Australian languages such as Warlbiri and Papago have no formal rules for phrase-structure and no identifiable transformational rules (Hale, 1992). The syntactical structure of these so-called free word order languages is characterized not by word order,orany formal grammar, butbycontext and the pragmatic nature of discourse. Therefore, Papago- and Warlbiri-speaking children may not be born with an innate syntax acquisition device but infer syntax of the local language from contextual and pragmatic factors. The theory that language acquisition and use are the result of interaction between innate capacity and socio-cultural factors is presented as the neophenogenetic perspective in this article. Theory of Selective Expression of Latent Language Finally, applying the “exaptation” hypothesis to language, PiattelliPalmarini (1989) has argued that all possible elements of language competence are available in the genome but the traits that survive Written Language Is Natural 269 and evolve are determined by a fit between these innate elements and environmental demands. Furthermore, traits that were originally mediating one function may come to be utilized for another function (Jenkins, 2000). For instance, the neural potential for executing motor gestures eventually might have come to be used for language production. This brief discussion of the theories of linguistic evolution shows that there is not much consensus about the nature of evolution of language. Of the four theoretical possibilities, studies that have investigated language from macroevolutionary, microevolu-tionary, and neophenogenetic perspectives have received the most attention and have generated a substantial corpus of empirical knowledge. For this reason, in the present article, evolution of language in its spoken and written forms will be examined from these three evolutionary perspectives; namely, macroevolution, mi-croevolution, and neophenogenesis. In the following section, a brief description of the history of written language systems is presented in the hope that it will facilitate the evaluation of the status of written language as compared to spoken language. History of Written Language Systems Archaeologicalfindingsindicate thatthefirst known writing system was in use at about 3500 B.C. in Sumer in Mesopotamia, a region in present-day southern Iraq. Recent excavations in widespread areas such as Uruk and Nineveh in Iraq, Susa in Iran, and Tell Brak and Habuba Kabira in Syria have also unearthed tablets with early inscriptions, which indicate that writing might not be the invention of a single Sumerian genius living in Mesopotamia, but was accomplished over widely separated areas (Walker, 1987). There is also a belief among scholars that writing systems emerged in China and among the Mayans very close in time (Coe, 1999; Gelb, 1974; Sampson, 1985). Evidence shows that the early Mesopotamian script is derived from an archaic counting device developed during the fourth millennium B.C. (Jean, 1992; Robinson, 1995; Schmandt-Basserat, 1992), which suggests that writing had an origin independent of spoken language. Records of commercial transactions in Mesopotamia were maintained in the form of solid clay tokens molded in the form of cones, spheres, and disks, each token 270 P. G. Aaron and R. M. Joshi representing a certain type of commodity. Soon, these forms of representations gave way to pictorial representations that were scratched on the surface of wet clay tablets. Subsequently, these drawings were stylized and became iconic figures, which bore only vague resemblance to the objects they represented. These early iconic writings were usedasmeaning symbols and, therefore, could not represent grammatical elements. This limitation was eventually overcome by making these icons stand for the syllables in spoken language. For instance, the Sumerian word for barley is se; hence, the drawing of a stalk of barley came to represent the syllabic sound se. Icons such as barley were eventually reduced to mere strokes, which became the cuneiform signs. Since the Sumerian language was predominantly monosyllabic (Michalowski, 1996), each cuneiform sign came to represent a single morpheme. While it is possible that the inspiration for Egyptian writing might have come from the Sumerians, the Egyptian writing system shares few characteristics with Mesopotamian cuneiform writings. Early Mesopotamian scripts were primarily logo-syllabic, whereas the Egyptian hieroglyphs were logo-consonantal. In his monograph about the origins of Egyptian writing, Arnett (1982) has presented evidence to show that the Egyptian hieroglyphs antedate dynastic Egyptians and the Sumerians. In its initial stages, the Egyptian writing system was primarily in the form of icons (Arnett, 1982). Subsequent evolution of Egyptian writing was marked by the use of symbols to represent consonants and further aided by the implementation of the rebus principle, which utilizes the drawing of an object to represent the name of the object rather than the object itself. A great breakthrough, as it was, the rebus principle nevertheless is useful in representing only a handful of words and becomes overburdened when all the words in a language are to be represented that way. A solution to this problem is to represent, in picture form, the first one or two consonants of words. Other rebuses could be used for the residual consonants in words. Thus, representational figures of objects came to represent consonants. This led to the development of mono, bi, and triconso-nantal hieroglyphs, depending upon the number of consonants each symbol represented. However, when icons of objects were used in hieroglyphic writing, they were not always used for representing the consonantal sounds; often, the icons represented the objects themselves (ideograms); at other times, they were used as Written Language Is Natural 271 FIGURE 1 Examples of phonetic and ideographic signs used in Egyptian hieroglyphic writing. Note: The signs M, and <S> do not represent a bird and an eye but the sounds /m/ and /ir/. In contrast, the ideographic signs *\ and C^HBrf represent objects or an idea, not the sounds. case- markers for gender and number (determinatives) (Figure 1). Thus, Egyptian hieroglyphic writing is a mixture of consonantal signs, ideograms, as well as logograms. As Gardiner (1969) observes, Egyptian hieroglyphic writing is “a picture writing eked out by phonetic elements” (p. 5). An almost full-fledged alphabetic writing system emerged in Greece about five centuries before Christ. It is generally thought that the ancestry of the Greek alphabetic system could be traced to the Phoenician script, which, in turn, might have been influenced by Egyptian, Aramaic, or Cananite-Semitic consonantal writing. The Greeks took the consonantal script and supplied a set of vowels, turning it into a full-fledged alphabetic system. The origins of the Chinese writing system is lost in antiquity, but we know that Chinese orthography has continued almost unaltered for nearly 2000 years. Chinese orthography, very much like the Egyptian system, is a mixture of iconic, determinative, and phonetic elements. It is therefore described as a logographic system frozen in its transition to a phonetic system. As this brief discussion indicates, socio-cultural factors such as economic necessities of the society and environmental factors such as the availability of materials such as clay and papyrus played an important role in the evolution of writing systems. In addition, similar to spoken language, writing systems have also evolved, not necessarily in a linear fashion from icons to alphabet, but have 272 P G. Aaron and R. M.Joshi progressed in different directions such as syllabic writing, morphemic writing, and morphophonemic writing. In order to address the issue of whether written language is as natural as spoken language or not, the evidence available from the studies of biology and evolution of language both in its spoken form and written form will be examined from the “macroevolutioninnateness,” “microevolution-neo-Darwinian,” and “neopheno-genetic interactive” perspectives. In this article, all three aspects of language, viz., phonology, morphology, and syntax, are examined for naturalness. In written language, orthography takes the place of phonology. Egyptian hieroglyphic writing, which has an almost 4000 years of uninterrupted history, provides a unique opportunity for examining the evolution of the writing system. Is Written Language as Natural as Spoken Language? A Macro-Evolutionary Question In the following section, the so-called evolution of spoken language is examined with reference to the principles of macroevolution, microevolution, and neo-phenogenesis. Subsequently, written language is compared with spoken language within the framework of these three perspectives. The article concludes with a discussion of the status of written language in relationship to spoken language along with the potential contribution of the history of development of written language to literacy instruction and evolutionary psychology. Biological Nature of Language: Macro-Evolutionary, Innateness Perspective A list of principles that support the belief that language faculty is biological and natural includes the following. • Language is species specific and there is a clear disjunction between human language and animal communication. • The similarity seen among languages at the “deep level” suggests that all human beings are endowed with an innate linguistic ability (universal grammar). • Language is universal in the sense all societies have language. Written Language Is Natural 273 • Children acquire language spontaneously without the help of formal instruction. • The language model provided by adults is far from being ideal. Nevertheless, children acquire proper linguistic skills. • Language is a modular skill because brain impairment can affect language skill, leaving other cognitive abilities intact. • Since some language disorders appear to be genetically determined, the heritability of language is high. Biological Nature of Language: Micro-Evolutionary, Neo-Darwinian Perspective A list of principles used for supporting the view that language faculty has evolved along neo-Darwinian lines includes the following: • Small variations in language occurred over a long period of time. When these variations reached a “critical mass” they resulted in significant changes that had adaptative advantages to the language user. • There are parallels between the early stages of language development in children and the evolution of language. • The anatomy of vocal tract and speech perception capabilities are synchronous with language skill. • The ontogeny and phylogeny of language present evidence of neoDarwinian features of evolution. • Language is as old as humankind. Interactive Nature of Language: Functionalism, Neophenogenetic Perspective A list of principles for thinking that language faculty is a product of an interaction between innate potential and socio-cultural factors is as follows: • Genes do not operate in a vacuum. The development of an or ganism and the faculty such as language is determined by inter actions among genes, hormones, physical factors, sensory expe rience, and social interactions. 274 P G. Aaron and R. M.Joshi • Communication through language is a social act that involves more than one person. Consequently, the speaker and listener influence each other. Biological Nature of Language: Evidence from a Macro-Evolutionary, Innateness Perspective Language Is Species Specific and There Is a Clear Disjunction between Human Language and Animal Communication SPOKEN LANGUAGE There is a good deal of debate as to whether nonhuman primates have the potential to acquire the rudiments of language. The question of whether chimpanzees could be taught elements of human language is an important one for psycholinguists because it can tell whether human language could be acquired without the species-specific innate linguistic ability. Those who have tried this experiment on chimpanzees and apes have reported positive but limited success (e.g., Gardner & Gardner, 1969; Premack, 1976; Rumbaugh & SanageRumbaugh, 1996). Critics, however, have noted that it may be possible to train an ape to use language much the way a dog can be trained to walk on its hind legs, but neither achievement will have any bearing on the question of whether these performances are equivalent to the human propensity (Fodor, Bever, & Garrett, 1974). WRITTEN LANGUAGE There is little or no evidence that nonhuman primates show any inclination or capability of communicating their needs and intentions in any form other than through vocalization and gestures. Thus, writing will qualify as a species-specific skill. The Similarity Seen among Languages at the “Deep” Level Suggests that Human Beings Are Endowed with an Innate Linguistic Ability (Universal Grammar) SPOKEN LANGUAGE The argument that human language is innate is based on the belief that a similar grammatical machinery is used in all the world’s languages and, therefore, a “universal grammar” underlies Written Language Is Natural 275 the human language instinct (Pinker, 1994). The essential feature of universal grammar is syntax (Bickerton, 2003). The rapidity and the universality with which children begin to use language has led linguists to propose that children have an adult-like syntactic competence. This syntactic capability, therefore, is believed to be innate and uniquely human. Critics, however, point out that whereas word order is a crucial property of syntax in some languages, such as English, it is not so in some Austronesian languages such as Acehnese, Walbiri, and Papago, in which context facilitates comprehension (Van Valin & LaPolla, 1997). These “variations” seen in the nature of syntax among languages has led some linguists to point out the laxity surrounding the concept of universal grammar (e.g., Bichakjian, 1989; Deacon, 1997; Givon, 1979; Ruhlen, 1994). Furthermore, Tomasello (2000) cites evidence to show that children learn imitatively pieces of language patterns that do not necessarily represent the elemental units of universal grammar. Acquisition of syntax, therefore, proceeds in a piecemeal fashion as children continue to express their communicative intentions (Tomasello, 2003). With these caveats in mind, the issue of the presence of underlying uniform principles in written languages will be examined. WRITTEN LANGUAGE The syntactical properties of written sentences are far more formal than those of spoken language. However, we will gain little by comparing sentences of these two genres since an argument can be raised that the syntactical properties of written language can be attributed to the possibility that written language imitates spoken language. In contrast to the sentence, spelling is a distinct feature of written language and lends itself to an examination of rules that govern the structural aspect of written language. If syntax is defined as the structure of sentences, written spelling can be viewed as the syntax of the written word, which is governed by three canonic principles that exert a constraining influence on the structure of the written word. The three canonic principles that impose constraints on orthographic structure of alphabetic writing systems are (a) grapheme-phoneme correspondence, (b) grapheme-morpheme correspondence, and (c) positional frequency and neighborhood letter effect. 276 P. G. Aaron and R. M. Joshi 1. The orthography or the spelling of alphabetic languages is not the result of a mere stringing of letters in a fixed sequence but is influenced by a system of phoneme-grapheme mapping as well as morpheme-grapheme mapping. In transparent orthographies such as Italian, the phoneme-grapheme mapping is straightforward, but in opaque orthographies such as English, the spelling of many of the words is governed by a more complex set of rules. Many such “rules” can be identified in the English orthography (Henry, 1990). 2. Inlogographic orthographies suchasChinese, the semantic lexicon rather than the phonologic lexicon plays a major role. This indicates the influence of morphology on written language. In English, too, the morphemic structure overrides pronunciation in the spellings of words such as “rehearsal” and “healthy.” 3. Positional frequency and neighborhood letter effect is the third principle that determines spelling structure. Positional frequency means that certain letter combinations tend to occur more often in certain positions within words than in other positions. For instance, two consonants generally do not occur in initial positions in English words. Kessler and Treiman (2003) computed the consistency of English spelling in terms of positional effect and concluded that when vowels, onsets, and codas are combined, the obtained figure was .761, which is equivalent to the complexity of a system wherein every sound has two spellings, with one of these spellings being used 86% of the time. In addition to positional effects, the spelling of words can also be influenced or constrained by other letters in the word. For instance, the letter q is always followed by the letter u; the letter t is more often followed by h than y and almost never by b. These canonic principles can, therefore, be collectively thought of as the syntax of orthography. A case can, therefore, be made that orthography of the written language follows its own rules of syntax in a less obvious but consistent manner (Miller & Weinert, 1998). Language Is Universal in the Sense All Societies Have Language SPOKEN LANGUAGE There are more than 6000 languages spoken in the contemporary world, and no human society without spoken language has Written Language Is Natural 277 been reported.Itisconcluded, therefore, that being human means having an innate propensity for language. WRITTEN LANGUAGE Why is it that unlike spoken language, not all societies have written language? Philologists have noted that writing emerged in geographically diverse areas such as the Middle East, China, and MesoAmerica within a short span of time, about 3000 to 5000 years ago. A combination of a biological predisposition and functional needs in the form of commerce and religion could provide a reasonable explanation regarding the diverse but almost simultaneous origin of written language. Had commerce been widespread throughout the world and a global economy was in place 5000 years ago, writing also might have become a global phenomenon. As Vachek (1989) puts it, “some language communities have not yet developed their written norms means that so far no need has been felt in them to establish a specialized norm” (p. 95). Another reason for writing not being universal could be that historically there had been deliberate efforts to keep reading and writing a monopoly; attempts by ordinary people to acquire these skills were thwarted frequently by the privileged. This was the case with the Egyptian scribes, the Vedic Brahmins, and slave owners in some plantation states in the United States. Taylor and Taylor (1995) report that at the beginning of the feudal Edo period (1600–1868), literacy in Japan could be found only among a small group of privileged people such as priests and members of noble families. A survey conducted in 1947, two years after the Second World War, indicated that more than 95% of the Japanese population could be considered literate. Thus, even though a potential for written language existed, the environmental conditions in Japan were not suitable for writing to emerge until a later period. Mellars (1998), writing in the context of the origins of spoken language, notes that: There is a need to make a clear and fundamental distinction between the notions of cognitive potential and behavioral performance. The absence of particular patterns of behavior or cultural expression in particular societies could be due to a whole range of factors: the lack of specific needs or stimulus for that behavior in particular economic, social, or environmental contexts; the lack of adequate raw materials to support particular technological processes . . . .(p. 107) 278 P. G. Aaron and R. M. Joshi Children Acquire Language Spontaneously without Formal Instruction SPOKEN LANGUAGE Children’s spoken language indicates that they make rapid progress in the acquisition of all aspects of language within the first few years of their lives. It is often claimed that this is accomplished without formal tutelage. It has, however, been reported that mothers use limited vocabulary and simple sentences while talking to very young children and use repetitions and questions to promote language comprehension and use (Trainor, Austin, & Desjardins, 2000). In literacy studies, this form of language is referred to as “motherese.” Thus, the acquisition of spoken language skills by children may not be entirely devoid of parental efforts. WRITTEN LANGUAGE Several studies that examined the writings of preschoolers as young as 3 years old indicate that children develop an awareness about writing without having received deliberate instructions. The concept of “emergent literacy” is based on the observation that early signs of literacy skills emerge rather than are learned. For instance, in a study of 3-year-old American and Chinese children, DeFord (1980) found that the child’s writing efforts were spontaneous and, therefore, not much different from that of the infant’s babbling. On the basis of her study of children aged 2 through 7 years, DeFord (1980) concluded that “learning to write is initiated tacitly, as in oral language” (p. 162). It also appears that children naturally pick up the writing system prevalent in their environment just as they acquire language spoken in their environment (Harste & Burke, 1982). Chinese-speaking children of preschool age from Hong Kong tend to use the horizontal and vertical lines and dots that characterize Chinese characters; they also arrange their marks in a square pattern just as conventional Chinese characters are produced (Chan & Louie, 1992). Another instance of emergent literacy is that very young Israeli children “write” from right to left just as Hebrew is written (Levin, Share, & Shatil, 1996). These studies indicate that a tendency to write emerges even before formal instruction begins. The strongest evidence for the acquisition of writing skills without formal schooling comes from studies of Cree nation of Indians who livein the northern tipofLake WinnipeginCanada and Written Language Is Natural 279 use Cree syllabary for writing (McCarthy, 1995). The Cree syllabary was introduced in 1840 by the Methodist minister James Evans and the Cree nation became fully literate in a brief 10 years, from 1841 to 1851 (Berry & Bennett, 1989). According to McCarthy (1995), “monolingual men, women, and children were able to become literate in their own language without formal education” (p. 59). Bennett & Berry (1991) comment that literacy spread almost uncontrollably through the Cree population like an epidemic, and the Cree appear to have achieved something close to universal literacy. In the previous report, Berry and Bennett (1989) noted that the use of the script penetrated the entire Cree-speaking population without the customary pedagogical aids such as schools, teachers, and standard writing materials. Transmission of the script took place under informal conditions on a one-to-one basis, much like the conditions in which spoken language is acquired. The puzzle is how a predominantly oral culture could become a culture of mass literacy in one generation. McCarthy’s answer to this question is that environmental need was the driving force behind this phenomenon. The Cree settlements were widely scattered and, before the days of telephone, writing was the only means of breaking down this isolation. This is evident from the fact that after the 1960s, when oral communication facilities were established through telephone connections, reading and writing in Cree syllabary started losing ground (Bennett & Berry, 1991). Another instance of informal acquisition of literacy, even though less dramatic, is seen among the Vai people. According to Scribner and Cole (1981), who have conducted field studies among the Vai people, the Vai script is not taught in school, but is acquired under informal circumstances,athome and from friends. In spite of acquiring literacy incidentally under informal conditions, nearly 33% of the Vai people are literate in Vai script. The reasons given by Scribner and Cole for this remarkable achievement is that gift giving and gift receiving during Vai ceremonies requires long-term record keeping. The question may be asked why do all children not automatically learn to write and spell without formal instruction. A tentative answer to this question is that as long as a single means of expression and communication—speech, in most instances—is available, cognitive economy prevails and there is no need for learning to communicate through yet anothermedium. For instance,children 280 P. G. Aaron and R. M. Joshi raised in bilingual families generally do not learn the language of their mothers unless they are formally instructed orforced to speak the language under conditions such as having grandparents who do not know the local language (Taylor, 1976). Other evidence for this “driven” nature of acquiring a communicative system is the occurrence of a high frequency of manual babbling seen in deaf infants who, having been deprived of the ability to communicate orally, resort to other means of communication (Petitto & Marentette, 1991). Following the manual babbling, manual gestures emerge as a means of communication even though the parents, having normal hearing, may not use such “sign language” (Marschark, West, Nall, & Everhart, 1986). A study of deaf children by Goldin-Meadows and Feldman (1977) also indicates that language will force itself through whichever channel is available for use. Preliminary analysis of data from a study shows that letting elementary school children use only written language to communicate to each other and refraining them from using oral language for about 30 minutes during every school day improves their reading and writing skills considerably (Joshi & Aaron, unpublished). Anotherexampleofcompetition amongmodesofcommunication is the decline of the use of Cree syllabary among the Cree-speaking people during the past 50 years because of the introduction of an alternate means of communication, the telephone. The Language Model Provided for Children by Adults Is Far from Being Ideal. Nevertheless, Children Acquire Proper Linguistic Skills SPOKEN LANGUAGE The assumption behind this statement is that a strong innate component is required for language learning because the model provided by adults is not particularly helpful in discovering the latent structure. Similarly, even though exposed to Pidgin with its loosely organized syntax, children exposed to Pidgin are able to develop their own grammatically more formal Creole (Bickerton, 1994). However, it has to be noted that direct studies of parent– child communication indicate that adult speech canbemore grammatical than was thought to be even though sentences tend to be short and grammatically simple, with few modifiers and pronouns but more content words (Brown & Bellugi, 1964; Newport, 1975). Written Language Is Natural 281 WRITTEN LANGUAGE English spelling is far from being ideal. However, sentiments about the advantages of the existing spelling have been expressed throughout the past three or four centuries, the most recent being that of Chomsky and Halle (1968), who claimed that English orthography comes remarkably close to being an optimal orthographic system for English. Some scholars have, however, remarked that Chomsky’s claim is overstated (Rogers, 1994) and that it is extravagant and unsupported (Sampson, 1985). Spelling errors committed by children are evidences of children’s efforts to align spelling with pronunciation (e.g., thay for “they”) so that in these instances of “misspellings,” the phonological features of the words being spelled are represented (Ehri, 1986, 1992; Gentry, 1982; Treiman, 1993). Studies of children’s invented spellings indicate that the “mistakes” committed by many beginning spellers reflect their efforts to regularize the “irregular” English spelling (Read, 1971). Language Is a Modular Skill SPOKEN LANGUAGE The modularity argument for language is based on data collected from neuropsychological patients with aphasic syndromes that show that language functions could be selectively impaired leaving other cognitive functions intact (Caplan, 1987; Coughton & Warrington, 1978; Goldstein, 1948; Kinsbourne & Warrington, 1964; Kleist, 1934; Luria, 1976). A study of developmental dyslexia also shows that spoken language can develop normally even though reading skill is not well developed, indicating the modular nature of spoken language (Aaron, Baxter, & Lucenti, 1980). WRITTEN LANGUAGE Many instances of reading deficits in the presence of intact spoken language have been reported by investigators as part of neurological symptoms complex. These are referred to as “alexia without aphasia” and “alexia without agraphia” (Dejerine, 1892; Hecaen & Albert, 1978). There is clinical evidence that the angular gyrus of the dominant hemisphere is specialized for deciphering linguistic stimuli involved in reading. Hecaen, Angeler-gues, and Houilier (1961) conducted a retrospective survey of a 282 P. G. Aaron and R. M. Joshi large series of patients with lesions in the angular gyrus region and noted that left parietal damage was associated with reading, writing, and calculation difficulties. Roeltgen and Heilman (1984) report that four patients who had lesions in a small area at the junction of the posterior angular gyrus and parieto-occipital lobule experienced difficulty in spelling. Kinsbourne and Warrington (1964) noted that the nature of spelling errors made by patients with and without aphasia are qualitatively different, which led them to conclude that spelling is a modular skill. According to McCarthy and Warrington (1990), the selective impairment of spelling skills is now a wellrecognized, albeit rare, phenomenon. Since Some Language Disorders Appear to be Genetically Determined, the Heritability of Language Is High SPOKEN LANGUAGE A familial tendency to inherit language disorders of the use of syntax and morphology has been reported by Gopnik (1990) and by Gopnik and Grago (1991). Cases of developmental language disorders of genetic origin are reported also by Alvares and Downing (1998); Tallal, Ross, and Curtis (1989); Tomblin (1989); Tomblin and Buckwalter (1998); and Weistuch and Schiff (1996). WRITTEN LANGUAGE The genetic nature of specific reading disability was reported over hundred years ago by Hinshelwood (1900) and has been confirmed by many investigators over the years. More precise measures of heritability of reading disability have become available in the past few years (DeFries & Alrcon, 1996; Olson, Rack, Con-ners, DeFries, & Fulker, 1991), ascribing a role for genetic factors in one aspect of literacy. Gayan and Olson (2001), by assessing reading skills with the aid of tests of word recognition, obtained an h2 of .54 for gene effect, .40 for shared environment, and .06 for nonshared environment. Similarly, Harlaar, Spinath, Dale, and Plomin (2005), who studied the word reading efficiency of British 7-year-old twins who were poor readers, obtained a heritability of .60. They also obtained a measure of .28 for shared environmental and .12 for non-shared environmental influence. After reviewing Written Language Is Natural 283 related studies, Olson (2004) concludes that group deficits in word reading are significantly heritable. Genetic studies of spelling difficulties, even though not numerous, do exist. Szeszulski (1989) administered tests of reading, spelling, and language to 40 dyslexic children and their parents. She found four distinct subgroups, of which one group had deficits in orthographic processing only and another had deficits in both phonological and orthographic processing. Stevenson, Graham, Feldman, and McLoughlin (1987) studied the reading skills of 285 MZ and DZ pairs of 13-year-old twins and obtained a heritability of .75 for spelling when intelligence was controlled, leading to the conclusion that there are strong genetic influences on spelling. By fitting a multiple regression model to spelling data obtained from 100 pairs of MZ twins and 71 pairs of same-sex DZ twins, DeFries, Stevenson, Gillis, and Wadsworth (1991) obtained a heritability of about 60% for spelling. Similar to Gopnik’s genetic study of dysphasia, Schulte-Koerne, Wolfgang, Karatina, and Gutenbrun-ner (1996) report familial aggregation of spelling disability in a sample of 32 German school-aged children and their first-degree relatives. If writing is merely an invention, why does spelling skill have a consistently high degree of heritability? Biological Nature of Language: Evidence from MicroEvolutionary, Neo-Darwinian, Innateness Perspective The neo-Darwinian theory of language evolution proposes that changes occurred in small increments over a vast expanse of time. Changes that proved to be advantageous to the survival of the hominids endured and were transmitted to the next generations. Over a long period of time, language evolved and emerged, as we know it today. Before taking upthe question of what evidence there is to show that written language also evolved, as much as spoken language did, we have to examine the sources of evidence for the theory of the evolution of spoken language utilized by the proponents of the view that language evolved along Darwinian lines. In the absence of solid evidence for the evolution of language, linguists have relied on models of language evolution. After looking at such models of evolution, we will examine written language for such evidence. 284 P. G. Aaron and R. M. Joshi Models Used for the Construction of the Theory of Micro-Evolution of Language along Darwinian Lines SPOKEN LANGUAGE Linguists have resorted to several strategies for reconstructing the putative evolutionary course of language (Campbell, 1999). Comparative study of contemporary languages (synchronic study of cohort languages) belonging to the same family is one such means of constructing the evolutionary history of languages. Another strategy is to study the course of language development in children since it is thought to parallel the course of language evolution (Bichakjian, 1988). Evolution of a language could also be constructed if a precursor of that language or a “protolanguage” could be identified. The changes that take place in the development of syntactically formal Creole from a synthetic Pidgin, which is a hodge-podge mixture of native languages and an European language, is also considered to be a model for language evolution (Bickerton, 1995). Language evolution has also been approached as a diachronic (historic) issue by studying the hypothetical “ancestral languages.” For example, Egyptian language, which fell into disuse by about the 700 A.D., can be reconstructed by studying contemporary languages belonging to the Semitic, Cushitic, and Berber families to which Egyptian was related. Yet another diachronic means of studying “ancestral language” is through its reconstruction from ancient written documents. The reconstruction of spoken Egyptian from Coptic and hieroglyphic writings is an example of such an approach. Lest this discussion give an impression that evolution of language is an accepted fact, it has to be pointed out that some linguists themselves are skeptical about the possibility of unequivocally reconstructing the evolutionary course of language. If language evolved, they argue, we should find languages in various states of evolution because, of the nearly 6000 languages, some are boundtobemoreadvanced than others.Inotherwords, languages should vary from each other in terms of their adaptability. However, many linguists subscribe to the view of “uniformitarianism,” which holds that all languages are equal. Aitchinson (1981) has expressed her cynicism about language evolution by stating more plainly that “the evolution of language, as such, has never been Written Language Is Natural 285 demonstrated, and the inherent equality of all languages must be maintained on present evidence” (p. 229). The issue on hand is not whether language evolved or not but whether evolutionary trends similar to the ones claimed for spoken language can also be identified in written language. WRITTEN LANGUAGE In this article, the evolution of written language is traced by examining the Egyptian writing system for which the most comprehensive history of about 4000 years is available. To obtain this information, we have relied primarily on Middle Egyptian (2000) byJames Allen, Fundamentals of Egyptian Grammar by Leo Depuydt (1999), Egyptian Grammar (1969) by Alan Gardiner (1969), and Ancient Egyptian: A Linguistic Introduction by Antonio Loprieno (1995). Other sources of information are indicated in the appropriate context (e.g., Callender, 1975; Polotsky, 1971). It has to be kept in mind that reconstruction of Egyptian language, having gone through more than one form of interpretation, namely, a general theory (Gardiner, 1969), a special theory (Polotsky, 1971), and a contemporary theory (Loprieno, 1995; Depuydt, 1999), itself is evolving. Egyptian, a language belonging to the Afro-Asiatic language family but closely related to Semitic group, was in use as a spoken language for more than 4000 years but totally disappeared by about 700 A.D. when it was replaced by Arabic. The development of the writing system can be traced in four major phases: (a) Old Egyptian (3000—2000 B.C.), (b) Middle Egyptian (2000—1300 B.C.), Late Egyptian (1300—700 B.C.), and (d) the Demotic period (700 B.C.— 500 C.E.). OldEgyptian is the oldest known phase of the language. Early inscriptions of this phase of the language consisted mainly of names and labels. Middle Egyptian appeared in writing around 2100 B.C. and survived as a spoken language for nearly 500 years (Allen, 2000). Late written Egyptian replaced Middle Egyptian by about 1600 B.C. and remained in use until about 600 B.C. Late written Egyptian, according to Allen (2000), differed substantially from the earlier phases, particularly in grammar. The Egyptian writing system, now well known as hieroglyphic writing, is a combination of phonologic and semantic elements with icons representing one, two, or three consonantal phonemes. Examples are monoconsonantal (X /m/; Jow./n/); 286 P. G. Aaron and R. M. Joshi (biconsonantal (tt/pr/); and triconsonantal (I /n-f-r/nefer; T /anh/, ahnkh) signs. In addition to representing sounds, the consonants also could represent morphemes. For example, the icon “ ,” in addition to representing the consonant /pr/, also represented the morpheme “house”; the icon “ ” also meant “beautiful” or “god.” When the icon represented a semantic unit, a stroke was added to it that, as a determinant, indicated that it was a morpheme and not a consonant. Lexical items were represented by icons as in i = (hemet,woman), Hitm. (nn/,water), and ^^», EE (/ir/,eye). These iconic signs could also be combined syntactically to form phrases, clauses, and sentences as in in (nefer’s house or the king’s palace). Another example wherein phonetic and lexical signs are combined to construct a phrase is C^ ra, ill = ms =, M king, &• = upper Egypt, *P lower Egypt; = \ls m M -Jp. 4* = Rameses, king of upper and lower Egypt. It may be argued that the evolutionary changes seen in Egyptian hieroglyphic writing may not be inherent to the written language but reflect corresponding changes that occurred in spoken Egyptian. This is not likely to be the case because the conventions and format of written Egyptian were relatively independent of the spoken language. The disjunction between spoken and written Egyptian has been commented upon by several Egyptologists. For instance, Allen (2000) notes that “The hieroglyphic writing system does not represent very well what Middle Egyptian was like as a spoken language” (p. xii) and that although Middle Egyptian ceased to be a spoken language by about 1600 B.C., hieroglyphic texts represented Middle Egyptian for many centuries more. A similar remark is made by Depuydt (1999), who notes that hieroglyphic writing does not represent Middle Egyptian fully, so much so that some statements about spoken Egyptian has to be inferred from the hieroglyphic writing. This indicates the independent status of written Egyptian as distinct from spoken language. According to Callender (1975) “Middle Egyptian must be defined as a written language, rather than a spoken language...” (p. 7). The format of Egyptian writing was often dictated not by spoken language but by social and religious conventions, which made written syntax depart noticeably from spoken Egyptian. For example, the phrase “servant Written Language Is Natural 287 of god,” which ought to be written as JE A hm-ntr, (hm = servant; ra = god) was actually written as (ra.hm) if? “god servant” where the logogram ra (god) preceded the phonogram hm (servant). This change in syntax was due to the tradition of giving precedence to the gods over men. In the monumental texts, from about 1310 to 1195 B.C., the hieroglyphic units are formally arranged in “ideal squares,” which are aesthetically pleasing but bear no relationship to the word order in spoken language. It could be seen that the combination of semantic and phonetic elements alienates written Egyptian greatly from spoken language. Egyptian writing, particularly the hieroglyphic script, therefore, was not a rendition of speech, but “although it possessed from the beginning a set of monoconsonantal signs,... it never departed from its complex fusion of semagrams and phonograms ... and in its final stages expanded the number and the functional role of its iconic elements” (Loprieno, 1995, p. 237). For these reasons, hieroglyphic writing deserves to be treated as a linguistic system in its own right. Evolutionary Nature of Language: Evidence from the Micro-Evolutionary, Neo-Darwinian Perspective Small Variations in Language Occurred over a Long Period of Time. When These Variations Reached a “Critical Mass,” They Resulted in Significant Changes That Had Selective Advantages to the Language User SPOKEN LANGUAGE Children acquire a corpus of vocabulary before developing grammatical skills. Similarly, Pidgin is spoken first, and Creole emerges later. Using these developmental changes as analogies, linguists have concluded that during language evolution, vocabulary occurred first and grammar later (Givon, 2002; Li & Hombert, 2001). Thus, during the course of evolution, lexical codes must have occurred first and grammatical structures last. Furthermore, according to the classical Darwinian theory of evolution, phyloge-netic changes should have some adaptive advantage in order to survive the process of natural selection. After studying the evolution of language, evolutionary linguists have identified three linguistic 288 P. G. Aaron and R. M. Joshi evolutionary features that might have bestowed an adaptive advantage and thus ensured natural selection. First, languages have all evolved to become learnable at the earliest age possible (Deacon, 1997; Bichakjian, 1999). Second, more evolved linguistic features are adaptive in the sense that they increase perceptual distinction, easeofcomprehen-sion, and rate of transmission than less evolved features (Givon, 1979; Pinker, 1994). Thus, head-first (key word in a grammatical unit) syntactical format is considered more evolved than head-last structure. Head-first sentences place less load on memory than head-last utterances (Bichakjian, 1999). Third, more evolved languages display a rich variety of subordinate clauses embedded in their sentences (Bickerton (2003). In this article, some of the evolutionary changes in phonology, morphology, and syntax of spoken language are examined first within this framework of Darwinian evolution. Subsequently, the evolution of written language is examined by applying the same criteria. Phonology During the course of evolution of Indo-European languages, complex sounds declined in number and were replaced by simple sounds (Bauer, 1992). For instance, in Indo-European languages, laryngeals have been considerably reduced.Acomparisonofances-tral IndoEuropean with modern French shows that the ancestral system was made up of 12 complex stops and one plain fricative /z/, but over the years, more plain consonants and more fricatives (e.g., /f/, /v/, /s/ ) emerged (Bichakjian, 1992). Morphology The following morphological changes appear to have enhanced perceptual distinctions and thereby made acquisition of language easier. Evolutionary changes in spoken languages, particularly IndoEuropean languages, indicate a general shift from synthetic to analytic morphology with auxiliaries, particles, and personal pronouns, having replaced personal endings and declensions (Givon, 1979). In general, affixes were replaced by independent Written Language Is Natural 289 particles such as articles, auxiliaries, and pronouns and a corresponding shift from bound morphemes to free morphemes occurred (Bichakjian, 1999). Developmentally, vocabulary is acquired before grammar (Givon, 2002). Another change that is observed in the evolution of IndoEuropean languages is that personal, temporal, and modal markers have shifted from post position to preposition. Also, reduction of adverbial and adjectival gender markers from three to two and in some cases a total elimination of these markers happened and were replaced by gender- number-person marked pronouns. Development of passive voice constructions is yet another evolutionary change (Bichakjian, 1992). Syntax The change from synthetic to analytic morphology, that is, a reduction in suffixes and affixes, was accompanied by a shift from left branching SOV (subject, object, verb) to right branching SVO word order (Bichakjian, 1999). When affixes tended to disappear, wordorderbecameimportant.Inmany Indo-Europeanlanguages, an associated change from head-last word order to head-first word order is also seen. Another evolutionary trend was to move away from one verb and one clause sentences to complex sentences. A related change is a move away from a form in which phrases are serially linked with pronouns as it is in Latin to the use of subordinate clauses as it is in English. As a consequence of these evolutionary developments, sentences changed from linear order to hierarchical structure (Givon, 1979). WRITTEN LANGUAGE The long recorded history of Egyptian hieroglyphic writing, along with its relative independence from spoken language, provides a unique opportunity to make a comparison of the evolution of written language with that of spoken language from the perspective of the Darwinian theory. An important point is that, similar to spoken language, early Egyptian writing consisted primarily of semantic representations, almost totally devoid of grammatical structures. 290 P G. Aaron and R. M.Joshi Orthography Egyptian writing, in the form of figures on pottery shards, appears to have had its origins in Upper Egypt in Pre- dynastic times, before 3100 B.C. (Arnett, 1982). In general, evolution of the Egyptian writing has followed a trend starting with proto-writing consisting of figures through rebus writing through morpheme writing, terminating in a combination of phonetic and semantic elements (Coulmas, 1997). As seen in the evolution of spoken language, a trend toward simplicity can be detected in the writing system also. The graphemic representations of the phonemes /z/ and /s/ merged and these hieroglyphs were used interchangeably; similarly, the sign for /n/ was assimilated into /m/; also /3/ changed to /y/ (Callender, 1975). In the writing of the Old Kingdom, plural was indicated by repeating the icon (e.g.,^ 4 4 srw officials); by the beginning of Middle Kingdom, three strokes were used as determinatives to indicate plurality (e.g., $" or $! srw officials). Other examples of simplification are avoidance of the repetition of same consonantal signs in contiguity within a word and between words (e.g., mwt~ti came to be written as mwti and ir — n . — i ist as ir-n.lst). In addition, final syllables disappeared or were eliminated along with a progressive loss of final vocalic and semivocalic endings. For example, nat arala (goddess) became ntart in the Middle Kingdom (Gardiner, 1969). Morphology The relationship between Late Egyptian and Middle Egyptian writing systems roughly parallels the evolution of spoken French from Latin (Gardiner, 1969). Many inflexions and linear forms seen in Old Egyptian had been gradually replaced by analytic forms (e.g., the Old Egyptian iw.f hr sdm, “he is upon hearing,” changes to sdm.f, “he hears,” in Middle Egyptian). Another change is a progressive move from the VSO to SVO word order (Gardiner, 1969). Earlier Egyptian has no signs for definite article. But, eventually, suffixes in Old Egyptian were replaced in Middle Egyptian by definite and indefinite articles. A disappearance of suffix conjugation, which improved the precision of expression, began with Old Written Language Is Natural 291 Egyptian and is complete in Middle Egyptian. Modals are virtually non-existent in Old Egyptian but make their appearance during the Middle Kingdom. In Old Egyptian, verbal forms such as perfect and imperfective depend primarily on semantic context, whereas in Middle Egyptian it is largely dictated by the syntactic environment. In early Egyptian writing, many feminine words were marked by plural ending, which may be different from the corresponding pronoun marker. In later Egyptian, gender is indicated by the feminine article, which agrees with feminine pronouns. Earlier Egyptian distinguishes no special reflexive pronouns, but in later times, words like “myself” and “thyself” are indicated. Middle Egyptian had not yet developed a precise set of tenses relating to action even though such elements emerged later and are evidentinLate Egyptian and fully expressed inCoptic. All these changes, therefore, can be taken to have increased the perceptibility and clarity of the written language. Syntax During the course of history, Egyptian has registered a change from synthetic to analytic patterns in the nominal syntax and the verbal system. When compared to Old Egyptian, Late Egyptian is characterized by a simplification syntactic patterns. Use of passive voice constructions is not widely prevalent in Old Egyptian but cametobeusedinthe Middle Egyptian. Similarly, weak negation represented by particles is superseded by stronger contrary negatives in Middle Egyptian. An additional feature seen in Middle Egyptian is the abundant use of nominal and adverbial sentences. The emergence of subordinate clauses in later Egyptian is an evolutionary trend that is well documented. According to Lo-prieno (1995), “.. . the most substantial evolution from earlier to later Egyptian is the one that concerns embedding, i.e., clausal subordination not signaled by an explicit marker of syntactic dependency” (p. 231). This change represents a major typological evolution in the history of Egyptian and is unparalleled in other families of the Afro-Asiatic phylum (Loprieno, 1995). In summary, it appears that many if not all of the evolutionary trends reported by evolutionary linguists for spoken language can 292 P. G. Aaron and R. M. Joshi also be demonstrated in a writing system such as the Egyptian hieroglyphics. There Are Parallels between the Early Stages of Ontogeny of Language in Children and Its Phylogenetic Evolution There are several instances in which individuals belonging to different genus and species resemble each other in their respective early stages of development. Focusing his attention on such parallelisms seen among organisms in their early stages of development, Ernst Haeckel, in 1866, proposed that individual development repeats the evolutionary history of the species and went on to formulate the “biogenetic law.” However, even some contemporaries of Haeckel disputed the validity of his theory and advanced alternate explanations for the similarities seen between ontogenetic and phylogenetic features without resorting to a causal explanation. The question of interest in the present context is “Do features noted in the early stages of the acquisition of writing and spelling skills by children resemble the features seen in the evolution of spoken and written languages?” SPOKEN LANGUAGE Since an unequivocal knowledge about early hominid language is not available, any conclusions drawn regarding the phy-logeny of language has to remain speculative. Notwithstanding this limitation, parallels between ontogeny and phylogeny have been proposed (c.f., Lock, 1983). Many of these conjectures, interestingly, are drawn from observations of ontogeny of child language (e.g., Hurford & Kirby, 1995) because it is believed that language most likely evolved along lines similar to the way it is learned (Harnad, 1996). In their early stages of language acquisition, children tend to use predominantly holophrases, which are primarily lexical in nature. Later on, when event-specific manipulative activity emerges, grammatical morphemes are added to this corpus of expressions (Slobin, 1985). These observations have led to the hypothesis that human languages might have followed a similar path during their evolution (Comrie, Matthews, & Polinsky, 1996). As noted earlier, another analogue for language evolution comes from the study of emergence of Creole language from Pidgin (Bickerton, 1994). In his “bioprogram” for language, Bickerton provides a skeletal model on which a syntactically stable Written Language Is Natural 293 Creoleisbuilt gradually from Pidgin, which,as used by adults, lacks many grammatical elements, is unstable, and has only loosely coordinated clauses and few function words. WRITTEN LANGUAGE It was noted earlier that a hypothesized evolutionary trend of writing systems conforms to the following general pattern: pictograms -> ideograms -> rebus writing -> syllabic writing -> consonantal writing -> alphabetic writing, even though this change is neither linear nor does it follow that alphabetic orthography is the ultimate destination of evolution. The ontogeny of children’s writing approximates this trend. Pioneering studiesofFrench children by Wallon and Lucrat (1957) showed that children’s drawings are only part of a more general graphic activity foreshadowing scribbling and eventually writing. Aaron and Joshi (unpublished) studied the development of writing in 120 children ranging in age from 3 through 8 years. These children came from two distinct linguistic backgrounds, half the world apart. One group of children spoke English at home and school and came from the Midwest in the United States. The second group of children came from a city in India and spokeathome andatschoolaDravidian language whose orthography is derived from the Ashokan Brahmi script. Children in the study were asked to write their names and then write a story about a cat and a dog. The study showed that development of spelling skill followed almost an identical path in both groups of children. Children from both groups who were 3 years old could not write their names, but drew lines and scribbles. A semblance of drawings of cats and dogs appeared at about age four. A majority of 5-year-old children were able write a few letters approaching “words.” These writings were always accompanied by drawings of “cat” and “dog.” Complete but simple sentences emerged at the age of about 7 years, following which drawings disappeared altogether. These observations are in agreement with studies of children’s writings conducted by Sulzby (1996) and Sulzby, Barnhart, and Hieshima (1989). Instances of parallelism seen between ontogeny and phy-logeny of writing are progression from drawings to represent words to the phonetic spelling, the gradual elimination of pictures from writing, absence of consistent directionality in early stages of writing (similar to Egyptian hieroglyphic writing), omission of vowel markers in the writings of preschool children (similar 294 P. G. Aaron and R. M. Joshi to Semitic writing), name writing without letter knowledge (similar to rebus writing), preponderant use of syllabic writing to represent words, and consonant cluster reduction (similar to cuneiform writing). The Anatomy of Vocal Tract and Speech Perception Capabilities Are Synchronous with Language Skill SPOKEN LANGUAGE The human supralaryngeal vocal tract and the emergence of speech are thought to be functionally connected (Deacon, 1992; Laitman, 1983; Lieberman, 1992). It is well recognized that one feature that separates human language from animal communication is the phonological property of the language, which is an inevitable correlate of the human vocal apparatus. Fossil evidence is, therefore, used by some linguists to track the origin and evolution of spoken language. WRITTEN LANGUAGE The thumb may be considered an analog of the vocal tract when written and spoken languages are compared. It is believed that object manipulation and sound production capabilities are ontogenetically linked (Greenfield, 1991). Opposed thumb, which enables precision grip, is a hallmark of mankind, and no nonhum-nan primate can duplicate it accurately (Napier, 1970). Proposing a close relationship between the presence of thumb and language, Mellars (1998) comments thatitisdoubtful thatNeanderthals were capable of symbolic language because of the absence of a fully evolved thumb. It is possible that the neural substrate for precision thumb use might have been present in early hominids (Place, 2000) and could have been later put to exaptative use for writing. Language Is as Old as Humankind SPOKEN LANGUAGE Based on evidence from historical linguistics, archeology, anthropology, and paleontology, it is thought that a potential for language emerged after the Upper Paleolithic period during the postNeanderthal era, which is estimated to be anywhere from 50,000 to100,000 years ago (Mellars, 1998; Renfrew, 1998). Written Language Is Natural 295 However, we do not know how long it took gesture and protolanguage to mature into full-fledged spoken language. WRITTEN LANGUAGE Even though cave paintings date back to some 20,000 years, archaeological records indicate that some time in the fourth millennium B.C., Sumerians resorted to keeping written records when the number of commercial transactions exceeded memory capacity. Even though spoken language is thought to be 50 to 100,000 years old, in terms of geological times this represents a relatively brief moment. Thus, when compared to written language, spoken language may be relatively ancient, but not so in absolute terms. A short history does not necessarily mean that the neural substrates that mediate writing came into place only about 5000 years ago, but very likely the potential for writing could have remained nascent in the human brain for a very long time until environmental conditions (such as commerce, papyrus) necessary for triggering the expression of the potential made their appearance. This explanation will fit in well within the neophenogenetic theory of the evolution of language. In addition, if ontogeny repeats phylogeny, albeit in a limited form, it may provide an explanation for the fact that written language is historically not as ancient as spoken language. Children begin to talk in holophrases before the age of 2 years, but begin to scribble only by about age 3. If the biogenetic law that ontogeny repeats phylogeny can be applied to this difference in onset times, this oneyear difference in the ontogeny of speaking and scribbling may parallel the difference of perhaps 100,000 years reported to have elapsed between the emergence of spoken language and written language. Is Written Language as Natural as Spoken Language? Interactive Nature of Language, Functionalism, and Neophenogenetic Perspective Language Faculty Is Thought to Be the Product of an Interaction between Innate and Sociocultural Factors SPOKEN LANGUAGE Foley and Van Valin (1984) and Velichkovsky (1996) have pointed out that since there are over 6000 natural languages in 296 P. G. Aaron and R. M. Joshi the world, each with its own inventory of symbols and constructions, the human child must be prepared to acquire whichever set of linguistic construction he/she encounters. This means that a large part of the task of language acquisition must be environment-specific, accomplished by means of social interactions. After studying discourse interactions among users of languages such as Cayuga, Ngandi, and Coos, the linguist Mithun (1992) concludes that a basic syntactically defined fixed word order is not universally present but in some languages the word order is flexible and reflects pragmatic functions. In such “free, scrambled word order” languages, meaning is determined primarily with reference to external factors such as newsworthiness of discourse and context (Hale, 1992). From a neophenogenetic perspective, language therefore can be viewed as the result of interaction between ge-nomic potential and environmental factors. WRITTEN LANGUAGE The view that written language is much influenced by sociocultural factors is likely to be accepted more readily than the claim that the acquisition of spoken language also depends on socio-cultural factors.AsnotedbyWaller (1984),the choiceofanoptimal language form is a pragmatic one dependent on the communication context, the means available, and the purposes and limitations of the language user. There is an abundance of evidence attesting to the influence of socio-cultural factors on the phylogeny and ontogeny of written language. As noted earlier, in early Mesopotomia, writing arose to meet a commercial need; in ancient Egypt, writing was used to ensure eternal after-life; the Maya code was used for calendrical purposes. In contemporary societies, when children are exposed to the proper environment with adults functioning as role models, young children initiate their own writing even before formal instruction begins (Sulzby, 1986). After reviewing a number of recent studies, Baker, Fernandex-Fein, Scher, and Williams (1998) have concluded that there is evidence that children’s home experiences involving print do help to prepare them for learning to read. An interesting instance of neophenogenesis is the possibility that experience with written language can exert an influence on neural organization as well. Neurologists who have treated illiterate aphasic patients have noted that even though right handed, a Written Language Is Natural 297 surprising number of these patients did not show typical Broca’s aphasic symptoms (e.g., von Gorlitzer, 1957; Moutier, 1908). These clinical observations led Cameron, Currier, and Haerer (1971) to suggest that language is not well planted in the dominant hemisphere in illiterates. On the basis of a large-scale empirical study, Lecours, Mehler, and Parente (1988) came to the conclusion that an interaction does occur between a genetic tendency toward left-right asymmetry and literacy experience. Communication through Language is a Social Act that Involves More than One Person. Consequently, the Speaker and Listener Influence Each Other, Which Affects Language Production and Comprehension. Without This Interaction, Languages Will Perish SPOKEN LANGUAGE According to some premises based on generative grammar, young children have adult-like syntactic competence, and for this reason children learn the language without much effort and do so within a very short span of time. This assumption, however, may need some modification. Studies by Tomasello (2000) show that young children’s early language is more concrete and item-based (Tomasello, 1992). The fact that language requires a minimum number of speakers is made obvious by the fact that a significant number of the world’s languages are on the verge of extinction. According to Comrie et al. (1996), about 90% of today’s spoken languages will be either dead or moribund about a hundred years from now. WRITTEN LANGUAGE The growth, spread, and extinction of written languages also show trends similar to the ones seen in spoken languages. Written language, however, is more durable than oral language for two reasons. One is that, whereas oral language is ephemeral and fleeting, writing language is relatively permanent. The other reason is that a script can be adapted to a new language and, therefore, can represent more than one language. Nevertheless, there are numerous examples of written languages that have fallen into disuse. Examples are the cuneiform script used to write Sumerian, Old Akkadian, and Babilonian, and, of course, Egyptian hieroglyphics. 298 P. G. Aaron and R. M. Joshi The Relationship between Written Language and Spoken Language In order to bestow legitimacy to the arguments so far presented, namely written language is as natural as language studied in its spoken form, it has to be shown that the parallelism seen between writing systems and spoken language is not a mere consequence of written language being an attendant and a proxy of spoken language but that written language has a status all its own. During Its Early Stages of Its Evolution, Written Language Was Not Used to Represent Speech In the historical past, writing began not as a means of recording spoken language, but as a counting device for keeping track of business transactions. The early Mesopotamian writings were records of transactions of agricultural commodities (Schmandt-Basserat, 1992). Coulmas (1997) describes early Sumerianas“word writing” and Falkenstein (1964) calls it “sentenceless language.” Early Egyptian writing (3000 B.C.), similarly, was merely “king lists” (Waterson, 1997). In China, the oldest writings were used for keeping track of offerings to gods; in MesoAmerica, writing was initially used for recording astronomical events and calendrical information (Daniels, 1996). A documentary form of list-writing can be seen even in the Torah (e.g., Irad begat Mehajael; Mehajael begat Methusael; etc., Ong, 1982). According to Goody (1977), these lists were not mere records of items or events but also reflect the opening up of a new mode of cognition, namely the recognition of the significance of past events and the importance of transmitting information to future generation. There Are Qualitative Differences between Written Language and Spoken Language Some investigators have shown that written texts have more integrated linguistic units, containing noun or verb phrases often expanded with clauses, whereas informal oral discourse is of a rather fragmented nature with limited reference to syntax, with intonation providing a major clue for linking utterances (Chafe, 1985; Olson, 1977). After noting that the labels “spoken language” Written Language Is Natural 299 and “written language” do not refer merely to different mediums, Miller and Weinert (1998) have proceeded to show that “the syntactic structure of phrases and clauses in spontaneous spoken language is notably different from the structure of phrases and clauses in written language” (p. 1). They support this view by comparing samples from languages such as English, German, and Russian. There Are Functional Differences between Written Language and Spoken Language The phenomenon of “diglossia” is an instance wherein functional differences between written and spoken language are readily seen. Diglossia refers toa relatively stable language situationin that there is a highly codified and grammatically complex high variety that is used for most written purposes but not for ordinary conversation (Ferguson, 1959; Hudson, 1999). Diglossia is seen in several languages, such as Tamil and Bengali spoken in India and Sinhalese spoken in Sri Lanka. The origin of linguistic dimorphism of Indian languages has historical roots dating back to samskrta and prakrta, which are literally formal and colloquial forms of Sanskrit, respectively. According to De Silva (1982), high Sinhalese is not a spoken language at all; it is a language of written Sinhalese. Written Language Can Have an Influence on Spoken Language The differences observed between spoken and written languages have led to the expectation that experience with written language will have a facilitating effect on cognitive competence as well as spoken language. An investigation of the Vai language speakers by Scribner and Cole (1981) confirms this expectation. These researchers tested literate and nonliterate subjects in Vai language for their ability to produce sentences and found that literates produced speech forms derived mostly from written models, whereas non-literates did not, indicating that writing has a general influence on speech. Michaels and Collins (1984) analyzed spoken narratives produced by Englishspeaking children and found that familiarity with written language greatly affects the children’s use of cohesive devices even in narratives. Scholes and Willis (1991) tested knowledge of morphemes of literate and nonliterate adults (in adult education classes) and their ability 300 P. G. Aaron and R. M. Joshi to comprehend syntactically complex sentences. The investigators concluded that literacy contributes to a knowledge of morphemic analysis and that attainment of many of the transformational syntactic rules employed by literate adult native speakers of English is not found in those who have not acquired literacy. Developmental studies also indicate that oral languageof even very young children reflects the influence of written language. Sulzby (1996), who studied children’s acts of reading and writing and collected children’s speech samples, concluded that the conversational level of children who read is more complex than those who do not read. It is also documented that vocabulary growth depends much on reading experience and that children who do not have much exposure to reading are limited in their vocabulary knowledge, and over a period of time fail to keep up with their reading peers. Stanovich (1986) has described this phenomenon as the “Matthew effect” after the gospel of St. Matthew, chapter 25, verse 29. One of the most durable findings of recent research in the area of reading instruction is the relationship between phoneme awareness and reading skill. A number of studies have shown that training in phoneme awareness has a facilitating effect on reading achievement (Bradley & Bryant, 1985; Lundberg, Frost, & Petersen, 1988). These results are generally understood to imply a cause-effect relationship between phoneme awareness and reading; that is, an awareness of phoneme produces positive effects on reading skill. However, some investigators are reluctant to attribute a causal role for phoneme awareness. Hewes (1983), for instance, noted that it is unlikely that linguists would have hit upon the notion of phonemes had they not been familiar with alphabetic systems. This means exposure to written language is responsible for creating an awareness of phonemes and not necessarily the other way around. Support for this position comes from a study by Morais, Bertelson, Cary, and Alegria (1986), who found that Portuguese peasants who had received only a minimal amount of schooling and instruction in reading were, years later, superior in tasks that tested phoneme awareness compared to those who had no experience with reading. Scholes and Willis (1991) report a similar finding with adult non-literates enrolled in an adult literacy program in the United States. A review study by Bertelson and DeGelder (1988) indicates that phoneme awareness is present Written Language Is Natural 301 only if a person can read an alphabetic representation of that language. Pontecorvo and Orsolini (1996) even assert that awareness of the phoneme is acquired only through the mastery of an alphabetic system of writing. Castles and Coltheart (2004) have also questioned the causal link between phonological awareness and reading, questioning the validity of the findings of studies that report improvement in reading after phonemic awareness training on the grounds that it is highly unlikely that these children were not exposed to print (TV, billboards, etc.) before receiving phonemic awareness training. In other words, exposure to print prior to phonemic awareness training might have helped these children to become sensitive to phonemes, which, in turn, might have improved their reading skills. An example of a spoken language having been influenced by written language is Japanese, whose Kanji characters had been borrowed from Chinese. Eventually, these borrowed characters entered spoken language in the form of Sino- Japanese loan words, which constitute as much as 50% of today’s Japanese vocabulary (Taylor, 1976). Taken together, these studies indicate that written and spoken language complement each other. These observations also indicate that writing need not be a mere phonetic transcription of spoken language and neither is a text simply speech written down (Coulmas, 1997). The Egyptian hieroglyphic system is one instance where the independent status of the writing system is clearly demonstrable. Some of the contemporary writing systems also exhibit a tendencytodrift away from spoken language because languages are constantly changing. For instance, Chinese orthography is a logographic system that is less phonetic now than it was earlier in history. Furthermore, modern English, unlike its ancestral inflectional form, has become more of an isolating language in which grammatical relations are signaled by word order rather than by inflexions (Leith, 1996). Correspondingly, English orthography has become more morphophonemic and less phonetic. Discussion and Conclusions In this article, we examined the question of whether the written form of language could be considered to be as natural as the spoken form of language from a bio-linguistic point of view. A 302 P. G. Aaron and R. M. Joshi second aim was to evaluate the validity of the belief that spoken language represents the essence of human mind, whereas written language is a proxy for spoken language. These issues were examined within the framework of micro-evolution, macro-evolution, and neophenogenesis. Evidence was presented to show that the biological features and the presumed evolutionary trends claimed for spoken language can also be seen in written language. Instances of the biological basis of writing systems include the heritability of certain forms of spelling disorders, the modularity of spelling skills, the spontaneous tendency of very young children to resort to scribbling and then writing, and the species-specific nature of writing. Instances of evolutionary trends seen in the evolution of written language are the long-term changes that have taken place in writing systems such as the Egyptian hieroglyphics and the observed parallels between phylogeny of writing system and the ontogeny of children’s writing. It would be reasonable, therefore, to conclude that written language, similar to spoken language, is a product of natural and socio-cultural forces even though we are not in a position to compute the proportion of biological and socio-cultural contribution toward the evolution of written language. From an educational perspective, the evolutionary history of written language suggests strongly that written language should be given much importance in literacy instruction. Legislative acts such as “no child left behind” are a step in the right direction. More specifically, intense training in written language can be used for improving not only literacy skills but also comprehension and the quality of spoken language. Several studies have cast doubt on the expectation that phoneme awareness alone will increase reading skills but propose that experience with written language can facilitate awareness of phonemes and that instruction in letter knowledge in combination with phonological training produces optimal literacy effects (Hammil, 2004). References Aaron, P. G., Baxter, C., & Lucenti, J. (1980). Developmental dyslexia and acquired alexia: Two sides of the same coin? Brain & Language, 11, 1–11. Written Language Is Natural 303 Aiello, L. C. (1998). The foundations of human language. In N.G. Jablonski & L.C. Aiello (Eds.), The origin and diversification of language (pp. 21–34). San Francisco, CA: California Academy of Sciences. Aitcheson, J. (1981). Language change: Progress or decay? Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Allen, J. (2000). Middle Egyptian: An introduction to the language and culture of hieroglyphs. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Alvares, R. L., & Downing, S. F. (1998). A survey of expressive communication skills in children with Angelman syndrome. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 7, 14–24. Arnett, W. S. (1982). The predynastic origin of Egyptian hieroglyphs. Washington, DC: University Press of America. Baker, L., Fernandez-Fein, A., Scher, D., & Williams, H. (1998), Home experiences related to the development of word recognition. In J. L. Metsala & L.C. Ehri (Eds.), Word recognition in beginning literacy (pp. 263–287). Mahway, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Bauer, B. L. (1992). Evolution in language: Evidence from the Romance axillary. In J. Wind, B. Chiarelli, B. Bichakjian, A. Nocentini, & A. Jonker (Eds.), Language origin: A multidisciplinary approach (pp. 517–528). Boston: Kluwer Academic Publishers. Bennett, J. A., & Berry, J. W. (1991). Cree literacy in the syllable script. In D.R. Olson & N. Torrance (Eds.), Literacy and orality (pp. 90–104). New York: Cambridge University Press. Berry, J. W., & Bennett, J. A. (1989). Syllabic literacy and cognitive performance among the Cree. International Journal of Psychology, 24, 429–450. Bertelson, P., & DeGelder, B. (1988). Learning about reading from illiterates. In A. Galaburda (Ed.), From neurons to reading. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Bichakjian, B. H. (1989). Language innateness and speech pathology. In J. Wind, E. Pulleyblank, E. DeGrolier, & B. Bichakjian (Eds.), Studies in language origins (Vol. 1., pp. 209–232). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. Bichakjian, B. H. (1992). Language evolution: Evidence from historical linguistics. In J. Wind, B. Chiarelli, B. Bichakjian, A. Nocentini, & A. Jonker (Eds.), Language origin: A multidisciplinary approach (pp. 171– 202). Boston: Kluwer Academic. Bichakjian, B. H. (1999). Language evolution and the complexity criterion. Psycholoquy, 10. Retrieved from http://www.cogsci.soton.ac.uk/cgi/psyc/ newpsy?10.0333 Bickerton, D. (1994). The language bioprogram hypothesis. Behavioral & Brain Sciences, 7, 173–221. Bickerton, D. (1995). Language and human behavior. Seattle, WA: University of Washington Press. Bickerton, D. (2003). Symbol and structure: A comprehensive framework for language evolution.InM.H.Christiansen&S.Kirby (Eds.),Language evolution. (pp. 77–93). New York: Oxford University Press. Bloomfield, L. (1933). Language. New York: Hart & Winston. Bradley, L.,&Bryant, P.(1985).Rhyme and reason in reading and spelling. Ann Arbor, MI: University of Michigan Press. 304 P. G. Aaron and R. M. Joshi Brown, R., & Bellugi, U. (1964). Three processes in the child’s acquisition of syntax. Harvard Educational Review, 34, 133–151. Callender, J. B. (1975). Middle Egyptian. Malibu, CA: Undena Publications. Cameron, R. F., Currier, R. D., & Haerer, A. F. (1971). Aphasia and literacy. British Journal of Disorders of Communication, 6, 161–163. Campbell, L. (1999). Historical linguistics. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Caplan, D. (1987). Neurolinguistics and linguistic aphasiology: An introduction. New York: Cambridge University Press. Castles, A., & Coltheart, M. (2004). Is there a causal link from phonological awareness to success in learning to read? Cognition, 91(1), 77–111. Chafe, W. L. (1985). Linguistic differences produced by differences between speaking and writing. In D.R. Olson, N. Torrance, & A. Hildyard (Eds.), Literacy, language and learning (pp. 105–123) Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Chan, L.,& Louie, L.(1992). Developmental trendofChinese preschool children in drawing and writing. Journal of Research in Childhood Education, 6, 93–99. Christiansen,M.H.,& Kirby,S.(2003). Language evolution: The hardest problem in science? In M.H. Christiansen & S. Kirby (Eds.), Language evolution, (pp. 1– 15). New York: Oxford University Press. ∗Chomsky, N. (1972). Language and mind. New York: Harcourt, Brace & Jovanovich. Chomsky, N. (1975). Reflections on language. New York: Pantheon. Chomsky, N. (1988). Language and problems of knowledge: The Managua lectures. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Chomsky, N., & Halle, M. (1968). The sound pattern of English. New York: Harper & Row. Coe, M. D. (1999). Breaking the Maya code. New York: Thames & Hudson. ∗Comrie, B. (1981). Language universals and linguistic typology. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Comrie, B., Matthews, S., & Polinsky, M. (1996). The atlas of languages. New York: Facts on File Inc. ∗Cosmides, L., Tooby, J., & Barkow, H. (1992). Evolutionary psychology and conceptual integration. In H. Barkow, L. Cosmides, & J. Tooby (Eds.), The adapted mind: Evolutionary Psychology and the generation of culture (pp. 70–93) New York: Oxford University Press. Coughton, A. K., & Warrington, E. K. (1978). Word comprehension and word retrieval in patients with localized cerebral lesions. Brain, 101, 163–185. Coulmas, F. (Ed.). (1997). The handbook of sociolinguistics. Oxford, UK: Blackwell. Daniels, P. T. (1996). The study of writing systems. In P.T. Daniels & W. Bright (Eds.), The world’s writing systems (pp. 1–2). New York: Oxford University Press. Darwin, C. (1871). The descent of man and selection in relation to sex. London: J. Murray. Deacon, T. W. (1992). Brain-language coevolution. In J. A. Hawkins & M. GellMann (Eds.), The evolution of human languages (pp. 49–83) Reading, MA: Addison Wesley. Deacon, T. W. (1996). Prefrontal cortex and symbol learning: Why a brain capable of language evolved only once. In B. M. Velichkovsky & D. M. Written Language Is Natural 305 Rumbaugh (Eds.), Communicating meaning: The evolution and development of language (pp. 103–138) Mahway, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Deacon, T. W. (1997). The symbolic species: The co-evolution of language and the brain. New York: W.W. Norton & Co. DeFord, D. E. (1980). Young children and their writing. Theory into Practice, 19, 157–162. ∗DeFrancis, J. (1989). Visible speech: The diverse oneness of writing systems. Honolulu: The University of Hawaii Press. DeFries, J., Stevenson, J., Gillis, J., & Wadsworth, S. (1991). Genetic etiology of spelling deficits in the Colorado and London twin studies of reading disability. Reading & Writing, 3, 271–283. ∗DeFries, J. C., & Alrcon, M. (1996). Genetics of specific reading disability. Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews, 2, 39–47. DeJerine, J. (1892). Contribution a l’etude anatomoclinique et clinique des differentes varietes de cecite verbale. C.R. Hebdomadaire des Sceances et Memoires de la Societe de Biologie, 4, 61–90. Depuydt, L. (1999). Fundamentals of Egyptian grammar. Ashland, OH: Book Masters. de Saussure, F. (1916). Course in general linguistics. London: Duckworth. De Silva, M.S. (1982). Some consequencesofdiglossia.InW.Hass, (Ed.).Standard languages, spoken and written (pp. 238–253) Manchester: Manchester University Press. ∗Dobzhansky, T. (1937). Genetics and the origin of species. New York: Columbia University Press. Ehri, L. (1986). Sources of difficulty in learning to spell and read. In M. L. Wolraich & D. Routh (Eds.), Advances in developmental and behavioral pediatrics (Vol. 7, pp. 121–195). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press. Ehri, L. (1992). Review and commentary: Stages of spelling development. In S. Templeton & D.R. Bear (Eds.), Development of orthographic knowledge and the foundations of literacy: A memorial festschrift for Edmund H. Henderson (pp. 307– 332) Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Eldredge, N. (1989). Macroevolutionary dynamics: Species, niches, and adaptive peaks. New York: McGraw-Hill. Falkenstein, A. (1964). Das Sumerische. Reprint from Handbuch der Orientalistik. Leiden: Brill. Ferguson, C. A. (1959). Diglossia. Word, 15, 325–340. Fodor, J. A., Bever, T. G., & Garrett, M. (1974). The psychology of language. New York: McGraw-Hill. Foley, W. A., & Van Valin, R. D. (1984). Functional syntax and universal grammar. New York: Cambridge University Press. Gardiner, A. (1969). Egyptian grammar, being an introduction to the study of hieroglyphs, 3rd ed. London: Oxford University Press. Gardner, A., & Gardner, B. (1969). Teaching sign language to a chimpanzee. Science, 165, 664–672. Gayan, J., & Olson, R. K. (2001). Genetic and environmental influences on orthographic and phonological skills in children with reading disabilities. Developmental Neuropsychology, 20, 487–511. 306 P. G. Aaron and R. M. Joshi Gelb, I. J. (1974). A study of writing. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Gentry, J. R. (1982). An analysis of developmental spelling in GNYS AT WRK. The Reading Teacher, 36, 192– 200. Givon, T. (1979). On understanding grammar. New York: Academic press. Givon, T. (2002). Bio-linguistics. Philadelphia: John Benjamins. Goldin-Meadows, S., & Feldman, H. (1977). The development of languagelike communication without a language model. Science, 197, 401– 403. Goldstein, K. (1948). Language and language disturbances. New York: Grune & Stratton. Goody, J. (1977). The domestication of the savage mind. New York: Cambridge University Press. Gopnik, M. (1990). Feature-blind grammar and dysphasia. Nature, 314, 715. Gopnik, M., & Crago, M. (1991). Familial aggregation of a developmental language disorder. Cognition, 39, 1–50. Gottlieb,G.(1991). Experiential canalizationofbehavioral development: Theory. Developmental Psychology, 27, (1), 4–13. Gould, S. J. (1984). Relationship of individual and group change. Ontogeny and phylogeny in biology. Human Development, 27, 233–239. Gould, S. J., & Lewontin, R. C. (1979). The spandrels of San Marco and the Panglossian program: A critique of the adaptationist programme. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, 205, 281–288. Gould, S. J., & Marler, P. (1987). Learning by instinct. Scientific American, 272(1), 68–82. Greenfield, P. M. (1991). Language, tools, and brain: The ontogeny and phy-logeny of hierarchically organized sequential behavior. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 14, 531–595. Haeckel, E. (1883). The History of Creation. Translated by F.R. Lankester. London: Kegan Paul. Hale, K. (1992). Basic word order in two “free word order” languages. In D.L. Payne (Ed.), Pragmatics of word order flexibility (pp. 75–99). Philadelphia: John Benjamins. Hammill, D. (2004). What we know about correlates of reading. Exceptional Children, 70 (4), 453–463. Harlaar, N., Spinath, F. M., Dale, P. S., & Plomin, R. (2005). Genetic influences on word recognition abilities and disabilities: A study of 7-year-old twins. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 46, 373–384. Harnad, S. (1996). The origins of words: A psychophysical hypothesis. In B.M. Velichkovsky & D.M. Rumbaugh (Eds.), Communicating meaning: The evolution and development of language. Mahway, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Harste, J., & Burke, P. (1982). Predicabilidad un universal de la lecto-escritura. In E. Ferreiro & M. Gomez (Eds.), Nuevas perspectivas sobre los procesos de lectura y escritura, XXI, 111–121. Hecaen, H., & Albert, M. (1978). Human neuropsychology. New York: Wiley. Hecaen, H., Angelergues, R., & Houilier, S. (1961). Les varietes cliniques des acalculias au cours des lesions retrolandiques. Revue Neurologique, 2, 85– 103. Written Language Is Natural 307 Henry, M. (1990). Words. Austin, TX: Pro Ed. Hewes, G. W. (1983). The invention of phonemically based language. In E. de Grolier (Ed.), Glossogenetics: The origin and evolution of language (pp. 143–162). New York: Harwood Academic. Hinshelwood, J. (1900). Congenital word-blindness. The Lancet, 1, 1506–1508. Hudson, A. (1999). Diglossia. In B. Spolsky (Ed.), Concise encyclopedia of educational Linguistics, (pp. 37–42). New York: Elsevier. Humboldt, von W. C. (1931). Linguistic variability and intellectual development. Reprinted in Miami Linguistic Series, No. 9. Coral Gables, FL: University of Miami Press. Hurford, J. R., & Kirby, S. (1995). Neural preconditions for protolanguage. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 18, 193–194. Jean, G. (1992). Writing: The story of alphabets and scripts. New York: Henry Abrams. Jenkins, L. (2000). Biolinguistics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Johnson, S. (1755). A dictionary of the English language. London: W. Strachan. Johnston, T. D., & Gottlieb, G. (1990). Neophenogenesis: A developmental theory of phenotypic evolution. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 147, 471– 495. Joshi, R. M., & Aaron, P. G. (unpublished). DEAR: Drop everything and write. A program which requires children to communicate among themselves in the classroom for a specified amount of time only through written language. Unpublished manuscript. Kalmar, I. (1985). Are there no primitive languages? In D.R. Olson, N. Torrance, & A. Hildyard (Eds.), Literacy, language, and learning. The nature and consequences of reading and writing (pp. 85–96). New York: Cambridge University Press. Kessler, B., & Treiman, R. (2003). Is English spelling chaotic? Misconceptions concerning its irregularity. Reading Psychology, 24, 267–289. Kinsbourne, M., & Warrington, E. K. (1964). Disorders of spelling. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 27, 224–228. Kleist, K. (1934). Gehirnpathologie. Leipzig: Barth. Laitman, J. T. (1983). The evolution of the hominid upper respiratory system and implications for the origins of speech. In E. de Grolier (Eds.), Glossogenetics: The origin and evolution of language (pp. 63–90). New York: Harwood Academic. LeCours, A. R., Mehler, J., & Parente, M. (1988). Illiteracy and brain damage: A contribution to the study of speech and language disorders in illiterates with unilateral brain damage. Neuropsychologia, 27(4), 575–589. Leith, D. (1996). The originsof English.In D.Graddol, D.Leith, & J. Swan (Eds.), English: History, diversity, and change (pp. 95–132). London: Routledge. Levin, I., Share, D. L., & Shatil, E. (1996). A qualitative-quantitative study of preschool writing: Its development and contribution to school literacy. In C. M. Levy & S. Ransdell (Eds.), The science of writing: Theories, methods, individual differences and applications (pp. 271–293). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Li, C. N., & Hombert, J. M. (2000). On the evolutionary origin of language. In M. Stamenov & V. Gallese (Eds.), Mirror neurons and the evolution of brain and language (pp. 77–92). Amsterdam: Benjamins. Lieberman, P. (1984). The biology and evolution of language. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. 308 P. G. Aaron and R. M. Joshi Lieberman, P. (1992). On the evolution of human language. In J. A. Hawkins & M. Gell-Mann (Eds.), The evolution of human languages (pp. 21–48). Reading, MA: Addison Wesley. Lock, A. J. (1983). Recapitulation in the ontogeny and phylogeny of language. In E. de Groliev (Ed.), Glossogenetics (pp. 225–273). New York: Harwood Academic. Loprieno, A. (1995). Ancient Egyptian: A linguistic introduction. New York: Cambridge University Press. Lundberg, I., Frost, J., & Petersen, O. (1988). Effects of an extensive program for stimulating phonological awareness in preschool children. Reading Research Quarterly, 23, 263–284. Luria, A. R. (1976). Basic problems in neurolinguistics. The Hague: Mouton. ∗Mair, V. H. (1996). Modern Chinese writing. In P. T. Daniels & W. Bright (Eds.), The world’s writing systems (pp. 340–365). New York: Oxford University Press. Marschark, M., West, S., Nall, L., & Everhart, V. (1986). Development of creative language devices in signed and oral production. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 41, 534–550. Maryanski, A. (1996). Was speech an evolutionary afterthought? In B.M. Velichkovsky & D. M. Rumbaugh (Eds.), Communicating meaning: The evolution and development of language (pp. 123–148). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. McCarthy, R. A., & Warrington, E. K. (1990). Cognitive neurospsychology. New York: Academic Press. McCarthy, S.(1995). The Cree syllabary and the writing system riddle: A paradigm in crisis. In I. Taylor & D. R. Olson (Eds.), Scripts and literacy (pp. 59–76). Boston: Kluwer Academic. Mellars, P. A. (1998). Neanderthals, modern humans, and the archaeological evidence for language. In N.G. Jablonski & L.C. Aiello (Eds.), The origin and diversification of language (pp. 89–116). San Francisco, CA: California Academy of Sciences. Michaels, S., & Collins, I. (1984). Oral discourse styles: Classroom interaction and the acquisition of literacy. In D. Tannen (Ed.), Spoken and written language (pp. 219–244). Norwood: Ables. Michalowski, P. (1996). Mesopotamian cuneiform. P. T., Daniels, & W. Bright (Eds.), The world’s writing systems (pp. 53–69). New York: Oxford University Press. Miller, J., & Weinert, R. (1998). Spontaneous spoken language: Syntax and discourse. Oxford: Clarendon Press. Mithun, M. (1992). Is basic word order universal? In D.L. Payne (Ed.), Pragmatics of word order flexibility (pp. 49–79). Philadelphia: John Benjamins. Morais, J., Bertelson, P., Cary, L., & Alegria, J. (1986). Literacy training and speech segmentation. Cognition, 24, 45–64. Moutier, F. (1908). L’aphasie de Broca. Paris: Steinheil. Muller, R. A. (1996). Innateness, autonomy, universality? Neurobiological approaches to language. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 19, 611–675. Napier, J.R.(1970).The roots of mankind. Washington, DC: Smithsonian Institution Press. Written Language Is Natural ∗Newmeyer, 309 F. J. (2001). The Prague school and North American functionalist approaches to syntax. Journal of Linguistics, 37, 101–126. Newport, E. L. (1975). Motherese: The speech of mothers to young children (Tech. Re. No. 52). San Diego: University of California, Center for Human Information Processing. Olson, D. R. (1977). The basis of language in speech and writing. Harvard Education Review, 47, 257–281. ∗Olson, D. R. (1994). The world on paper. New York: Cambridge University Press. Olson, R. K. (2004). SSSR, environment, and genes. Scientific Studies of Reading, 8, 111–124. Olson, R. K., Rack, J. P., Conners, F. A., DeFries, J. C., & Fulker, D. W. (1991). Genetic etiology of individual differences in reading disability. In L. V. Feagans, E. J. Short, & L. J. Meltzer (Eds.), Subtypes of learning disabilities: Theoretical perspectives and research (pp. 243–273). Ong, W. J. (1982). Orality and literacy: The technologizing of the word. New York: Methuen. Petitto, L., & Marentette, P. (1991). Babbling in the manual mode: Evidence for the ontogeny of language. Science, 251, 1493–1496. Piattelli-Palmarini, M. (1989). Evolution, selection and cognition: From “learning” to parameter setting in biology and in the study of language. Cognition, 31, 1–44. Pinker, S. (1994). The language instinct. New York: William Morrow. Pinker, S. (1998). The evolution ofthe human language faculty. In N. G. Jablonski & L.C. Aiello (Eds.), The origin and diversification of language (pp. 117–126). San Francisco, CA: California Academy of Sciences. Pinker, S., & Bloom, P. (1990). Natural language and natural selection. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 13, 707–784. Place, U. T. (2000). The role of the hand in the evolution of language. Psycholoquy, 11 Retrieved from http://www.princeton.edu/-harnad/psyc.html Polotsky, H. J. (1971). Egyptian. In H.J. Polotsky (Ed.), Collected papers (pp. 320– 338). Jerusalem, The Hebrew University Magnes Press. Pontecorvo, C., & Orsolini, M. (1996). Writing and written language in children’s development. In C. Pontecorvo, M. Orsolini, B., Burge, & L. Resnick (Eds.), Children’s early text construction (pp. 3–23). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Premack, A. J. (1976). Why chimps can read? New York: Harper & Row. Pulgram, E. (1965). Graphic and phonic systems. Word, 21, 208–224. ∗Ragir, S. (1985). Retarded development: The evolutionary mechanism underlying the emergence of the human capacity for language. Journal of Mind & Behavior, 6, 451–468. Read, C. (1971). Pre-school children’s knowledge of English phonology. Harvard Educational Review, 41, 1–34. Renfrew, C. (1998). The origins of world linguistic diversity: An archaeological perspective. In N. G. Jablonski & L. C. Aiello (Eds.), The origin and diversification of language (pp. 171–192). San Francisco, CA: California Academy of Sciences. Roberts, C., & Street, B. (1997). Spoken and written language. In Coulmas, F. (Ed.), The handbook of sociolinguistics (pp. 168–186). Oxford: Blackwell. 310 P. G. Aaron and R. M. Joshi Robinson, A. (1995). The story of writing. London: Thames & Hudson. Roeltgen, B., & Heilman, S. (1984). Lexical agraphia: Further support for the two strategy hypothesis of linguistic agraphia. Brain, 107, 811–827. Rogers,H.(1994). Optimal orthographies.InI.Taylor&D.R. Olson (Eds.),Scripts and literacy (pp. 31–43). Boston: Kluwer Academic. ∗Ruhlen, M. (1992). An overview of genetic classification . In J. A. Hawkins & M. Gell-Mann (Eds.), The evolution of human languages (pp. 68–98). Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley. Rumbaugh, D. M., & Savage-Rumbaugh, S. (1996). Biobehavioral roots of language: Words, apes and a child. In B.M. Velichkovsky & D.M. Rumbaugh (Eds.), Communicating meaning: The evolution and development of language. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Sampson, G. (1985). Writing systems: A linguistic introduction. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press. Schmandt-Basserat, D. (1992). Before writing: From counting to cuneiform. Austin, TX: University of Texas Press. Scholes, R. J., & Willis, B. J. (1991). Linguists, literacy, and the intensional-ity of Marshall McLuhan’s Western man. In D. R. Olson & N. Torrance (Eds.). Literacy and orality (pp. 215–235). New York: Cambridge University Press. Schulte-Koerne, G., Wolfgang, D., Mueller, K., & Gutenbrunner, C. (1996). Familial aggregation of spelling disability. Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry & Allied Disciplines, 37, 817–822. Scribner, S., & Cole, M. (1981). The psychology of literacy. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Stanovich, K. E. (1986). Matthew effects in reading: Some consequences of individual differences in the acquisition of literacy. Reading Research Quarterly, 21, 360–407. ∗Stevenson, J., Graham, P., Feldman, M., & McLoughlin, A. (1987). A twin study of genetic influences on reading and spelling ability and disability. Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry & Allied Disciplines, 28, 229–247. Stubbs, M. (1980). Language and Literacy. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul. Sulzby, E. (1986). Writing and reading: Signs of oral and written language organization in the young child. In W. H. Teale & E. Sulzby (Eds.), Energent literacy: Writing and reading (pp. 50–89). Norwood, NJ: Ablex Publishing. Sulzby,E.(1996). Rolesoforal and written languageaschildren approach conventional literacy. In C. Pontecorvo, M. Orsolini, B. Burge, & L.B. Resnik (Eds.), Children’s early text construction (pp. 25–46). Mahway, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Sulzby, E., Barnhart, J., & Hieshima, J. (1989). Forms of writing and rereading from writing: A preliminary report. In J. Mason (Ed.), Reading/writing connections (pp. 31–63). Needham Heights, MA: Allyn & Bacon. Szeszulski, P. (1989). Familial resemblance in component reading, spelling, and language processes among subgroups of dyslexics. Dissertation Abstracts International, 49(8-B), 3474. Tallal, P., Ross, R., & Curtis, S. (1989). Familial aggregation in specific language impairment. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 54, 167–173. Written Language Is Natural 311 Taylor, I. (1976). Introduction to psycholinguistics. New York: Holt, Rinehart, & Winston. Taylor, I., & Taylor, M. (1995). Writing and literacy in Chinese, Korean and Japanese. Philadelphia: John Benjamins. Tomasello, M. (1992). First verbs: A case study in early grammatical development. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. Tomasello, M. (2000). Do young children have adult syntactic competence? Cognition, 74, 200–253. Tomasello, M. (2003). On the different origins of symbols and grammar. In M. H. Christiansen & S. Kirby, (Eds.), Language evolution (pp. 111–139). New York: Oxford University Press. Tomblin, J., & Buckwalter, P. (1998). Heritability of poor language achievement among twins. Journal of Speech, Language, & Hearing Research, 41, 188– 199. Tomblin, J. B. (1989). Familiar concentration of developmental language impairment. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 54, 287–295. Trainor, L., Austin, C. M., & Desjardins, R. N. (2000). Is infant-directed speech prosody a result of the vocal expression of emotions? Psychological Science, 11, 188–193. Treiman, R. (1993). Beginning to spell. New York: Oxford University Press. ∗Trinkaus, E., & Villeneur, I. (1991). Mechanical advantages of the Neanderthal thumb flexion: A test of an hypothesis. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 84, 249–266. Vachek, J. (1973). Written language. General problems and problems of English. The Hague: Mouton. Vachek, J. (1989). Written language revisited. Philadelphia: John Benjamins. Van Valin, R., & LaPolla, R. (1997). Syntax, structure, meaning and function. New York: Cambridge University Press. ∗Velichkovsky, B. M. (1996). Language development at the crossroads of biological and cultural interactions In B.M. Velichkovsky & D. M. Rumbaugh (Eds.), Communicating meaning: The evolution and development of language (pp. 23–63). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. von Gorlitzer, M. (1957). Zur Frage de paarig veranlagten Sprachzentren. Der Nervenartz, 28, 212–216. Walker, C. B. F. (1987). Reading the past: Cuneiform. Los Angeles: University of California Press. Waller,R. (1984). Typography and discourse. In R. Barr, M. L. Kamill, P. B. Mosenthal,&P. D. Pearson (Eds.), Handbook of reading research (Vol II), (pp. 341–380). New York: Longman. Wallon, H., & Lucrat, L. (1957). Graphisme et modele dans les dessins de l’infant. Journal de Psychologie, 3, 257–278. Waterson, B. (1997). The Egyptians. Oxford: Blackwell. Weistuch, L., & Schiff, M. (1996). Chromosomal translocation in a child with SLI and apraxia. Journal of Speech & Hearing Research, 39, 668– 671. Copyright of Reading Psychology is the property of Routledge and its content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's express written permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use.