Food preference of the green shore crab, Hemigrapsus oregonensis, before and after exposure to food treatments Chris Blackhurst Saint Martin’s University Dr. Hartman and Dr. Olney Senior Seminar April 23, 2007 Table of Contents Abstract 1 Introduction 2 Materials and Methods 4 Results 8 Discussion 11 Literature Cited 14 Acknowledgements 15 Abstract The green shore crab, Hemigrapsus oregonensis, is an abundant crab in much of the Pacific Northwest of the United States. They are found typically under protective objects, like rocks or sand, and scavenge the bottom of the intertidal areas for food. As most crabs, they use odor to detect their food. This study has found that H. oregonensis did not prefer to feed on beef or shrimp initially. After being fed beef or shrimp H. oregonensis choose to eat foods that they were familiar with over other foods or not feeding. An aquarium vacation feeder-fed control group was shown to prefer feeding over not feeding. H. oregonensis was tested in a modified Ymaze in three intervals of food treatment exposure; no exposure, one week exposure, two weeks exposure. 1 Introduction Oceanic intertidal zones are rich in species diversity and composition. Rocky intertidal zones particularly support a wide range of adaptations among species. Interactions among species are a delicate thing and small disturbances can have a big impact in a system with so many factors (Addessi, 1994). Understanding how different stimuli affect organisms and ecosystems as a whole is important to understand the impact humans have on such a system. The use of sensory input is the most basic form of interaction an organism has with its environment. One such sense is that of smell and chemosensitivity. The use of chemical stimuli is common among intertidal organisms. Chemosensitivity typically manifests in response to odor detection. While it is not intertidal the shrimp, Palaemonetes pugio, has displayed an attraction to specific chemoattractants in various food sources in a study by Carr and Derby. Using various natural and artificial mixtures of foods and their componenets Carr and Derby were able to show that some foods had specific chemoattractans which the shrimp preferred over others. Observation of P. pugio showed that the shrimp moved toward certain mixtures of chemoattractants more aggressively as well as displayed other increases in activity (Carr and Derby, 1986). In a similar study by Derby and Atema the Walking Legs Lobster Homarus americanus was excided and attracted to specific mixtures of amino acids and other organic compounds. 35 compounds were exposed to H. americanus as well as a number of extracts of prey. H americanus was observed to prefer the odor of prey items over chemical compounds individually. Some mixtures were also used that produced a response less that that of prey items but more than individual chemicals in H. americanus (Derby and Atema, 1982). The studys done 2 by both Carr and Derby, and Derby and Atema show that shrimp and lobster respond to chemical odor of prey respectively. Zimmer-Faust et al. was able to show a direct correlation between the presence of food odor and the navigation of intertidal crabs. Using fluorescent enhanced odor plumes of mimicking food sources it was seen that various species of intertidal crabs would be attracted towards the odor plumes. Crabs were less focused as the odor plume diffused in the water further away from the source however as the crabs approached the source their paths became more focused. This shows that some intertidal crabs use chemicals to navigate toward prey and food (Zimmer-Faust et al. 1995). Ristvey and Rebach have show that the crab Cancer irroratus actually responds to familiar foods more than unfamiliar foods. C. irroratus was fed one of two mussel types for a period of 28 days. After the 28 days the crabs were exposed to effluents made from each of the mussel types as well as a third. Observations were made concerning the level of activity the crabs displayed when exposed to these effluents. Ristvey and Rebach observed that crabs responded more actively to the effluent of the mussel which they had been feeding on. Such reactions may have been due to simple chemical memory which dictates preference or an actual choice for foods familiar to the crab (Ristvey and Rebach, 1999). Considering the widespread use of chemosensitivity shown among crustaceans it is expected that other crustaceans use chemicals sensing in similar manners. While C. irroratus is not a intertidal organism, similar utilization of odor response would be expected among intertidal species. Among intertidal crabs in Washington State, perhaps the most abundant is the green shore crab, Hemigrapsus oregonensis (Oliver and Schmelter, 1997). In this study, the food preference of intertidal shore crab, H. oregonensis, was tested when presented foods before and 3 after familiarization test the hypothesis that H. oregonensis will select food with which it is familiar more than other foods. Materials and Methods This study was conducted from February 13, 2007 to April 9, 2007. Hemigrapsus oregonensis were collected at low tide from Budd Inlet in Olympia Washington. The collection site was a rocky hill on the south side of the inlet near the Port of Olympia. The hill was artificially created during dredging of the inlet and inlaid with rocks for stability. Crabs were found under rocks on-site during low tides. Criteria for collection were crabs of carapace width greater than 2 cm. Sex of each crab was not a factor for collection. Crabs were kept temporarily in an acrylic container 18 cm x 32 cm x 13 cm with seawater between 5 cm and 7 cm covering them. Salinity of collection area was measured with a refractometer at 20 ppt and the temperature measured with a digital thermometer at 9o C. H. oregonensis are small crabs approximately 3cm to 5cm in carapace width and dark green in color. The legs usually have hair on them. H. oregonensis are typically found under rocks and other protecting structures in intertidal areas (Niesen, 1997). Housing and conditions Crabs selected for study were housed in a 208.2 L artificial seawater tank. Water temperature was 10o ± 3oC and salinity was kept at 22 ppt ± 3 ppt. These artificial conditions are modeled after conditions observed at collection site. Water was filtered mechanically through cotton and carbon mesh filters. Water flow was further enhanced through the use of a second pump to force more water though the filters. Tank acclimation and environment 4 Three populations of twenty crabs were kept in acrylic tanks measuring 8 cm x 32 cm x 13 cm. 7 – 12 cm of rock was placed along the bottom of the tubs to provide a substrate for the crabs. Each population had 15 males and 15 females. Acrylic tubs were submerged in the 55 gallon tanks to allow water to be circulated to maintain the temperature and to filter any waste. Crabs were allowed to acclimate to lab conditions for 24 hours before their first Y-maze test. During acclimation crabs were not fed to encourage a food choice response in the crabs during the initial interval. Modified Y-Maze The modified Y-maze used in this experiment consisted of an open box 28 cm x 13cm x 12cm of acrylic (Figure 1). Ramps coming from the middle of the box were constructed approximately 8 cm long. Each ramp was placed face to face forming an enclosed test area. A central pit was created by the ramps. This pit was filled with aquarium rock forming a surface which the crabs could navigate. Each ramp was built 3 cm short of the back wall, the opening formed trapped H. oregonensis in the end containing the food they chose during the trial. 5 Figure 1. This modified Y-maze allowed the crabs to assort to either side or stay in the center. Food was present in each end of the maze allowing crabs to peruse either food but not both, as crabs fall into the food chamber to access the food. Preparation of food Food was aged for two days at room temperature, approximately 18o C to 20o C to increase the potency of the food odor. After two days the food was frozen to keep the level of aging uniform for all food during the duration of the experiment. Before introduction into the experiment, the food was thawed and cut into pieces weighing 10 g. Uniform amounts of food were used to ensure that consumption of food among the groups could be watched comparatively. The foods used were fresh beef and fresh raw shrimp (Wight et al., 1990). Initial response to food After being allowed to acclimate to the artificial salt water environment the each crab population of 30 crabs were placed in the modified Y-Maze represented in Figure 1 and allowed to move freely in the maze for 2 hours. Observations were recorded on where the crabs were at 6 the end of the 2 hours. Each end of the maze contained a food treatment; one end of the maze had 10g of beef and while the other end had 10g of shrimp. Each food was rubbed across the surface of the respective back wall to maximize odor. Familiarization of food and feeding. In each trial populations of 30 crabs were maintained for two weeks in the tank. At the initial, one, and two week intervals each population was run through the Y-maze. This process was replicated three times. In each trial one population of 30 crabs was fed beef, another population was fed shrimp and another was fed a composite vacation feeder. The vacation feeder as well as the aforementioned aged shrimp and beef were added one of three respective populations. At the end of the first and second week all food was removed from the tanks and food withheld for one full day. After 1 day without food the crabs were tested in the Y-maze. After testing 10 g of food was then added to each respective population. Modified Y-Maze tests Observations were made as to how many crabs sought the food with which they were familiar as compared to the other food provided. Procedures concerning food testing methods were adapted from those used on large rock crabs by Ristvey and Rebach (1999).Food was placed in each end of the Y-maze, at one end was beef and at the other was shrimp. Each type of food was smeared across the back wall of one side of the maze respectively to maximize odor exposure. Vacation feeder food was omitted from testing due to its lack of odor and use as a neutral control. Data Analysis. The number of crabs that assorted themselves over two hours to food they had been conditioned with was compared to the number who assorted themselves to the food in which 7 they were not conditioned, as well as those that did not sort to any food during testing. Data were compared using a two-way analysis of variance test (ANOVA) to determine if there was significant difference in each feeding treatment of the number of crabs who sort to different foods and the time interval of testing. If the ANOVA showed no significant interaction (p>0.05) among the factors then the individual factors were examined. If there was interaction, one-way ANOVA tests were run for the treatment comparing the number of crabs to food choice for each individual time interval. If one-way ANOVA tests showed significance, a multiple comparisons were examined with Tukey tests. All statistical was conducted using Minitab statistical software (Minitab 2006). Results The number of Hemigrapsus oregonensis that ran through the modified Y-maze was recorded in terms of where the organisms were at the end of a two hour trial. Three replicates for all three feeding treatments were produced. Crabs were counted as either having sorted to beef or shrimp. Crabs that did not sort to either food were recorded as not being sorted. Two-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) tests were run for all replicates of each of the three treatments. The two-way ANOVA compared time since exposure to food to the type of food used. Two-way ANOVA testing for the beef-fed H. oregonensis showed a significant interaction among the factors of time and feeding treatment (F = 16.02; df = 4; p = 0.0005). Beef fed crabs showed a preference for the beef end of the maze over no preference or the shrimp end (F = 77.72; df = 2; p = 0.0005). There was no significant difference how crabs sorted and duration of exposure to each feeding treatment (F = 0.07; df = 2; p = .932). Two-way ANOVA results for H. oregonensis in the shrimp treatment had signifigant interaction among the factors of time and feeding treatment (F = 14.54; df = 4; p = 0.0005). 8 Crabs the shrimp-fed group chose the shrimp end of the maze significantly more often than no choice or the beef end of the maze(F = 80.49; df = 2; p = 0.0005). Shrimp-fed H. oregonensis also showed no significant difference in terms of length of exposure to food and how the crabs sorted (F = .001; df = 2; p = 0.991). A two-way ANOVA showed no significant interaction among feeding treatments, duration of exposure, and how the crabs sorted in the vacation fed group (F=0.263; df = 4; P = 0.069) so both feeding treatments and duration of exposure were examined independently. At the initial interval before H. oregonensis had been exposed to any food in captivity, there was no significant difference as to where the crabs sorted (F = 3.43; df = 2; p = 0.102). After being exposed to vacation feeders at the one week interval, H. oregonensis showed significant differences among food choices (F=19.50; df =2; p = 0.002). A Tukey test on the data showed that choices for shrimp were significantly greater than choosing to not pursue food (97.80% confidence level). At the two week interval vacation-fed crabs showed a significant difference in where they assorted themselves (F = 9.38; df = 2; p = 0.014). A subsequent Tukey test showed that H. oregonensis chose feeding significantly more than not pursuing food but did not prefer either food (97.80% confidence level). H. oregonensis tended to choose food it had been fed previously in both the beef treatments (Figure 2) and shrimp treatment (Figure 3). This trend was evident after one week of feeding and reinforced by similar repetitions after the second week of feeding. While Vacationfed H. oregonensis did not tend toward either beef or shrimp, they did tend to choose food, either beef or shrimp, over not feeding (Figure 4). 9 Beef-Fed 25 Number of Crabs 20 Beef 15 none Crabs 10 shrimp 5 0 initial week 1 week 2 Time Figure 2. Mean number of beef-fed crabs of three replicates that sorted to different treatments at the initial pretreatment interval, after one week of treatment and two weeks of treatment. After one week and two weeks of being fed beef, more H. oregonensis chose beef than either shrimp or no food. Error bars are represented as one standard error about the mean. Vacation-Fed 15 10 Beef Shrimp none Crabs 20 Number of Crabs 25 5 0 initial week 1 Week 2 Time Figure 3. Mean number of vacation-fed crabs of three replicates that sorted to different treatments at the initial pretreatment interval, after one week of treatment and after two weeks of treatment. It can be seen that H. oregonensis chose food options over not feeding. Error bars are represented as one standard error about the mean. 10 Shrimp-Fed 25 Beef 15 Shrimp none Crabs Number of Crabs 20 10 5 0 initial week 1 Week 2 Time Figure 4. Mean number of shrimp-fed crabs in three replicates that sorted to different treatments at the initial pretreatment interval, after one week of treatment and after two weeks of treatment. It can be seen that after one week and two weeks of being fed shrimp more H. oregonensis chose shrimp over either beef or not feeding. Error bars are represented as one the standard error about the mean. Discussion Several studies have shown that crustaceans use the olfactory sense to detect and pursue prey. In the field the blue crab, Callinectes sapidus, has been shown to forage using odor through their reaction to introduced odor chemicals. Odors of popular prey items were introduced into an area of the typical foraging ground of C. sapidus. In the treated C. sapidus was seen to forage more aggressively than untreated areas (Finelli et al., 2000). While other crustaceans such as the hermit crab, Cilibanarius antillensis, has been shown to associate physical locations to chemical cues. Sea grass odor was used to attract C. antillensis. The selected sea grass is the natural haven of C. antillensis and the observed attraction to the odor shows that the hermit crab uses the odor as a means of finding the seagrass. Chemosensitivity of C. antillensis is fine enough that such cues are detectable from most other odors. These distinctions are specifically diferentiat location from other used odors such as the odors of prey (Chiussi et al,. 2001). However among the 11 studies examining the importance of odor, few had been done for any species of crab in terms of odor memory. Data obtained through this study directly supports my hypothesis that H. oregonensis would choose food they had been previously exposed to over unfamiliar food. This study compared feeding treatments of beef, shrimp, and vacation feeder and time exposed to such treatments. H. oregonensis was found to statistically be more prone to seek feeding treatments they had been exposed to for food over other treatments. As seen in Figure 2. beef-fed crabs would later seek out beef over shrimp. This is complimented by Figure 4 in which those crabs fed shrimp can be seen perusing shrimp food over beef after feeding experience. Figure 3 shows no significant difference between choosing beef and those choosing shrimp in crabs fed vacation feeder treatment. This suggests that food selection in other treatments was not based on caloric benefit but rather that crabs chose food based on previous experience with that food. Food preference in Cancer irroratus was shown to be a function of odor learning as these crabs show preference to prey they are familiar with (Ristvey and Rebach 1999). My results similarly show that food selection in H. oregonensis is influenced by previous experience. These results, in conjunction with those of Ristvey and Rebach, suggest that crabs in general may use this chemical learning in prey selection. The specific design of this study did not differentiate between a true choice and a chemically dictated choice similar to the study by Ristvey and Rebach. In turn it remains unclear as to why these crabs choose familiar foods over others, which may indeed be higher in nutrients and calories. If such prey preference is a learned response it may be that crabs learn other things. It is possible that while crabs remember prey they have eaten recently, they may also remember how 12 they accessed such prey. A crab opening a clam may remember how it opened the clam and be able to apply that experience to future clams. This study has shown that H. oregonensis preferentially chose food based on experience. It does not show why familiar food is chosen over others. Future research should include a comparative analysis of food sources. To show these results coupled with showing that one food was significantly more valuable in terms of caloric value and other nutrients would suggest that odor preference is a function of simple chemical memory, a genetic development that allows crabs to survive. If crabs were to show a preference for the more valuable food it would show true learning, choosing the most beneficial food because it is the most beneficial. 13 Literature Cited Addessi, L. 1994. Human disturbance and long-term changes on a rocky intertidal community. Ecological Applications 4(4):786-797. Carr. W. E. S., Derby. C. D. 1986 Behavioral chemoattractants for the shrimp, Palaemonetes pugio: identification of active components in food extracts and evidence of synergistic mixture interactions. Chemical Senses. 11:49-56. Chussi, R., Diaz, H., Rittschof, D., Forward Jr., R.B. 2001. Orentation of the hermit crab Cilibanarius antillensis:effects of visual and chemicals cues. J. of Crustacean Biology. 21(3):593-605. Derby, C. D., and J. Atema. 1982. Chemosensitivity of walking legs of the lobster Homarus americanus: neurophysiological response spectrum and thresholds. Journal of Experimental Biology. 98:303-315. Finelli, C.M., Pentcheff, N.D., Zimmer, R.K., Wethey, D.S. 2000. Ohysical constraints on ecological process: a field test of odor mediated foraging. Ecology. 81(3):784-797. Minitab Inc. 2006. Minitab Release 15.1.0.0 Statistical Software. Minitab Inc., Quality Plaza, Pennsylvania State College. Niesen, T.M. 1997. Marine life of the Pacific Northwest. Gulf Publishing, Huston, Texas. 66-69 Oliver, J., Schmelter, A. Life history of the native shore crabs Hemigrapsus oregonensis and Hemigrapsus nudus and their distribution, relative abundance and size frequency distribution at four sites in Yaquina Bay, Oregon [Internet]. Corvallis (OR): Oregon State University; 1997 [cited 2007 Mar 15]. Available from http://oregonstate.edu/~yamadas /crab/ch5.htm Ristvey, A., Rebach, S. 1999. Enhancement of the response of rock crabs, Cancer irroratus, to prey odors following feeding experience. Biological Bulletin. 197(3):361-367. Wight, K., Francis, L., Eldridge, D. 1990. Food aversion learning by the Hermit Crab Pagurus granosimanus. Biological Bulletin, 178-3:205-209. Zimmer-Faust, R.K., C.M. Finelli, N.D. Pentcheff, and D.S. Wethey. 1995. Odor plumes and animal navigation in turbulent water flow: A field study. Biological Bulletin 188: 511516. 14 Acknowledgements I would like to thank Dr. Mary Jo Hartman for helping me design this experiment and understand the statistical analyses, both Dr. Hartman and Dr. Margaret Olney for repeated efforts to refine my presentation of this study, Cheryl Guglielmo for helping me set up this experiment again and again and giving me the freedom to fiddle with things until they were just right. I would also like to give special acknowledgement to Marie Payne for helping me in every step of this process and for keeping me sane through repeated setbacks and breakdowns.