Noradrenaline, Cognitive Reserve and Alzheimer’s Disease: A Hypothesis Ian H Robertson Trinity College Institute of Neuroscience and School of Psychology Trinity College Dublin 2 Ireland Address for correspondence: Institute of Neuroscience Lloyd Building Trinity College Dublin 2 Ireland Tel: +35318962684 Fax: +35318963183 Email iroberts@tcd.ie 1 Abstract The gap between symptoms and pathology in Alzheimer’s Disease has been explained by the hypothetical construct of ‘cognitive reserve’ – a set of variables including education, intelligence and mental stimulation which putatively allow the brain to adapt to – and hence mask - underlying pathologies by maintaining cognitive function in spite of underlying neural changes. This review proposes a hypothesis that a biological mechanism may mediate between these social/psychological processes on the one hand, and apparently reduced risk of Alzheimer’s Disease on the other, namely repeated activation of the noradrenergic system over a lifetime by the processes implicated in cognitive reserve. Noradrenaline’s neuroprotective effects both in vivo and in vitro, and its key role in mediating the neuroprotective effects of environmental enrichment on the brain, make NA’s key role in mediating cognitive reserve - by disease compensation, disease modification or a combination of both - a viable hypothesis. Key Words Alzheimer’s Disease; Cognitive Reserve; Cognition; Aging; Noradrenaline; Locus Coeruleus; Environmental Enrichment. 2 1. The cognition-pathology gap in Alzheimer’s Disease One of the major obstacles to developing effective treatments or preventions for Alzheimer’s Disease (AD) is the imperfect correlation between biological measures of pathology in the brain on the one hand – amyloid plaques, neurofibrillary tangles, PET-measured cerebral perfusion or volumetric MRI for instance - and measured cognitive function and real life performance on the other (McKhann, Knopman et al. 2011). In the famous ‘nun study’ for instance (Riley, Snowdon et al. 2002), no less than 32% of elderly participants with Braak Stage III and IV pathology (out of a maximum of 6 stages determined post-mortem) had normal memory function before death. Furthermore, while there was a modest but significant correlation of 0.57 between autopsy-determined pathology and general cognition among individuals who had some memory impairment, there was no significant relationship between pathology and global cognition in those with intact memory function, in spite of the existence of other types of other cognitive impairment in many of this latter group. A common explanation for this cognition-pathology discrepancy is that cognition and memory function are maintained at relatively high levels in spite of the developing underlying pathology, of which the presence of amyloid plaques may be one important disease-specific marker; this may happen because of compensatory adjustments made by the brain which help it reorganize to maintain function in spite of the developing pathology (Dubois, Feldman et al. 2010; McKhann, Knopman et al. 2011). The development of a human amyloid marker in the form of amyloid PET scanning with Pittsburgh compound B (PiB) (Klunk, Engler et al. 2004) appears to offer a promising advance towards better characterization of, and treatment for, such underlying amyloid pathology. Such a development was hoped to have help close the cognition-pathology gap. Unfortunately this gap remains very large in spite of this important development. Roe et al (Roe, Mintun et al. 2008) examined PiB amyloid load in relation to measures of cognitive deterioration in a sample of elderly individuals with and without diagnosed AD and did indeed find that those who were PiB positive showed very significantly lower cognitive function and significantly higher clinical dementia ratings than those who were PiB negative. There was one important caveat to this 3 finding, however – this reassuring relationship between pathology and cognition disappeared in the group with the highest levels (post-college) of education and was significantly attenuated on most measures for those with intermediate education (some college or graduate college education). The cognition-pathology gap among the best educated in other words, widened to the extent that no correlation remained between these two sets of variables. Such findings do not just apply to education as a variable, however. In a study of social networks (Bennett, Schneider et al. 2006), the researchers found that while post-mortem pathology and pre-death cognition showed a reasonable correlation among individuals with relatively sparse social networks, the cognition-pathology correlation again disappeared – as in the case for education and cognition in Roe’s PiB study – among groups with a high level of social contact and strong social networks. Discrepancies between cognition and pathology such as these have been explained by the concept of ‘cognitive reserve’, a concept first developed by Yakov Stern (Stern, Albert et al. 1999). Individual differences in the efficiency, capacity or flexibility of brain networks (‘neural reserve’ in Stern’s terminology) or individual differences in the ability to compensate for brain pathology (Stern’s ‘neural compensation’), may allow brains affected by Alzheimer’s pathology to maintain adequate cognitive functioning (Stern 2009). The aim of this paper is not to review comprehensively the variables and processes linked to cognitive reserve, as this has been well done elsewhere (Stern, Alexander et al. 1992; Stern, Gurland et al. 1994; Stern, Albert et al. 1999; Valenzuela and Sachdev 2006; Tucker and Stern 2011). Nor does the paper attempt to elucidate the distinction between cognitive reserve and the concept of ‘brain reserve’, which Stern and his colleagues propose refers to intra-individual differences in biological substrates of the brain leading to different degrees of resilience to the effects of disease or injury. Cognitive reserve and brain reserve have been used interchangeably by other authors such as Valenzuela and Sachdev (Valenzuela and Sachdev 2006), but the question posed in this paper pertains to a hypothetical biological mediating mechanism by which either cognitive or brain reserve may shape the considerable pathology- 4 cognition discrepancy commonly observed in Alzheimer’s Disease and other brain disorders. The magnitude of this cognition-pathology discrepancy is considerable, estimated by Valenzuela and Sachdev in their meta-analysis of education, occupation, IQ and mental activities components of brain reserve as a mean odds ratio of 0.54 for lowered risk of incident dementia over a median 7.1 year period (Valenzuela and Sachdev 2006). There are three main theoretical interpretations of this finding. The first is that the reserve variables and the reduced risk of AD diagnosis are correlated not due to any direct causal link, but rather because each is associated with a third common, possibly genetic, factor that causes both the high brain/cognitive reserve factor (educational and occupational attainment and IQ) and the increased resilience to the disease process. This view would hold that the observed correlation between, say, education and lowered risk of AD, is therefore more reflective of the pre-existing resilience than a direct effect of, say, education per se (Whalley, Deary et al. 2004). A Swedish study of identical twins, however, showed that those who had the minimal legal level of education for their age cohort had significantly higher levels of dementia than their identical twins who had higher levels of education, confirming that pre-existing genetic variables could not account for the cognitive reserve-symptom relationship (Gatz, Mortimer et al. 2007). The second theoretical explanation for this relationship is that the apparently protective variables such as education build the brain’s capacity to compensate for a disease process which in itself is unaffected by education, mental stimulation or social interaction. Such compensatory variables could range from the increased cortical volume that has been shown to arise from intensive new learning during exam preparation in students (Draganski, Gaser et al. 2006) or by learning a new skill such as juggling (Draganski, Gaser et al. 2004); it could also arise from cognitive-trainingrelated increases in white matter volume (Takeuchi, Sekiguchi et al. 2010) or from changes to critical neurotransmitter receptor densities (McNab, Varrone et al. 2009). Changes to brain regions crucial for compensatory adjustments such as the prefrontal cortex may play a particular role (Erickson, Colcombe et al. 2007). 5 The third theoretical explanation is that education, mental stimulation and social interaction directly impact the Alzheimer’s Disease process itself, and not only the brain’s capacity to compensate for the disease(Landau, Marks et al. 2012). Only the second and third of these theoretical positions propose a causal role for the protective effects of these cognitive reserve variables and the aim of this paper is to advance a hypothesis about a possible biological route (enhanced noradrenergic signaling) which might mediate between cognitive reserve and reduced AD vulnerability in both of these theoretical cases 2 and 3 above, which I will term ‘compensatory’ and ‘disease modifying’ respectively. I ask in other words whether variables such as education/IQ, mental stimulation and social engagement reduce risk of AD by improving the brain’s ability to compensate for disease as outlined above and/or by directly influencing the AD disease process itself. Because it is currently not possible to measure directly brain noradrenergic function in vivo in humans, however, it is first necessary to look to the animal literature on the cognitionpathology gap. 2. Environmental Enrichment and Neurodegeneration Environmental enrichment improves cognitive function in a range of species. Dogs randomly allocated to enriched environments over a period of more than two years in one study showed lower amyloid plaque burden in their brains at post mortem than dogs kept in standard environments (Pop, Head et al. 2010). Transgenic TgCRND8 Aβ-overproducing mice were randomly allocated to 60 days of enriched environment before Aβ plaques appeared or 60 days after they had begun to appear (Herring, Lewejohann et al. 2011), thus allowing comparison of the ‘preventative’ effects of enrichment and the ‘therapeutic, post-disease-onset’ effects. They found that the enriched environment before disease onset reduced the number and size of amyloid plaques, which the authors suggest may occur due to increased degradation and clearance of Aβ. ‘Therapy’, on the other hand, reduced the growth and fusion of plaque seeds, possibly by inhibiting Aβ aggregation, the authors suggested. These and several other studies (Lazarov, Robinson et al. 2005; Costa, Cracchiolo et al. 2007) suggests that one aspect of cognitive reserve - enrichment/mental stimulation - may at times directly influence various AD-linked pathologies, through for instance NA amelioration of amyloid cellular toxicity, and not only influence the 6 brain’s capacity to adapt to existing pathology. This series of studies suggest that independently of any cognitive reserve-enhancing mechanisms that may be taking place, allowing better adaptation to pathology, there are also effects of enrichment on the pathologies themselves, thus perhaps increasing ‘neural reserve’, in Stern’s terms. Recently, human evidence in favour of this hypothesis was obtained by Landau and colleagues (Landau, Marks et al. 2012) who found a significant correlation between estimated lifetime cognitive activity and amyloid burden the brain measured by PET [11C]PiB β-Amyloid imaging in 65 people with a mean age of 76. Greater participation in cognitively stimulating activities such as reading books or newspapers, writing letters or e-mails, going to the library or playing games across the lifespan was associated with reduced [11C]PiB while controlling for age, sex, and years of education. Older participants in the highest cognitive activity group had [11C]PiB uptake comparable to young controls, whereas those with lowest levels had [11C]PiB uptake comparable to patients with AD. While greater cognitive activity was associated with greater physical exercise, exercise however was not associated with [11C]PiB uptake. These two studies are in accord with the third theoretical position, namely that one aspect of cognitive reserve - stimulation/enrichment - may directly impact the disease process. Other studies, however, have found results more consistent with the second theory, namely that of compensatory capacity. For instance, one study with transgenic (APP) Aβ-overproducing mice, found that an enriched environment normalized memory performance in these mice in spite of increased neuritic plaque burden, a finding more consistent with the neural compensation hypothesis of heightened resilience to extant pathology (Jankowsky, Melnikova et al. 2005) and a similar finding was obtained with APPsw transgenic mice (Arendash, Garcia et al. 2004) and with APP-23 mice (Wolf, Kronenberg et al. 2006). A similar conclusion has been reached in a longitudinal study of educational level in humans, with 872 brain donors, of whom 56% were demented at death: these authors found that while longer years in education were associated with decreased dementia risk, this association was found to be statistically independent from the effects of underlying pathology; this led them to conclude that the education mitigated the impact of pathology rather than directly influencing pathological processes (Brayne, 7 Ince et al. 2010). A similar conclusion was reached by another human study using estimated premorbid IQ (American National Adult Reading Test) as a proxy marker of lifetime environmental enrichment; the authors found that this measure of cognitive reserve predicted cognitive performance independently of, and additively to, the effects of amyloid and other measured pathologies (Vemuri, Weigand et al. 2011). While environmental enrichment may build reserve both altering key pathologies linked to Alzheimer’s Disease, the human evidence for this is weaker than the animal data, and the strongest evidence is for such enrichment to help maintain improved cognition through other, possibly compensatory, mechanisms. So how does environmental enrichment produce its effects on cognition? 3. Mechanisms of Action of Environmental Enrichment on Cognitive Function Environmental enrichment across a range of species improves cognitive function and there are a range of mechanisms mediating these effects including neurogenesis, synaptogenesis and increased levels of Brain Derived Neurotrophic Factor (BDNF) and related neurotrophins, among others (vanPraag, Kempermann et al. 2000). Reduced intracerebral inhibition and epigenetic changes at the level of chromatin structure have also been implicated (Baroncelli, Braschi et al. 2010). In addition, environmental enrichment has significant effects on neurotransmitter function, and in particular and in particular on noradrenergic tone. Noradrenaline concentrations in brains of mice spending 40 days in enriched versus standard environments increased significantly in parieto-temporo-occipital cortex, the cerebellum and the pons/medulla oblongata (Naka, Shiga et al. 2002) with no corresponding changes observed in serotonin or dopamine levels in these brain regions. Selective enrichment-induced modulation of noradrenaline release in mouse hippocampus via pre-synaptic NMDA receptors has also been observed (Grilli, Zappettini et al. 2009). The specific aspects of environmental enrichment that improved a key cognitive function - memory - in mice, was investigated in another study (Veyrac, Sacquet et al. 2008). These researchers compared enrichment via novelty versus enrichment via complexity, in the context of odour enrichment and its effects on odour memory. Daily exposure to single novel odorants improved memory while daily exposure to a complex bouquet of odours which remained stable over the enrichment period, did not. Furthermore, the daily changing single odour novelty 8 condition produced increased neurogenesis in the olfactory bulb, while the stable complex odour environment did not. This study showed novelty as a key feature of the cognitively enriching and neuroplasticity-enhancing aspects of the enriched environment. These positive effects of enrichment were however mediated by noradrenaline, the authors showed, because they were blocked under labetalol, a mixed b with a1- adrenoceptor antagonist. The short term enhancing effects of novelty on hippocampal-dependent memory had already been demonstrated, as had the mediating role of noradrenaline (Kitchigina, Vankov et al. 1997). Furthermore, age-related LTP deficits in rats have been shown to be significantly reduced by novelty, and these effects may be mediated noradrenergically (Sierra-Mercado, Dieguez et al. 2008). In humans, Tulving and colleagues found that the probability of long-term storage of information varies directly with the novelty of the information(Tulving, Markowitsch et al. 1996). Noradrenaline activation then, may be one important mechanism by which environmental enrichment improves cognitive function, and novelty may be a key feature of environmental enrichment’s beneficial effects. These effects would affect the brain’s ability to compensate for disease - for instance by neurogenesis-induced raised cortical volume, increased connectivity or other effects on brain structure and function that would allow better compensation for the AD disease process. They would not, however speak to the possibility of a direct effect on the disease process itself. Is there then any evidence of a more specific relationship of NA function to Alzheimer’s Disease? 4. Noradrenaline and Alzheimer’s Pathology The locus coeruleus (LC) is the major source of NA in the brain and projects synaptically and extra-synaptically to the entire cerebral cortex, as well as thalamic nuclei, limbic structures and the hippocampus; the basal ganglia regions is the only major structure that does not receive input from the LC (Sara 2009). While AD research has strongly focussed on deficits in the cholinergic system (Babic 1999; Terry and Buccafusco 2003; Ikonomovic, Klunk et al. 2011), a number of studies have shown significant cell loss in the LC in Alzheimer’s Disease (Tomlinson, Irving et al. 1981; Dringenberg 2000; Matthews, Chen et al. 2002; Szot, S.S et al. 2006; Insua, Suáreza et al. 2010). In addition, LC degeneration is seen in very early, pre-AD 9 disorders such as Mild Cognitive Impairment (MCI) and the drop in NA levels in such patients is tightly linked to the progression and extent of memory dysfunction and cognitive impairment (Grudzien, Shaw et al. 2007). Furthermore epidemiological research shows that the dopamine β-hydroxylase -1021C/T polymorphism, which influences NA availability in the brain, is a significant risk factor for AD, with genotypes indicating low NA availability having as much as a doubled risk of AD (Combarros, Warden et al. 2010). The LC therefore may suffer early degeneration in the AD disease process. Since NA plays a role as an anti-inflammatory molecule, one impact of the resulting decrease in NA-ergic neurons is likely to be on inflammatory processes. NA, for instance, downregulates transcription of inflammatory genes in astrocytes and microglia (Feinstein, Heneka et al. 2002), and reduces amyloid neural toxicity via anti-inflammatory mechanisms, both in vitro and in vivo in APP mouse models (Heneka, Nadrigny et al. 2010). NA also stimulates BDNF production (Mannari, Origlia et al. 2008) which in turn reduces amyloid toxicity on hippocampal neurons (Counts and Mufson 2010) and also promotes neurogenesis (Masuda, Nakagawa et al. 2012). Furthermore, NA in vitro has been shown to rescue cholinergic (Traver, Salthun-Lassalle et al. 2005) and dopaminergic neurons (Troadec, Marien et al. 2001) by reducing oxidative stress; more generally, NA also stimulates both DA and glutamate release in the brain (Grinberg, Rueb et al. 2011). This combination of mechanisms - anti-inflammatory, BDNF-increasing, neurogenesis-inducing, amyloid toxicity-reducing, amyloiddepleting, dopamine and cholinergic cell-rescuing, dopamine and glutamatestimulating, constitutes a set of NA-induced neuroprotective mechanisms which may offer a possible biological basis for the phenomenon of cognitive or brain reserve. Such mechanisms offer the theoretical possibility of a direct noradrenergicallymediated effect on the disease process and not just on the compensatory process. 5. Noradrenaline’s role in cognitive function There is one critical missing link in the hypothesis outlined above. While noradrenaline may have a number of neuroprotective and compensation-enhancing effects on the brain, and cognitive reserve variables such as mental activity and education may be associated with lower levels of AD pathology, is there any evidence 10 that these cognitive reserve variables have any effects on NA function? To address this question, a brief review of the role of NA in cognitive processes is required. The brain’s locus coeruleus (LC)–noradrenaline system is crucially involved in task engagement and optimizing performance according to environmental contingencies (Aston-Jones and Cohen 2005). High-frequency phasic LC activity is elicited by salient or task-relevant stimuli, and the resultant release of NA to the cerebral cortex enhances stimulus processing by selectively increasing neuronal gain within taskrelevant regions - in short, it increases signal to noise ratio for important signals (Aston-Jones, Chiang et al. 1991; Aston-Jones, Rajkowski et al. 1994; Sara 2009). The role of phasic LC activity in facilitating stimulus processing is supported by animal studies that highlight the phasic LC response as an important antecedent to appropriate behavioral responding in stimulus detection paradigms (Aston-Jones, Rajkowski et al. 1994; Usher, Cohen et al. 1999). Based primarily on such intracranial recordings from animals, the adaptive-gain theory of LC-NE function (Aston-Jones and Cohen 2005) states that relative levels of tonic and phasic LC activity relate to task performance in a manner that reflects the classic Yerkes–Dodson arousal curve (Yerkes and Dodson 1908): Performance and phasic LC responding are optimal at an intermediate level of tonic LC activity, but shifts toward either end of the tonic activity continuum are associated with declining performance and nonspecific or attenuated phasic responses. The importance of this LC arousal function in humans has been highlighted by pharmacological and genetic studies that corroborate the role of NA as a critical determinant of engagement and task performance on tests of attention (Smith and Nutt 1996; Coull, Nobre et al. 2001; Minzenberg, Watrous et al. 2008; Greene, Bellgrove et al. 2009; Murphy, Robertson et al. 2011). Assessing NA levels directly in the living human brain is not currently possible, but recent research suggests that LC/NA activity may be indexed by the pupillary response to motivationally salient or novel stimuli (Aston-Jones and Cohen 2005; Einhauser, Stout et al. 2008; Gilzenrat, Nieuwenhuis et al. 2010; Gabay, Pertzov et al. 2011; Jepma and Nieuwenhuis 2011), including in primates (Bouret and Richmond 2009). For many decades, pupil dilation has been shown to co-vary with ongoing 11 cognitive processes in the human brain (Beatty 1982; Loewenfeld 1993). This is quite distinct from the cholinergically-mediated hypersensitivity in pupil dilation response to a cholinergic antagonist, tropicamide, that has been demonstrated in people suffering from Alzheimer’s Disease (Scinto, Daffner et al. 1994), and also in a reduced dilation response to light (Fotiou, Fountoulakis et al. 2000). In contrast to this, the NA-based pupil dilation is a response to a number of different cognitive challenges which shows considerable variation within and between health individuals of all ages. Cognitive or mental effort increases pupil size, in proportion to the difficulty or challenge, across the domains of memory (Kahneman and Beatty 1966; Peavler 1974), problem-solving (Hess and Polt 1964; Bornemann, Foth et al. 2010), auditory perceptual processing (Kahneman and Beatty 1967), attention (Murphy, Robertson et al. 2011; Laeng, Ørbo et al. 2012) and mathematical cognition (Kahneman 1973), among others. Furthermore, stimulation of the LC with consequent up-regulation of NA activity has been shown to improve cognitive, perceptual and memory performance in monkeys and rodents (Berridge and Waterhouse 2003; Sara 2009; Bornemann, Foth et al. 2010). Bearing in mind the evidence that human NA activity may be indexed by pupillary responses to cognitive challenge, let us now consider the evidence for pupillary dilation responses - our proxy marker for NA activity - in relation to the four cognitive reserve variables. 6. Cognitive Reserve and Noradrenergic Function Is there any evidence that four of the most common elements of ‘cognitive reserve’ Education/IQ, Mental Activity, Social Interaction and Enriched/Novel Environments influence noradrenergic activity as indexed by pupillary measures? Let us consider each in turn. Education/IQ Education levels are very strong predictors of reduced risk of diagnosis with Alzheimer’s Disease (Stern, Gurland et al. 1994; Kidron, Black et al. 1997; Gatz, Mortimer et al. 2007; Stern 2009; Brayne, Ince et al. 2010; Dumurgier, Paquet et al. 12 2010; Roe, Mintun et al. 2010) as are premorbid IQ levels (Tucker and Stern 2011; Vemuri, Weigand et al. 2011), with education levels and the IQ Quotient being highly correlated (Ceci and Williams 1997). Van Der Meer and colleagues (Van Der Meer, Beyer et al. 2010) showed that adults with higher than average IQ (fluid intelligence) showed similar pupil dilation to those shown by people of average IQ. When the analogies were made more difficult, however, the high IQ participants showed much bigger pupil dilation to these problems, which the authors interpret as the deployment of greater cognitive resources in those of higher intelligence; this finding was replicated by Bornemann et al (Bornemann, Foth et al. 2010). Given the evidence that pupil dilation indexes noradrenergic activity (Gilzenrat, Nieuwenhuis et al. 2010; Murphy, Robertson et al. 2011; Laeng, Ørbo et al. 2012), one element of the putative ‘resource’ in question is likely to be increased noradrenaline levels. Increased NA levels improve and ‘clean’ neural signals in the brain (Sara 2009) and contribute to the increased level of optimal arousal which more challenging tasks demand, in line with the original observation by Yerkes and Dodson (Yerkes and Dodson 1908) and in line with what is known about the inverted-U function of neurotransmitters such as NA(Arnsten, Steere et al. 1996). These limited data offer some support to the hypothesis that Education/IQ may influence cognitive reserve in part via the enhanced noradrenergic activity in the brain, with possible resulting consequent neuroprotective or compensation-enhancing downstream effects as outlined earlier in this article. Mental Activity Mental activity is correlated with reduced risk of Alzheimer’s Disease (Wang, Karp et al. 2002; Verghese, Lipton et al. 2003; Valenzuela and Sachdev 2006; Wolf, Kronenberg et al. 2006; Valenzuela and Sachdev 2009; Valenzuela, Matthews et al. 2011; Landau, Marks et al. 2012), though there is no consensus as to whether this is purely due to mental activity’s effects in improving brain robustness (eg neuron density), as Valuenzuela and colleagues have argued (Valenzuela, Matthews et al. 2011), or whether in addition actual AD pathology (eg amyloid load) may also be affected(Landau, Marks et al. 2012). Mental activity and engagement, however, robustly and reliably increases pupil dilation, with the size of the dilation proportional to the level of the mental challenge 13 (Kahneman 1973). This has been shown across a range of cognitive challenges, ranging from auditory pitch discrimination (Kahneman and Beatty 1967) to the Stroop effect, where the difficult incongruent words elicit a much greater pupillary response than the congruent or control words (Laeng, Ørbo et al. 2012) and to perception of ambiguous figures, where pupil dilation occurs immediately before the alternative percept comes into awareness (Einhauser, Stout et al. 2008). Similar findings apply in the memory domain, where pupils have been shown to dilate to a greater extent when participants correctly remember previously learned items, even when the words were presented acoustically. The pupil dilation was also greater when items when memory encoding was deep compared to shallow (Otero, Weekes et al. 2011). Undoubtedly mental activity, in particular challenging and engaging mental activity, stimulates pupil dilation and therefore may be triggering NA release in the brain. To the extent that over a lifetime a person is - maybe many hundreds of times per day repeatedly stimulating the locus coeruleus (LC) to secrete noradrenaline, this could offer a possible mechanism by which such activity could both build a brain better able to resist the disease process through mechanisms such as NA- and BDNF- enhanced neurogenesis or synaptogenesis, for instance, and/or actually offer neuroprotection to neurons through its anti-inflammatory and other related mechanisms as outlined earlier. Novelty While novelty and change have not been regarded as contributing to cognitive reserve in the human literature, the evidence cited earlier in this article on the role of environmental enrichment on protecting against AD-type symptoms in rodents suggested that novelty may be the crucial element mediating between effects of enrichment and its beneficial neural and cognitive effects (Veyrac, Sacquet et al. 2008). Given that factors such as rich leisure and occupational activities throughout life have been linked to cognitive reserve, the possibility arises in the light of Veyrac’s study, that novelty may be a potentially potent component of these effects. But is there any evidence that in humans, novelty has any specific effects on the noradrenergic system? It seems that there is: Steiner and colleagues examined electroencephalographic, skin conductance and pupil dilation responses to auditory stimuli. While all three types of response were apparent in response to target tones, the researchers found that ‘the pupillary dilation response, however, demonstrated an 14 unexpected sensitivity to stimulus novelty only’ (p 1648) (Steiner and Barry 2011). Other studies have shown a specific pupillary sensitivity to surprise, which is closely linked to novelty (Reisenzein, Bördgen et al. 2006). To the extent that the pupillary response is a marker of NA activity, there is therefore evidence that novelty - a possible key factor in mediating rodent environmental enrichment effects - does indeed selectively upregulate NA activity in humans also. Social Interaction Social interaction may be a special case of mental activity, but given that human beings are a fundamentally social species, evolved to live in groups, it warrants separate attention, particularly in the light of evidence that social networks can, like education/IQ and mental activity, strongly moderate the relationship between AD pathology and cognitive function, to the extent that no correlation between postmortem pathology and cognitive function while alive was found in individuals with strong social networks (Bennett, Schneider et al. 2006). This finding has been replicated (Crooks, Lubben et al. 2008), though a further study suggested that it may be the quality more than the quantity of social networks that protects against AD(Amieva, Stoykova et al. 2010). There is relatively little direct evidence about the effects of social stimuli on pupil dilation/NA response, though Kuchinke and colleagues showed that the pupil response to words spoken with either positive or negative prosody was much larger than to words spoken in a neutral voice (Kuchinke, Schneider et al. 2011). 7. Working Memory and Cognitive Reserve IQ is one of the key elements of cognitive reserve (Stern 2009; Vemuri, Weigand et al. 2011), and a considerable amount of research has shown that the central cognitive component of fluid intelligence is working memory capacity - the ability to hold and manipulate a constantly changing stream of information (Engle, Tuholski et al. 1999). In that context, it is very interesting to note the results of a follow up of 801 elderly individuals over a period of 4.5 years (Wilson, Mendes de Leon et al. 2002). In line with previous studies on mental activity, the authors found that baseline participation in cognitively stimulating activities was protective against subsequent cognitive decline, with those at the highest - 90th percentile - of baseline cognitive activity being 47% less likely to develop AD than were those at the lowest - 10th percentile - 15 of baseline mental activity: there was no such link with level of physical activity. Furthermore, high baseline cognitive activity predicted a lower rate of cognitive decline, but level of mental activity was specifically predictive of rate of change in only two categories of cognitive function - working memory capacity and perceptual speed. Level of baseline mental activity did not significantly predict rate of change of other types of memory or of visuospatial ability, for instance. Working memory capacity, therefore, may be a possible mediator between two aspects of cognitive reserve – IQ and mental activity – on the one hand, and reduced risk of cognitive decline on the other. This finding is particularly interesting given the evidence that working memory capacity has been shown to be improvable with training (Jaeggi, Buschkuehl et al. 2008; Klingberg 2010), and indeed to result in alterations of dopamine receptor densities in prefrontal cortex (McNab, Varrone et al. 2009) as well as in increases in white matter density (Takeuchi, Sekiguchi et al. 2010). While perceptual speed is also a potential candidate mediator, and is also, like working memory, trainable (Green and Bavelier 2003; McGugin, Tanaka et al. 2011), the evidence for its biological effects are so far weak. On the other hand, NA seems to play a crucial role in age-related decrements in working memory capacity: Wang and colleagues showed that in primates, a significant age-related decrement in working memory manifested in reduced delay-phase neuronal firing in prefrontal cortex, was normalized by up-regulating the noradrenergic system (Wang, Gamo et al. 2011). 8. Conclusions and Implications The question posed in this paper concerns a possible mechanism that may mediate between cognitive reserve on the one hand, and reduced risk of Alzheimer’s Disease diagnosis on the other. It is hypothesized that the key elements of cognitive reserve educational level, IQ, mental and social engagement - all involve up-regulation of the noradrenergic system which is otherwise depleted with age - and that such optimization of noradrenergic activity may reduce the risk of AD. The mechanisms by which AD activity may be protective are multifarious but fall into two broad categories, roughly in line with the compensatory and disease-modifying theories outlined in the introduction. The compensatory theory proposes that cognitive reserve is associated with a number of non-specific changes to the brain - cortical volume, white matter density, synaptic 16 density, neurotransmitter receptor densities among others - and that the cognitionpathology gap is explained by a bigger and better connected brain being able to reorganize around the pathology caused by the AD disease process. Noradrenaline plays a key role in the learning and stimulation that underpins these cognitive reserve variables because of its unique neuromodulation and long-term-potentiationenhancing (Harley 1987) characteristics and central role in mediating the effects of environmental enrichment on neurogenesis, synaptogensis, the stimulation of BDNF and many other mechanisms. An early review made this observation about the LC/NA system: ‘ (it) affects synapses throughout the CNS, suppressing most, but permitting or even accentuating activity in those that are transmitting novel or significant stimuli...this favours the development of persistent facilitatory changes in all synapses that are currently in a state of excitation.’ (Kety 1972) Kety also predicted that NA would have a crucial role in the cornerstone of learning and neuronal plasticity, a prediction subsequently confirmed after the discover of long-term potentiation, LTP (Harley 1987). This included evidence that NA increases caused by stimulation of the LC improved memory retrieval in rats (Devauges and Sara 1991) . Sara’s review of the LC/NA system (Sara 2009) concluded that its intra and extracellular dispersion throughout the brain exceeded that of most other neurotransmitters, with the only major region not receiving input from the LC being the basal ganglia. Noradrenaline is centrally and specifically involved in mediating environmental stimulation, attention and learning and therefore is a strong candidate for mediating the compensatory aspect of cognitive reserve. The resulting synaptogenesis and neurogenesis, among other mechanisms, leads to a brain structure better able to resist pathological processes; the human research (Brayne, Ince et al. 2010; Valenzuela, Matthews et al. 2011) tends to support this compensatory model of mediation, whereby the cognitive reserve acts brain’s ability to withstand the pathology. But what about the pathology itself, does NA have a role, as the disease-modifying model proposes, in reducing the AD pathology itself? The evidence from human research is scanty, and where it does exist, does not tend to support this hypothesis. The animal research, on the other hand, offers the possibility that in addition to the 17 compensatory mechanism, NA optimization may also reduce pathology, or at least diminish the neural toxicity-inducing effects of pathology (Heneka, Nadrigny et al. 2010), and there is some limited human evidence in support of this also (Landau, Marks et al. 2012). The figure schematically outlines the hypothesized role of NA in cognitive reserve, outlining the two possible modes of action that NA may have in facilitating both compensation to the disease processes, and also to modifying the disease processes themselves. Figure about here While accepting that the balance of the human evidence is for an indirectly protective effect of cognitive reserve on resistance to pathology rather than on the pathology itself - the compensatory theory - a hypothetical role for noradrenaline in mediating between cognitive reserve and reduced risk of AD offers a number of potentially useful lines of research to test this hypothesis. Mental stimulation or cognitive enrichment effects may for instance be potentiated by noradrenergic agonists, with the sorts of enhanced rehabilitation effects that have been found, for instance, in stroke rehabilitation (Crisostomo and Duncan 1988; Walker-Batson, Curtis et al. 2001). Use of possible NA markers such as pupil dilation my further offer a method for more precisely measuring how cognitive reserve variables affect the ability to compensate for neurodegenerative pathologies. If this is successful, then it may be possible to quantify and hence control for these variables which otherwise complicate the evaluation of novel pharmaceutical agents whose symptomatic effects may be otherwise masked in patients with high cognitive reserve. Furthermore, by identifying such a possible biological correlate of cognitive reserve, it may be possible to more easily identify people in the very early stages of Alzheimer’s Disease, with a view to offering pharmacological treatment before too much neuronal death has occurred because of amyloid toxicity or other mechanisms. Finally, the intriguing possibility supported largely by animal research - that NA may directly affect the AD disease process, is one which demands further testing and exploration. Acknowledgements I would like to thank Atlantic Philanthropies for their generous support of my research in this area, through their funding of the NIEL (Neuroenhancement for 18 Inequalities in Elder Lives) programme at Trinity College Institute of Neuroscience. I would also like to thank my colleagues Redmond O’Connell, Joshua Balsters, Sabina Brennan, Robert Coen, Marina Lynch and Carlo Miniussi for their careful comments and help with this manuscript, and Sabina Brennan for preparing the figure. Disclosures There are no conflicts or interest or other disclosures to make. Bibliography Amieva, H., R. Stoykova, et al. (2010). "What Aspects of Social Network Are Protective for Dementia? Not the Quantity But the Quality of Social Interactions Is Protective Up to 15 Years Later." Psychosomatic Medicine 72(9): 905-911. Arendash, G. W., M. F. Garcia, et al. (2004). "Environmental enrichment improves cognition in aged Alzheimer's transgenic mice despite stable [beta]-amyloid deposition." NeuroReport 15(11): 1751-1754. Arnsten, A. F. T., J. C. Steere, et al. (1996). "The contribution of a-2 noradrenergic mechanisms to prefrontal cortical cognitive function: Potential significance to Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder." Archives of General Psychiatry 53: 448-455. Aston-Jones, G., C. Chiang, et al. (1991). Discharge of noradrenergic locus coeruleus neurons in behaving rats and monkeys suggests a role in vigilance. Progress in Brain Research. C. D. Barnes and O. Pompeiano. Amsterdam, Elsevier Science. 88: 501-520. Aston-Jones, G. and J. D. Cohen (2005). "An integrative theory of locus coeruleus-norepinephrine function: adaptive gain and optimal performance." Annu Rev Neurosci 28: 403--450. Aston-Jones, G., J. Rajkowski, et al. (1994). "Locus-coeruleus neurons in monkey are selectively activated by attended cues in a vigilance task." Journal of Neuroscience 14: 4467-4480. Babic, T. (1999). "The cholinergic hypothesis of Alzheimer's disease: a review of progress." Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry 67(4): 558. Baroncelli, L., C. Braschi, et al. (2010). "Nurturing brain plasticity: impact of environmental enrichment." Cell Death and Differentiation 17: 10921103. 19 Beatty, J. (1982). "Task-evoked pupillary responses, processing load, and the structure of processing resources." Psychological Bulletin 91: 276–292. Bennett, D. A., J. A. Schneider, et al. (2006). "The effect of social networks on the relation between Alzheimer's disease pathology and level of cognitive function in old people: a longitudinal cohort study." The Lancet Neurology 5(5): 406-412. Berridge, C. W. and B. D. Waterhouse (2003). "The locus-coeruleusnoradrenergic system: modulation of behavioral state and statedependent cognitive processes." Brain Research Reviews 42: 33-84. Bornemann, B., M. Foth, et al. (2010). "Mathematical cognition: individual differences in resource allocation." ZDM 42(6): 555-567. Bouret, S. and B. J. Richmond (2009). "Relation of Locus Coeruleus Neurons in Monkeys to Pavlovian and Operant Behaviors." Journal of Neurophysiology 101(2): 898-911. Brayne, C., P. G. Ince, et al. (2010). "Education, the brain and dementia: neuroprotection or compensation?" Brain 133(8): 2210-2216. Ceci, S. J. and W. M. Williams (1997). "Schooling, intelligence, and income." American Psychologist 52: 1051-1058. Combarros, O., D. R. Warden, et al. (2010). "The dopamine β-hydroxylase 1021C/T polymorphism is associated with the risk of Alzheimer's disease in the Epistasis Project." BMC Medical Genetics 11: 162. Costa, D. A., J. R. Cracchiolo, et al. (2007). "Enrichment improves cognition in AD mice by amyloid-related and unrelated mechanisms." Neurobiology of Aging 28(6): 831-844. Coull, J. T., A. C. Nobre, et al. (2001). "The Noradrenergic Alpha 2 Agonist Clonidine Modulates Behavioural and Neuroanatomical Correlates of Human Attentional Orienting and Alerting." Cerebral Cortex 11(1): 7384. Counts, S. E. and E. J. Mufson (2010). "Noradrenaline activation of neurotrophic pathways protects against neuronal amyloid toxicity." Journal of Neurochemistry 113(3): 649-660. Crisostomo, E. and P. Duncan (1988). "Evidence that amphetamine with physical therapy promotes recovery of motor function in stroke patients." Annals of Neurology 23: 94-97. Crooks, V., J. Lubben, et al. (2008). " Social Network, Cognitive Function, and Dementia Incidence Among Elderly Women " American Journal of Public Health 98: 1221-1227. Devauges, V. and S. J. Sara (1991). "Memory retrieval enhancement by locus coeruleus stimulation: evidence for mediation by Œ≤-receptors." Behavioural Brain Research 43(1): 93-97. Draganski, B., C. Gaser, et al. (2004). "Neuroplasticity: Changes in grey matter induced by training." Nature 427(6972): 311-312. 20 Draganski, B., C. Gaser, et al. (2006). "Temporal and Spatial Dynamics of Brain Structure Changes during Extensive Learning." The Journal of Neuroscience 26(23): 6314-6317. Dringenberg, H. C. (2000). "Alzheimer's disease: more than a ‘cholinergic disorder' — evidence that cholinergic–monoaminergic interactions contribute to EEG slowing and dementia." Behavioural Brain Research 115(2): 235-249. Dubois, B., H. H. Feldman, et al. (2010). "Revising the definition of Alzheimer's disease: a new lexicon." The Lancet Neurology 9(11): 1118-1127. Dumurgier, J., C. Paquet, et al. (2010). "Inverse association between CSF AŒ≤ 42 levels and years of education in mild form of Alzheimer's disease: the cognitive reserve theory." Neurobiology of Disease 40(2): 456-9. Einhauser, W., J. Stout, et al. (2008). "Pupil dilation reflects perceptual selection and predicts subsequent stability in perceptual rivalry." Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 105(5): 1704-1709. Engle, R. W., S. W. Tuholski, et al. (1999). "Working memory, short-term memory, and general fluid intelligence: A latent-variable approach." Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 128: 309-331. Erickson, K. I., S. J. Colcombe, et al. (2007). "Training-Induced Functional Activation Changes in Dual-Task Processing: An fMRI Study." Cereb. Cortex 17(1): 192-204. Feinstein, D. L., M. T. Heneka, et al. (2002). "Noradrenergic regulation of inflammatory gene expression in brain." Neurochemistry International 41(5): 357-365. Fotiou, F., K. N. Fountoulakis, et al. (2000). "Changes in pupil reaction to light in Alzheimer's disease patients: a preliminary report." International Journal of Psychophysiology 37(1): 111-120. Gabay, S., Y. Pertzov, et al. (2011). "Orienting of attention, pupil size, and the norepinephrine system." Atten. Percept. Psychophys 73: 123–129. Gatz, M., J. A. Mortimer, et al. (2007). "Accounting for the relationship between low education and dementia: A twin study." Physiology & Behavior 92(1-2): 232-237. Gilzenrat, M., S. Nieuwenhuis, et al. (2010). "Pupil diameter tracks changes in control state predicted by the adaptive gain theory of locus coeruleus function." Cognitive, Affective and Behavioral Neuroscience 10(2): 252269. Green, C. S. and D. Bavelier (2003). "Action video game modifies visual selective attention." Nature 423(6939): 534-537. Greene, C., M. A. Bellgrove, et al. (2009). "Noradrenergic genotype predicts lapses in sustained attention." Neuropsychologia 47: 591-594. Grilli, M., S. Zappettini, et al. (2009). "Exposure to an enriched environment selectively increases the functional response of the pre-synaptic NMDA 21 receptors which modulate noradrenaline release in mouse hippocampus." Journal of Neurochemistry 110(5): 1598-1606. Grinberg, L. T., U. Rueb, et al. (2011). "Brainstem: neglected locus in neurodegenerative diseases." Frontiers in Neurology 2. Grudzien, A., P. Shaw, et al. (2007). "Locus coeruleus neurofibrillary degeneration in aging, mild cognitive impairment and early Alzheimer's disease." Neurobiol Aging 28(3): 327--335. Harley, C. W. (1987). "A role for norepinephrine in arousal, emotion and learning?: Limbic modulation by norepinephrine and the Kety hypothesis." Progress in Neuropharmacology and Biological Psychiatry 11: 419-458. Heneka, M. T., F. Nadrigny, et al. (2010). "Locus ceruleus controls Alzheimer's disease pathology by modulating microglial functions through norepinephrine." Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 107(13): 6058-6063. Herring, A., L. Lewejohann, et al. (2011). "Preventive and therapeutic types of environmental enrichment counteract beta amyloid pathology by different molecular mechanisms." Neurobiology of Disease 42(3): 530538. Hess, E. H. and J. M. Polt (1964). "Pupil size in relation to mental activity during simple problem-solving." Science 143: 1190–1193. Ikonomovic, M. D., W. E. Klunk, et al. (2011). "Precuneus amyloid burden is associated with reduced cholinergic activity in Alzheimer disease." Neurology 77(1): 39-47. Insua, D., M.-L. Suáreza, et al. (2010). "Dogs with canine counterpart of Alzheimer's disease lose noradrenergic neurons." Neurobiology of Aging 31(4): 625-635. Jaeggi, S. M., M. Buschkuehl, et al. (2008). "Improving fluid intelligence with training on working memory." Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 105(19): 6829-6833. Jankowsky, J. L., T. Melnikova, et al. (2005). "Environmental Enrichment Mitigates Cognitive Deficits in a Mouse Model of Alzheimer's Disease." The Journal of Neuroscience 25: 5217-5224. Jepma, M. and S. Nieuwenhuis (2011). "Pupil Diameter Predicts Changes in the Exploration-Exploitation Trade-off: Evidence for the Adaptive Gain Theory." Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 23(7): 1587-1596. Kahneman, D. (1973). Attention and effort. Englewood Cliffs, NJ, Prentice-Hall. Kahneman, D. and J. Beatty (1966). "Pupil diameter and load on memory." Science 154: 1583–1585. Kahneman, D. and J. Beatty (1967). "Pupillary responses in a pitchdiscrimination task." Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics 2(3): 101-105. 22 Kety, S. S. (1972). "The possible role of the adrenergic systems of the cortex in learning." Res Publ Assoc Res Nerv Ment Dis 50: 376-89. Kidron, D., S. E. Black, et al. (1997). "Quantitative MR volumetry in Alzheimer's disease." Neurology 49(6): 1504-1512. Kitchigina, V., A. Vankov, et al. (1997). "Novelty-elicited, Noradrenalinedependent Enhancement of Excitability in the Dentate Gyrus." European Journal of Neuroscience 9(1): 41-47. Klingberg, T. (2010). "Training and plasticity of working memory." Trends in Cognitive Sciences 14(7): 317-324. Klunk, W. E., H. Engler, et al. (2004). "Imaging brain amyloid in Alzheimer's disease with Pittsburgh Compound-B." Annals of Neurology 55(3): 306319. Kuchinke, L., D. Schneider, et al. (2011). "Spontaneous but not explicit processing of positive sentences impaired in Asperger's syndrome: Pupillometric evidence." Neuropsychologia 49(3): 331-338. Laeng, B., M. Ørbo, et al. (2012). "Pupillary Stroop effects." Cognitive Processing 12(1): 13-21. Landau, S. M., S. M. Marks, et al. (2012). "Association of Lifetime Cognitive Engagement and Low {beta}-Amyloid Deposition." Archives of Neurology: archneurol.2011.2748. Lazarov, O., J. Robinson, et al. (2005). "Environmental Enrichment Reduces AŒ≤ Levels and Amyloid Deposition in Transgenic Mice." Cell 120(5): 701-713. Loewenfeld, I. (1993). The Pupil: Anatomy, Physiology, and Clinical Applications. Detroit Wayne State Univ Press. Mannari, C., N. Origlia, et al. (2008). "BDNF Level in the Rat Prefrontal Cortex Increases Following Chronic but Not Acute Treatment with Duloxetine, a Dual Acting Inhibitor of Noradrenaline and Serotonin Re-uptake." Cellular and Molecular Neurobiology 28: 457-468. Masuda, T., S. Nakagawa, et al. (2012). "Noradrenaline increases neural precursor cells derived from adult rat dentate gyrus through beta2 receptor." Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry 36(1): 44-51. Matthews, K. L., C. P. L. H. Chen, et al. (2002). "Noradrenergic changes, aggressive behavior, and cognition in patients with dementia." Biological Psychiatry 51(5): 407-416. McGugin, R. W., J. W. Tanaka, et al. (2011). "Race-Specific Perceptual Discrimination Improvement Following Short Individuation Training With Faces." Cognitive Science 35(2): 330-347. McKhann, G. M., D. S. Knopman, et al. (2011). "The diagnosis of dementia due to Alzheimer‚Äôs disease: Recommendations from the National Institute on Aging-Alzheimer‚Äôs Association workgroups on diagnostic 23 guidelines for Alzheimer's disease." Alzheimer's & dementia : the journal of the Alzheimer's Association 7(3): 263-269. McNab, F., A. Varrone, et al. (2009). "Changes in Cortical Dopamine D1 Receptor Binding Associated with Cognitive Training." Science 323(5915): 800-802. Minzenberg, M. J., A. J. Watrous, et al. (2008). "Modafinil Shifts Human Locus Coeruleus to Low-Tonic, High-Phasic Activity During Functional MRI." Science 322(5908): 1700-1702. Murphy, P. R., I. H. Robertson, et al. (2011). "Pupillometry and P3 index the locus coeruleus–noradrenergic arousal function in humans." Psychophysiology 48: 1532–1543. Naka, F., T. Shiga, et al. (2002). "An enriched environment increases noradrenaline concentration in the mouse brain." Brain Research 924(1): 124-126. Otero, S. C., B. S. Weekes, et al. (2011). "Pupil size changes during recognition memory." Psychophysiology: no-no. Peavler, W. S. (1974). "Pupil size, information overload, and performance differences. Psychophysiology." 11: 559–566. Pop, V., E. Head, et al. (2010). "Synergistic Effects of Long-Term Antioxidant Diet and Behavioral Enrichment on beta-Amyloid Load and NonAmyloidogenic Processing in Aged Canines." Journal of Neuroscience 30(29): 9831-9839. Reisenzein, R., S. Bördgen, et al. (2006). "Evidence for strong dissociation between emotion and facial displays: The case of surprise." Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 91: 295-315. Riley, K. P., D. A. Snowdon, et al. (2002). "Alzheimer's neurofibrillary pathology and the spectrum of cognitive function: Findings from the Nun Study." Annals of Neurology 51(5): 567-577. Roe, C. M., M. A. Mintun, et al. (2008). "Alzheimer Disease and Cognitive Reserve: Variation of Education Effect With Carbon 11-Labeled Pittsburgh Compound B Uptake." Arch Neurol 65, 1467-1471. Roe, C. M., M. A. Mintun, et al. (2010). "Alzheimer disease identification using amyloid imaging and reserve variables." Neurology 75(1): 42-48. Sara, S. J. (2009). "The locus coeruleus and noradrenergic modulation of cognition." Nature Reviews Neuroscience 10: 211-223. Scinto, L. F., K. R. Daffner, et al. (1994). "A potential noninvasive neurobiological test for Alzheimer's disease." Science 266(5187): 10511054. Sierra-Mercado, D., D. Dieguez, et al. (2008). "Brief novelty exposure facilitates dentate gyrus LTP in aged rats." Hippocampus 18(8): 835843. Smith, A. and D. Nutt (1996). "Noradrenaline and attention lapses." Nature 380: 291. 24 Steiner, G. Z. and R. J. Barry (2011). "Pupillary responses and event-related potentials as indices of the orienting reflex." Psychophysiology 48(12): 1648-1655. Stern, Y. (2009). "Cognitive reserve." Neuropsychologia 47(10): 2015-2028. Stern, Y., S. Albert, et al. (1999). "Rate of memory decline in AD is related to education and occupation - Cognitive reserve?" Neurology 53(9): 19421947. Stern, Y., G. E. Alexander, et al. (1992). "Inverse relationship between education and parietotemporal perfusion deficit in Alzheimer's disease." Annals of Neurology 32(3): 371-375. Stern, Y., B. Gurland, et al. (1994). "Influence of Education and Occupation on the Incidence of Alzheimer's Disease." JAMA: The Journal of the American Medical Association 271(13): 1004-1010. Szot, P., W. S.S, et al. (2006). "Compensatory changes in the noradrenergic nervous system in the locus ceruleus and hippocampus of postmortem subjects with Alzheimer's disease and dementia with Lewy bodies." Journal of Neuroscience 26: 467–478. Takeuchi, H., A. Sekiguchi, et al. (2010). "Training of Working Memory Impacts Structural Connectivity." The Journal of Neuroscience 30(9): 3297-3303. Terry, A. V., Jr. and J. J. Buccafusco (2003). "The Cholinergic Hypothesis of Age and Alzheimer's Disease-Related Cognitive Deficits: Recent Challenges and Their Implications for Novel Drug Development." J Pharmacol Exp Ther 306(3): 821-827. Tomlinson, B. E., D. Irving, et al. (1981). "Cell loss in the locus coeruleus in senile dementia of Alzheimer type." Journal of Neurological Science 49: 419–428. Traver, S., B. n. d. Salthun-Lassalle, et al. (2005). "The Neurotransmitter Noradrenaline Rescues Septal Cholinergic Neurons in Culture from Degeneration Caused by Low-Level Oxidative Stress." Molecular Pharmacology 67(6): 1882-1891. Troadec, J.-D., M. Marien, et al. (2001). "Noradrenaline provides long-term protection to dopaminergic neurons by reducing oxidative stress." Journal of Neurochemistry 79(1): 200-210. Tucker, A. and Y. Stern (2011). "Cognitive Reserve in Aging." Current Alzheimer Research 8(4): 354-360. Tulving, E., H. J. Markowitsch, et al. (1996). "Novelty and Familiarity Activations in PET Studies of Memory Encoding and Retrieval." Cerebral Cortex 6(1): 71-79. Usher, M., J. D. Cohen, et al. (1999). "The role of locus cueruleus in the regulation of cognitive performance." Science 283: 549-553. Valenzuela, M. and P. Sachdev (2009). "Can Cognitive Exercise Prevent the Onset of Dementia? Systematic Review of Randomized Clinical Trials 25 with Longitudinal Follow-up." American Journal of Geriatric Psych 17(3): 179-187 10.1097/JGP.0b013e3181953b57. Valenzuela, M. J., F. E. Matthews, et al. (2011). "Multiple Biological Pathways Link Cognitive Lifestyle to Protection from Dementia." Biological Psychiatry(0). Valenzuela, M. J. and P. Sachdev (2006). "Brain reserve and dementia: a systematic review." Psychological Medicine 36(04): 441-454. Van Der Meer, E., R. Beyer, et al. (2010). "Resource allocation and fluid intelligence: Insights from pupillometry." Psychophysiology 47(1): 158169. vanPraag, H., G. Kempermann, et al. (2000). "Neural consequences of enviromental enrichment." Nature Reviews Neuroscience. Vemuri, P., S. D. Weigand, et al. (2011). "Cognitive reserve and Alzheimer's disease biomarkers are independent determinants of cognition." Brain 134(5): 1479-1492. Verghese, J., R. B. Lipton, et al. (2003). "Leisure Activities and the Risk of Dementia in the Elderly." N Engl J Med 348(25): 2508-2516. Veyrac, A., J. Sacquet, et al. (2008). "Novelty Determines the Effects of Olfactory Enrichment on Memory and Neurogenesis Through Noradrenergic Mechanisms." Neuropsychopharmacology 34: 786-795. Walker-Batson, D., S. Curtis, et al. (2001). " A double-blind, placebocontrolled study of the use of amphetamine in the treatment of aphasia." Stroke 32: 2093-2097. Wang, H.-X., A. Karp, et al. (2002). "Late-Life Engagement in Social and Leisure Activities Is Associated with a Decreased Risk of Dementia: A Longitudinal Study from the Kungsholmen Project." Am. J. Epidemiol. 155(12): 1081-1087. Wang, M., N. J. Gamo, et al. (2011). "Neuronal basis of age-related working memory decline." Nature 476: 210-213. Whalley, L. J., I. J. Deary, et al. (2004). "Cognitive reserve and the neurobiology of cognitive aging." Ageing Research Reviews 3(4): 369-382. Wilson, R. S., C. F. Mendes de Leon, et al. (2002). "Participation in Cognitively Stimulating Activities and Risk of Incident Alzheimer Disease." JAMA: The Journal of the American Medical Association 287, 742-748. Wolf, S. A., G. Kronenberg, et al. (2006). "Cognitive and Physical Activity Differently Modulate Disease Progression in the Amyloid Precursor Protein (APP)-23 Model of Alzheimer's Disease." Biological Psychiatry 60(12): 1314-1323. Yerkes, R. M. and J. D. Dodson (1908). "The relation of strength of stimulus to rapidity of habit-formation." Journal of Comparative and Neurological Psychology 18: 459-482. 26 Figure Legend Schematic role of hypothesized role of noradrenaline in mediating between cognitive reserve and reduced risk of AD. 27