Gender differences in children's conceptions of competence and motivation in physical education.Find More Like This Authors: Lee[a], Amelia M. Fredenburg[b], Karen Belcher[c], Don Cleveland[a], Nancy Sport, Education & Society; Oct99, Vol. 4 Issue 2, p161, 14p, 2 charts ABSTRACT This study investigated the competence perceptions and motivational beliefs of 50 physical education students who had adopted stereotypical perceptions of two gender oriented physical activities, dance and basketball. Specifically, the research asked students to explain why basketball is sometimes viewed as more appropriate for boys and dance is viewed as more appropriate for girls. The research also aimed to understand why students think they will not be as skilled and why many seem not to try in activities not viewed as gender appropriate. Participants were 50 fifthgraders who had personally adopted stereotypical perceptions regarding appropriateness of basketball and dance for male and female participation. A standardized openended interview technique was utilized to ascertain what mediated student viewpoints about competence and motivation and how children viewed and assessed teacher motivational practice. Results indicated that competence and motivational beliefs of students with well-established sexrole conceptions are driven by a sense of gender appropriateness. For decades researchers have used theories of motivation, such as self-efficacy (Bandura, 1969, 1986, 1989), expectancy value (Atkinson, 1964), and goal orientation (Nicholls, 1984) to understand why some children feel more competent than others and to explain differences in their motivational levels. These models have focused on the concept of perceived ability or competence as a mediating construct in achievement behaviour. Because a great deal of empirical work has established links between students' ability perceptions and their motivation, persistence, affective reactions and choices of activities to engage in (Eccles et al., 1984; Nicholls, 1984; Stipek & MacIver, 1989), some researchers have focused on helping teachers select teaching approaches that will enhance competence beliefs (Ames, 1992). In general, teachers have been encouraged to bind a student's success to effort rather than ability, and to emphasize improvement rather than performing better than others in the class. The goal of this research was to study the competence perceptions of children in physical education and to clarify their beliefs about why some children are more motivated than others. We also asked children to assess the motivational influences of four different teaching practices. Gender Differences in Perceptions of Competence Over the years, several researchers have reported gender differences in children's beliefs about their own competence in various movement and sport activities, and these differences must be considered in studies of ability-related perceptions. In general, boys consistently report higher perceptions of their overall physical competence and are more positive than gifts about their ability in most traditional sport activities (Eccles et al., 1989; Harter, 1982; Marsh et al., 1984). There is evidence to suggest, however, that females might not display a lack of confidence in all situations (Lenney, 1977). For example, research has documented that sex linked movement tasks can mediate gender differences in ability perceptions, with males displaying more confidence on masculine-typed tasks and females displaying more confidence on feminine-typed tasks (Clifton & Gill, 1994; Lirgg, 1991; Sanguinetti et al., 1985). While masculinity and femininity have multiple meanings that are socially constructed (Vygotsky, 1978), masculinetyped tasks are generally those requiring strength, power, and competitiveness. Typical femininetyped tasks, however, are associated with the attributes of graceful movement qualities. This distinction is consistent with Western society's definition of masculinity as behaviour that is aggressive, competitive, and tough (Streitmatter, 1994). Thus, team sports will typically receive a masculine label while activities such as dance and gymnastics will be defined as feminine. From these studies there is agreement that stereotypes can have powerful influences on children's thinking about their own competence and thus might limit their effort and persistence to those activities considered sex-appropriate. Although there is agreement in the literature that sex stereotypes influence beliefs about competence, few researchers have actually assessed how children view and label various tasks. Before the specific research questions in this study could be answered it was important to confirm that stereotypes about movement activities do exist at young ages. Researchers interested in studying gender differences in ability perceptions for tasks stereotyped as masculine or feminine have been warned against using sex-typing from previous studies without establishing that the perceptions about the task are the same for the group being studied (Lirgg, 1991). For decades, researchers (e.g. Metheny, 1965) have attempted to identify activities according to sex-type by asking respondents to rate activities they consider acceptable for male or female participation. While there are some inconsistencies among studies, there is agreement for the extreme masculine and feminine ratings. Typically team sport activities such as football, basketball, and soccer are considered more masculine, and creative activities such as dance and gymnastics are considered more feminine (Clifton & Gill, 1994; Csizma et al., 1988). If boys and girls differ in their task specific expectancies for sex-typed movement and sport activities, these differences could mediate motivational beliefs and behaviours in physical education classes. While gender alone cannot explain differential motivational and achievement patterns in students, it is important for teachers to understand the stereotypical views of their students before they can provide learning environments that will develop positive attitudes. Recent research indicates, for example, that overall young males value sport activities more than females do and some believe that females as a whole do not have the natural ability to be successful in physical education (Lee et al., 1995). A Focus on Explanatory Models In recent years researchers have sought to explain how gender images influence the experiences male and female students will have in physical education. Satina and her colleagues (Satina et al., 1998), for example, used patriarchal consciousness (Lorde, 1984) to demonstrate how curricular offerings and teacher practices contribute to a social construction of gender and account for the low motivational and achievement patterns in female students. Other researchers (e.g. Wright, 1997) have analysed how a teacher's use of language can create a learning environment which positions female students as marginal in relation to the dominant practice of traditional team sports. The level of motivation and non-compliance of the females in athletic and highly competitive classes confirms their femininity (Wright, 1996, 1997). Despite this initial focus on explanatory models, subjects in the research focusing on gender differences in self-confidence on sex-typed tasks have rarely been asked to explain the basis of their views. Early childhood experiences and the way girls and boys are socialized into movement and sport activities will influence how students perceive, interpret, and respond to teachers and the curriculum they offer. It is clear that if young girls have gender-typed beliefs incorporated into their beliefs about their own competence in movement and sport they will have lower expectations for success and will value only those activities perceived as appropriate for their gender (Meece & Courtney, 1992). Perhaps more important for teachers to understand are the beliefs, goals and attitudes of children who have positive and/or negative views of their ability in stereotyped activities. There is little information to explain why children believe certain sports are more appropriate for males than for females. Most of the research, especially in the US, designed to study gender differences in ability perceptions for tasks labelled masculine or feminine has relied on surveys and questionnaires with little attempt to study students' thinking about the different activities and to understand the rationale underlying their motivational beliefs. Recently a number of researchers have explored, through open-ended interviews, students' thinking about the general nature of physical education (see Graham, 1995). For example, Portman (1995) asked questions to determine students' views of effort, and to determine which students have fun during physical education. Veal & Compagnone (1995) and Lee et al. (1995) studied students' perceptions of their own effort and skill. Children's thinking about competence in movement has been influenced by culture and gender expectations and the stories children tell about different types of learners are important to a more complete description of motivational characteristics (Maehr & Nichols, 1980; Xiang et al., 1995). Previous research has assumed that motivational beliefs are rather general individual characteristics and has tended to ignore students' justifications for their gender-role labels and their beliefs about competence for themselves and others in movement and sport. This study, by way of contrast, was designed to examine what children think and feel about children's competence and participation in sex-typed movement activities. The Teacher's Role A major goal of educational reform efforts today is to identify ways that teachers can help students feel more competent and thus increase their levels of motivation in all school related activities. Considerable research supports the notion that teachers can enhance students' levels of motivation by evaluating them on effort and improvement rather than ability, emphasizing individualized learning, and providing task related feedback that would assist them in their efforts to improve (Ames, 1992; Brophy, 1987). These teachers would work with students on an individual basis and focus on technique rather than performance. Students would be encouraged to select challenging but attainable tasks. Praise and encouragement planned to direct student attention to a point in technique is consistent with this approach and might help students focus on skill improvement. If teachers succeed in creating this mastery oriented environment then students, regardless of their competence beliefs, should be more intrinsically motivated to learn. Futhermore, much learning in physical education and school in general is product oriented and the environment encourages a student to focus on the quantity of performance rather than the quality (Brophy, 1983; Goudas, & Biddle, 1994). Even though there is evidence that a mastery environment is more likely to enhance motivation in students some teachers use social comparison to recognize winners and skilled performers. The use of extrinsic rewards such as stickers and tokens, or the public display of students' achievement are examples of social comparison practices used by teachers. When the recognition practices used by teachers focus on skill achievement and performance outcomes it is possible that the rewards will undermine intrinsic motivation (Ames, 1992). Students, especially those who compare unfavourably with others in the class, begin to view themselves as lacking ability and this view in turn can decrease effort and interest (Ames, 1992). However, there is some evidence to indicate that when rewards are given for effort rather than performance, goal directed behaviour and feelings of competence can be enhanced (Blumenfeld, 1992). Taken together, the literature reviewed suggests that teacher practices can have an impact on a range of motivational variables including the enhancement or determinants of ability self-perceptions. While many educational practices can increase students' motivation to learn, it can be argued that the effectiveness of each practice will depend in part, on how it is perceived and defined by the learner. Before a complete understanding of the effectiveness and potential harmfulness of teachers' efforts to enhance student motivation is possible the perspective of the students must be considered. This study examined children's perspectives about competence and motivational beliefs in gender oriented movement activities. Specifically, the following questions were addressed: (1) how do children with stereotypical beliefs justify their decisions about student competence and motivation in sex-linked tasks, and (2) what beliefs do children have about the effectiveness of various strategies used by teachers to enhance all students' motivation to learn? Method Participants and Procedures for Group Assignment This study is part of a larger project designed to investigate age and gender differences in performance and learning expectancies for movement activities. The total sample included 745 first, third, and fifth grade males and females (M = 376, F = 369) drawn from physical education classes in six different public and private schools located in different geographical regions of a State in the south-eastern part of the US. The schools were chosen to provide a good crosssection of the State in terms of social class and academic achievement. Parental consent was obtained for all subjects. Participants for the present study were 50 fifth grade students (M = 27, F = 23), aged 10-11 years, who met the criteria established for four homogeneous groups with stereotypical perceptions of sport activities in general and the accompanying beliefs about their own competence (e.g. boys are more skilled in basketball and girls are more skilled in dance). The identification of these groups required a two step process. First, the entire sample (N = 745) was asked to respond to a self-report instrument designed to determine gender appropriateness of movement and sport activities. Using a five-point Likert scale, students ranked gymnastics, dance, jogging, soccer, tennis, roller skating, football, softball, basketball, cheerleading, jump rope and volleyball as: 1 = definitely for girls, 2 = mostly for girls, 3 = both for boys and girls, 4 = mostly for boys, or 5 = definitely for boys. These physical activities have been used in previous research on sport stereotypes and gender (Csizma et al., 1988; Lirgg, 1991), and with the exception of cheerleading are typically included in a physical education curriculum. Second part of the survey involved students responding to four questions about each of the 12 activities. The questions were phrased to elicit information concerning (1) students' perceived competence about their current abilities, (2) their confidence in their ability to learn the activity in the future, (3) the extent to which they valued the activity, and (4) their expressed interest in learning the activity. The wording of the questions was suggested initially by Eccles et al. (1983) and has been used by other researchers since that time (e.g. Clifton & Gill, 1994). The second question which was designed to assess the children's assessments of their confidence to learn the activity was used in this study as a measure of each child's perceived learning expectations. Specifically, the children were asked to indicate on a five-point Likert scale how well they would do if their teacher taught a unit (1 = not very well, 2 = not well, 3 = OK, 4 = well, and 5 = very well). To select activities that the children viewed as sex-linked the mean scores from the self-report instrument were ranked from low (i.e. more appropriate for girls) to high (i.e. more appropriate for boys). The activity with the lowest mean, rated more feminine was cheerleading (M = 1.5). The activity with the highest mean, rated most masculine, was football (M = 4.5). Other activities labelled definitely for gifts or mostly for girls were dance (M = 2.1) and gymnastics (M = 2.4), while those determined mostly or definitely for boys were basketball (M = 3.5) and soccer (M = 3.5). The ranks of the combined male and female means for the activities included in this study were quite similar to those obtained by other researchers (Csizma et al., 1988), with cheerleading, dance, and gymnastics viewed as activities more appropriate for females, and football, basketball, and soccer viewed as more appropriate for males. Though not the most extreme, basketball and dance were selected for in-depth analysis in this study because these activities were part of the required curriculum at each of the participating schools. For the entire sample (N = 745) there was the expected gender difference (p < 0.01) in responses to the question designed to get information about students' confidence levels for learning basketball and dance. Males (M = 4.4) were more confident than females (M = 3.8) in basketball and females (M = 4.3) were more confident than males (M = 3.1) in dance. The selection of the 50 fifth graders to be used in this phase of the study was based on their rankings concerning the gender appropriateness/inappropriateness of basketball and dance, along with their self-perceptions about ability to learn these activities. A homogeneous group with stereotypical perceptions was created. Members of this cohort included males and females who indicated with a four or five rating that basketball was predominately for males, and who with a one or two rating indicated that dance was predominately for females. This group was then subdivided into male and female cohorts who appeared to have personally adopted or internalized such stereotypical perceptions by predicting that they could learn appropriate gender activities or conversely that they could not learn activities that were inappropriate for their gender. This purposeful criterion referenced sampling technique (Patton, 1990) resulted in an unbalanced portion of males and females in four groups. Group A consisted of boys (n = 16) who indicated that dance was predominately for females and projected a low ability to learn dance themselves. Group B consisted of girls (n = 13) who indicated that dance was predominately for females and projected a high ability to learn dance themselves. Group C consisted of boys (n = 11) who indicated that basketball was predominately for males and further projected a high ability to learn basketball themselves. Group D consisted of girls (n = 10) who indicated that basketball was predominately for males and who further projected a low ability to learn basketball themselves. Interview A standardized open-ended interview technique (Patton, 1980) was utilized to ascertain what factors mediated student viewpoints about competence and motivation and how children viewed and assessed teacher motivational practices. Interviews were 20-30 minutes in length and occurred in the school setting. The procedures for the interview were adopted from those first used by Thorkildsen et al. (1994) to study children's perceptions of practices influencing motivation. The interviewer explained that the purpose of the interview was to find out what students think about the kinds of activities they learn in physical education. They were assured that there were no right or wrong answers and that anything they said would remain anonymous. The interviewer then obtained children's permission to tape record the interview. Justifications for beliefs. To begin the interview session the interviewer shared with the student hypothetical situations about stereotypical motivational beliefs and behaviours of males and females. The wording was adjusted according to the activity. For example, the interviewer explained that some boys are not very motivated in dance class because they think dance is more appropriate for girls than for boys. Sometimes they just stand around and wish they could participate in other activities instead. They also think they will not do as well in dance as girls. Following this explanation, the children were asked if they know boys like the ones described and all said yes. Next the interviewer asked questions about why the children felt some activities were more for girls and why girls try harder. Questions such as: Why do boys think dance is more appropriate for girls? Why do boys seem not to try? Why would girls be more motivated to learn dance? Follow up questions were used to clarify students' perceptions. The wording in the story was adjusted for basketball suggesting that some girls would not be motivated Evaluation of teacher practices. The next set of interview questions presented to each student asked for a reaction from the students to four possible teaching strategies which might motivate both males and females to learn dance or basketball. The interviewer told the children, 'I'm going to describe some things teachers might do to help girls as well as boys want to learn basketball (wording was changed for dance). I want you to think about the practices and tell me what might work and if the strategies are good for teachers to try.' The following strategies for basketball were described to the children. Strategy 1--Individualized Mastery. The teacher works with each student on an individual basis according to his or her skill level. if there are some students that are highly skilled the teacher might say 'you can already do this so why don't you practise these activities which are harder.' The teacher helps the ones in class who are having trouble by suggesting easier tasks to practice. The students who are not successful with the task are also provided information about their movement that might help them be more successful. Strategy 2--Recognition for Performance Outcomes. In this class the teacher gets real excited when students do well and tells the good athletes how skilled they are at basketball. This teacher keeps records of how successful each student is in all the practice activities and recognizes those who do well by posting their names on the bulletin board. Pictures of the skilled students might be displayed. The teacher encourages the lower skilled students to work harder so they can be recognized. Strategy 3--Rewards for Performance Outcomes. This teacher rewards every student who succeeds at a practice task by giving them a token. These tokens can be accumulated during the week and each Friday the students with ten tokens can go to the gym store and trade for various sport items such as ball caps, jump ropes, tickets to basketball games, and books about famous athletes. Strategy 4--Rewards for Effort. In this class the teacher also gives tokens but rewards students for working hard and trying rather than performing to a specific level. If students stay on task and put a lot of effort in the practice activities then they will get tokens. The students that fool around and do not take the activities seriously do not get any tokens. The wording was changed for the descriptions of the teachers in dance classes. For example the mastery oriented teacher would encourage the skilled dancers to practice harder steps and would provide individual help for those who are having trouble. The teacher who recognizes performance outcomes would display pictures of the skilled dancers and encourage the less skilled to work harder so they can be recognized. In strategy three, the teacher rewards students who can perform the dances with tokens which can be exchanged for items such as dance tapes, t-shirts or tickets to dance concerts. The teacher who rewards effort would give tokens to students who work hard and try during dance class. Following the description of each strategy children were asked a series of questions designed to get them to evaluate the strategy. Questions included: 'Would this strategy make all students in the class want to learn basketball (or dance)?"Why or why not?"Would the girls (boys) want to learn basketball (dance) in this class?' Finally, the strategies were presented to the children two at a time in a paired comparison format. For each pair children were asked to determine which teacher would be more successful in motivating both boys and girls to learn basketball (or dance) and to fully explain their answers. Responses were recorded. Analyses Inductive analysis was used to describe the content of the interview transcripts (Bogdan & Biklen, 1992; Lincoln & Guba, 1985; Patton, 1980). A content analysis of the children's justifications for competence and motivational beliefs was used to gain an understanding of their stereotypical views. After reading through a sample of the transcriptions several times, two researchers unified statements used by students to justify their motivational beliefs. Initial categories were formed by organizing the statements into several themes that emerged from the data. Categories were discussed and finally collapsed into three primary categories with sample statements used to define each theme. Using the sample responses as a guide the four researchers independently categorized each of the 342 responses by gender into one of the categories. A 95% agreement between the coders was achieved. For the paired comparisons children were asked to indicate which strategy wound be more effective compared to every other strategy. For example, a student would be asked to choose between strategies one and two, one and three, one and four, two and three, two and four, and three and four. Again children were asked to explain their answers. Frequencies and percentages for each strategy were calculated and reported. Finally, an interpretive analysis was done on the children's entire interview transcripts to determine more about their perspectives of gender differences and competence in sex-linked movement activities. For this analysis the researchers read each transcript and discussed consistencies in children's statements about gender-related competence and motivation in sex-linked activities. The interview data were categorized into themes that would best describe students' theories about gender appropriateness and teachers' efforts to motivate students. Children's statements regarding their justifications, their explanations, and the grounds for their choices of teacher strategies were used to define and describe motivation from the perspective of the learner. Triangulation in qualitative research is a way of improving the probability that the interpretations of the data are credible and trustworthy (Lincoln & Guba, 1985). In this study triangulation involved the use of multiple researchers across quantitative and qualitative data. To verify the content analysis and the strategy rankings independent coders classified the statements and tallied the frequencies for each strategy. The children's motivational beliefs were verified by the research group sorting and analysing the justifications and descriptions looking for and discussing consistencies and inconsistencies. Results Justifications for Competence and Motivational Beliefs The results from the interview questions which asked children to explain their views about appropriateness and competence in basketball and dance are presented in Table 1. The children gave multiple responses, each of which was coded. Thus, the distribution across categories is based on the total set of responses (369 for basketball, and 183 for dance). Both males and females had more comments to make about basketball than dance, and for each activity males were more responsive than females. Many of the responses for both basketball and dance related to the social acceptance of the activity. As shown in Table 1, the males used 53 (53%) social acceptability statements to justify their motivational beliefs about dance and 119 (52%) to justify their beliefs about basketball. The social acceptance category is defined as a student's perception concerning the role, importance, value, interest, and/or enjoyment associated with participation. Typical male responses to explain motivational and competence beliefs for males in basketball were 'They (boys) are more interested in sports, they like it better, they think it's more fun'. The following quotation reflects the voice of one of the males as he explains his beliefs: Boys have to live with the sport, they have to watch it on TV with their Dads, and with everyone talking about it. They have to be into the sport. The responses males made to justify the beliefs of girls were more diverse with several making assumptions about feelings that girls have and what the girls would rather do than practice basketball skills. A typical response was: 'Cause girls don't like sweating, and getting hot'. It is evident from the following reasons provided by young males that they perceive basketball to be a boys' sport and that the female students will choose activities that are more acceptable for girls. 'Cause girls stuff is like gymnastics and all that'. 'They really aren't into the game, they just want to play with their friends'. 'Girls enjoy feminine things like sewing'. When asked to justify their beliefs about why students think dance is more appropriate for girls, and why the boys might not try as hard to be successful, the responses reflect a clear message that dance is not socially acceptable for boys. This belief is evident in the following justifications provided by male students: 'It's girly and too sissy for guys'. 'It just seems weird to do dances, not natural'. 'It's the thing to do for girls'. While females offered fewer justifications for their beliefs, nearly half of the references were based on the social acceptance of the activity (38 or 45% for dance and 62 or 45% for basketball). The females agreed that young males are expected to be sport and basketball enthusiasts and have accepted this role. The following explanations are typical of the responses from females: 'Boys enjoy it more, they like to play--and you don't see a lot of girls playing it'. One female explained that 'boys play everything and girls just like cheerleading and some of the other sports'. The females agreed that basketball is not a popular activity for girls. The following explanations are examples: 'Girls have better things to do, they think it's boring'. 'Girls don't like it cause you get hot and sweaty'. 'Girls see more boys playing basketball, girls do things like volleyball and softball'. In thinking about basketball some females indicated a lack of interest marked by feelings of embarrassment and fear of failure. For these students basketball elicited thoughts such as 'Girls know they won't do as well and people will make fun of them'. 'People pay attention to you and you feel left out if you never get the ball', or 'Girls don't want to play because they will be embarrassed'. The female students shared with the males a strong belief that dance is not socially acceptable for boys. They seem to accept that 'The boys are too macho for it; machos don't dance'. One female explained that 'boys don't do as well in dance because they don't like to do things that are graceful'. Another female argued that 'Girls are supposed to be gentle and graceful'. Many of the responses given as justifications for competence and motivational beliefs contained references to an overall natural ability for girls in dance and boys in basketball. The physical edge category refers to abilities reflective of physical growth, development, and maturation, as well as skill or talent. For the males 31 (31%) of the responses to justify competence in dance, and 78 (34%) of the responses to justify competence in basketball reflected this theme. The females also used natural ability justifications for responses in dance (32 or 39%) and basketball (50 or 36%). In discussing the physical edge in basketball, male and female students were clear in their explanations that boys have more power, are usually taller, and can handle the ball better. The following quotations reflect the voices of students as they explain why boys think they are better in basketball. Girls are not as strong, they don't have enough power to get the basketball to the goal. Boys are better, they are faster, stronger, and have better eye--hand coordination. I think if gifts get hurt and something, they will start holding back--it's a physical game. They are just born with it. The responses coded in the physical edge category to justify children's beliefs about competence and motivation in dance activities were similar for males and females. 'Girls are more flexible, have better balance, and are not as clumsy', represent typical responses made by the children. In explaining their views the females pointed out that the boys aren't built for dancing. They can't do splits, they can't stretch, and they can't move as good were reasons provided for the lack of motivation in boys. Responses that were coded as 'Effort' made reference to a student's willingness and opportunity to improve either through practice, observation, persistence, patience, and/or determination. Of the 552 responses made by the children, 89 contained a statement directed toward personal effort as a justification for male and female competence and motivation in basketball and dance. The children, males and females alike, overwhelmingly agreed that boys are better in basketball and girls are better in dance and that success is due in part to opportunity for practice and a willingness to try. It is evident from the following examples, however, that both opportunity and effort are driven by a sense of gender appropriateness. Boys practice basketball and they take their time when playing. Boys are more motivated to learn basketball. Girls have more experience in dance--they take lessons. In basketball girls just don't try, or at least they don't try as hard as boys. In dance, boys just fool around and they never try to dance. Evaluation of Teaching Practices When students were asked to evaluate the effectiveness of four strategies teachers use to motivate children in basketball and dance, they judged rewards for effort as the most effective, even though some gender differences were evident. When asked to compare rewards for effort with the other three teaching strategies, the children chose the practice of rewarding effort a greater number of times. Nearly half of the responses by males and females in both basketball and dance indicated a preference for this teaching strategy. An individualized approach was judged to be the second most effective, followed by rewards for performance and finally recognition for performance outcomes. In basketball, the children chose rewards for performance 10 times, which represents only 16% of the total number of responses. In dance only 11 responses from males and two responses from females indicated a preference for performance recognition (see Table 2). The interpretative analysis of the children's interview transcripts provides the basis for their decisions and highlights endorsements of strategies that seem to be specific to gender. Most children agreed that the use of rewards to encourage effort among girls in basketball and boys in dance would be a productive practice for teachers to consider. Males and females said that girls would be motivated to learn basketball in this class because they would be rewarded for how hard they try rather than how good they are. Typical responses from males to support the effort reward for girls were 'because all they have to do is try'. 'Even if they are bad and try, they get a reward'. The children exhibited less consensus in their belief that the rewards would be successful in motivating boys to want to learn dance. Some responses supported a belief that earning tokens to take to a gym store would not necessarily motivate boys in dance because they would not be interested in the prizes offered. One female responded: 'They don't like to dance and they wouldn't like the store'. The children ranked a mastery oriented environment as the second most effective approach teachers can use to sustain motivation in basketball and dance but some saw the practice of allowing children to work at different levels as potentially harmful. This concern is expressed in the following responses from children when asked if an individualized strategy would motivate all children in basketball and dance: Not the girls in the low basketball group--it's like first graders in reading--it's just not fair. Not those who aren't very athletic if the teacher spends more time with those who are better. No, the skilled dancers will be doing something harder and the less skilled would be doing something simple--they would rather be with their friends. No, the ones who are less skilled will feel left out. While rewarding effort was generally endorsed by children as an effective strategy for motivating both males and females, rewarding outstanding performance received little support. Many children spoke about the harmful effects associated with rewarding only the highly skilled with prizes. These concerns are reflected in the following responses: Because some don't have the skill in dance and wouldn't get the tokens. No, most boys wouldn't get tokens because they aren't as good. No because the girls are less skilled than the boys and wouldn't get to go the store. The children in this study sent a strong message reflecting a belief that praising skill performance would have debilitating motivational effects on both males and females. The practice of displaying pictures of highly skilled learners was viewed as being harmful to many students and was judged to be basically unacceptable because it was unfair. The typical response reflected a concern that only a few students are highly skilled and it would not be an effective motivational practice for teachers to recognize only these students. Discussion Findings indicate that direct intervention might be needed for students with stereotypical beliefs about appropriate and inappropriate activity choices incorporated into their perceived ability in movement and sport. The recent work of Wright (1997) has demonstrated how pedagogical practices and more specifically teachers' choice in language during physical education classes can contribute to the construction of these beliefs. Using a detailed analysis of the talk between teachers and students, this researcher revealed that the use of language was not neutral but rather contributed to a gendered learning context which was stereotypical. In the present study, girls with well-established sex-role conceptions about basketball saw no value in trying hard, explaining that they had other activities more important and deserving of their effort. They did not value basketball achievement but at the same time avoided participation because of a fear of being unsuccessful and embarrassment. Responses from the boys painted a clear picture that they had also received heavy doses of sex-role information about the inappropriateness of dance activities. In a society where traditional team sports are viewed as masculine, efforts to construct class climates that are equitable will require that teachers structure a physical activity curriculum so that all students are included. Physical educators should be encouraged to challenge the traditional, dominant stereotypical views associated with male and female participation and achievement in various types of physical activities. Because sex differentiated conceptions are established early in life (Meece & Courtney, 1992), teachers need to go beyond equitable treatment of boys and girls and design specialized strategies and curricular interventions to reduce sex bias in some students' beliefs and behaviours. These could focus on providing information about female basketball players and male dancers, and exposing all students to male and female role models for a variety of sport and movement activities. A broad curriculum with a variety of team games, dance and gymnastics activities has a greater potential to provide experiences that are meaningful to all students. Teachers must send a message that participation in a broad range of cooperative and competitive activities is important for both males and females. The children in this study agreed that teachers should focus on effort rather than performance if the goal is to motivate all children in sex-typed activities. These student views are consistent with evidence presented by Ames (1992) indicating that emphasis on social comparison rather than effort by teachers will likely have detrimental effects on student motivation. According to Ames (1992) and students in this study agree, teachers can maintain motivation and encourage students to try hard by focusing on effort rather than ability. However, while the reward for effort was judged to be the strategy with the most potential, students in the study were unsure about its effectiveness with the boys in dance. According to the children rewards for effort are fair but might not overcome the power of social influences, especially for boys. The individualized approach was favoured over the two strategies giving recognition or rewards for outstanding performance, but children made it clear that having students work at different levels would not necessarily enhance motivation for the lower skilled. The concerns about individual work expressed by the students in this study suggest that from their perspective, this approach tends to make public those who are the more and less abled students. With careful planning a skilled teacher can make differentiation non-public and avoid embarrassing the less abled child, but it is possible that the students in this study had not experienced an individualized approached applied in this way. While the students questioned the benefits of providing different practice tasks, an approach which would give children more time and extra help on the same tasks was very attractive. Finally, the practices emphasizing performance outcomes received few endorsements. The children spoke about problems with any strategy that would identify the lower skilled children, but in general favoured the rewards over public recognition. This research focused on homogeneous groups of students who have accepted and internalized for themselves and others stereotypical views about basketball and dance. While these students had incorporated rigidly held sex-typed beliefs into their concepts about themselves, other students may try to resist the influence of stereotypes on their thinking and behaviour. In this study, for example, there were many children who believed that basketball and dance were appropriate for both boys and girls. These children should be studied to determine the antecedents for their motivational beliefs. The issue of gender differences in competence beliefs for sex-typed movement activities is complex and can be studied from many perspectives. Before we can point to ways teacher practices can overcome existing gender differences in participation and performance, research must continue to examine student beliefs, values, and expectations in a range of activities. As others have frequently highlighted (e.g. Graham, 1995; Portman, 1995; Veal & Compagnone, 1995), a complete understanding will require further study of teacher practices and motivational patterns from a student perspective. Correspondence: Amelia M. Lee, Department of Kinesiology, Louisiana State University, Baton Rouge, LA 70803, USA. Tel: + 504 388 3551; Fax: + 504 388 3680; e-mail: (Amlee@lsu.edu). TABLE 1. Justifications for competence and motivational beliefs about basketball and dance Legend for Chart: A B C D E - Category Males (N = Males (N = Females (N Females (N 27) Frequency 27) Percentage = 23) Frequency = 23) Percentage A B C D E 16 53 31 100 16 53 31 100 13 38 32 83 16 45 39 100 33 119 78 230 14 52 34 100 27 62 50 139 19 45 36 100 Dance Effort Social acceptance Physical edge Total Basketball Effort Social acceptance Physical edge Total TABLE 2. Frequency and percentage for teacher strategies by activity Legend for Chart: A B C D - Strategy Basketball Male n Basketball Male % Basketball Female n E F G H I - Basketball Female % Dance Male n Dance Male % Dance Female n Dance Female % A C D E 20 30 16 4 6 Rewards for performance 12 Rewards for effort 30 Mastery oriented Recognition for performance B F G H I 27 32 33 26 33 6 10 11 11 2 3 18 12 20 14 15 15 19 46 26 43 39 41 35 45 References AMES, C. (1992) Classroom: goals, structures, and student motivation, Journal of Educational Psychology, 84, pp. 261-271. ATKINSON, J.W. (1964) An Introduction to Motivation (Princeton, IN, Van Nostrand). BANDURA, A. (1969) Principles of Behavior Modification (New York, Holt, Rinehart, and Winston). BANDURA, A. (1986) Social Foundations of Thought and Action: A Cognitive Theory (Englewood Cliffs, NJ, Prentice-Hall). BANDURA, A. (1989) Human agency in social cognitive theory, American Psychologist, 44, pp. 1175-1184. BLUMENFELD, P.C. (1992) Classroom learning and motivation: clarifying and expanding goal theory, Journal of Educational Psychology, 84, pp. 272-281. BROPHY, J.E. (1983) Conceptualizing student motivation, Educational Psychologist, 18, pp. 200-215. BROPHY, J.E. (1987) Sociology students' motivation to learn, in: M. MAEHR & D. KLEIBER (Eds) Advances in Motivation and Achievement, Vol. 5, pp. 181-210 (Greenwich, CT, JAI Press). BOGDAN, R.C. & BIKLEN, S.K. (1992) Qualitative Research for Education (Boston, MA, Allyn and Bacon). CLIFTON, R.T. & GILL, D.L. (1994) Gender differences in self-confidence on a feminine-typed task, Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 16, pp. 150-162. CSIZMA, K.A., WITTING, A.F. & SCHURR, K.T. (1988) Sport stereotypes and gender, Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 10, pp. 62-74. ECCLES, J.S., MIDGLEY, C. & ADLER, T. (1984) Grade-related changes in the school environment: effects on achievement motivation, in: J.G. NICHOLLS (Ed.) The Development of Achievement Motivation, Vol. 13, pp. 283-331 (Greenwich, CT, JAI Press). ECCLES, J.S., WIGFIELD, A., FLANAGAN, C., MILLER, C., REUMAN, D. & YEE, D. (1989) Self-concepts, domain values, and self esteem: relations and changes at early adolescence, Journal of Personality, 57, pp. 283-310. ECCLES, J, ADLER, T.F., FUTTERMAN, R., GOFF, S.B., KACZALA, C.M., MEECE, J. & MIDGLEY, C. (1983) Expectancies, values, and academic behaviors, in: J.T. SPENCE (Ed.) Achievement and Achievement Motives, pp. 75-146 (San Francisco, CA, W.H. Freeman). GRAHAM, G. (1995) Physical education through student's eyes and in student's voices: introduction, Journal of Teaching in Physical Education, 14, pp. 364-371. GOUDAS, M. & BIDDLE, S. (1994) Perceived motivational climate and instrinsic motivation in school physical education classes, European Journal of Psychology of Education, IX(3), pp. 241250. HARTER, S. (1982) The perceived competence scale for children, Child Development, 53, pp. 87-97. LEE, A.M., CARTER, J.A. & XIANG, P. (1995) Conception of ability in physical education, Journal of Teaching in Physical Education, 14, pp. 384-393. LENNY, E. (1977) Women's self-confidence in achievement settings, Psychological Bulletin, 84, pp. 1-13. LINCOLN, Y.S. & GUBA, E. (1985) Naturalistic Inquiry (Beverly Hills, CA, Sage). LIRGG, C.D. (1991) Gender differences in self-confidence in physical activity: a meta-analysis of recent studies, Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 13, pp. 294-310. LORDE, A. (1984) Age, race, class and sex: women redefining differences, in: A. LORDE (Ed.) Sister Outsider, pp. 114-123 (Trumansberg, NY, Crossing Press). MAEHR, M.L. & NICHOLS, J.G. (1980) Culture and achievement motivation: a second look, in: N. WARREN (Ed.) Studies in Cross-Cultural Psychology, pp. 221-267 (New York, Academic Press). MARSH, H.W., BARNS, J., CAIRNS, L. & TIDMAN, M. (1984) Self-description questionnaire: age and sex effects in the structure and level of self-concept for preadolescent children, Journal of Educational Psychology, 76, pp. 940-956. MEECE, J.L. & COURTNEY, D.P. (1992) Gender differences in students' perceptions: consequences for achievement-related choices, in: D.H. SCHUNK & J.L. MEECE (Eds) Student Perceptions in the Classroom, pp. 209-227 (Hillsdale, NJ, Erlbaum). METHENY, E. (1965) Connotations of Movement in Sport and Dance (Dubuque, IA, Brown). NICHOLLS, J. (1984) Conceptions of ability and achievement motivation, in: R. AMES & C. AMES (Eds) Research on Motivation in Education: Student Motivation, Vol. 1, pp. 39-73 (New York, Academic Press). PATTON, M.Q. (1980) Qualitative Evaluation Methods (Beverly Hills, CA, Sage). PORTMAN, P. (1995) Who is having fun in physical education classes? Experiences of sixthgrade students in elementary and middle schools, Journal of Teaching in Physical Education, 44, pp. 445-553. SATINA, B., SOLMON, M.A., COTHRAN, D.J., LOFTUS, S.J. & STOCKIN-DAVIDSON, K. (1998) Patriarchal consciousness: middle school students' and teachers' perspectives of motivational practices, Sport, Education and Society, 3, pp. 181-200. SANGUINETTI, C., LEE, A. & NELSON, J. (1985) Reliability estimates and age and gender comparisons of expectations of success in sex-typed activities, Journal of Sport Psychology, 7, pp. 379-388. STIPEK, D.J. & MACIVER, D. (1989) Developmental change in children's assessment of intellectual competence, Child Development, 60, pp. 521-538. STREITMATTER, J. (1994) Toward gender equity in the classroom: everyday teachers' beliefs and pracatices (Albany, NY, State University Press). THORKILDSEN, T.A., NOLEN, S.B. & FOURNIER, J. (1994) What is fair? Children's critiques of practices that influences motivation, Journal of Educational Psychology, 86, pp. 475486. VEAL, M.L. & COMPAGNONE, N. (1995) How sixth grades perceive effort and skill, Journal of Teaching in Physical Education, 14, pp. 431-444. VYGOTSKY, L. (1978) Mind in Society: The Development of Higher Psychological Processes (Cambridge, MA, Harvard University Press). WRIGHT, J. (1996) The construction of complementarity in physical education, Gender and Education, 8, pp. 61-79. WRIGHT, J. (1997) The construction of gendered contexts in single sex and co-educational physical education lessons, Sport, Education and Society, 2, pp. 55-72. XIANG, P., LEE, A. & SOLMON, M. (1995) Achievement Goals of American and Chinese Students: A Cross-Cultural Analysis, paper presented at the annual meeting of American Educational Research Association (AERA), San Francisco, CA, April. ~~~~~~~~