Kalat Chapters 12 – 15 Notes CHAPTER 12 EMOTIONAL BEHAVIORS Chapter Outline I. What Is Emotion? A. Strong emotions tend to increase readiness for action. Most general theories of emotion deal with the relationship between emotion and action. B. Emotions, Autonomic Arousal, and the James-Lange Theory 1. Emotional situations arouse the sympathetic and the parasympathetic branches of the autonomic nervous system. The sympathetic nervous system prepares the body for brief, vigorous action while the parasympathetic nervous system alters the body’s activities to save energy and prepare for later events. 2. One theory on emotion is that when you feel an emotion, your heart rate responds and then prompts other responses. 3. James-Lange theory: Autonomic arousal and skeletal actions occur before an emotion. An emotion is the label we give to our physiological responses. 4. In this theory, emotions have three components—cognitions, actions, and feelings. Cognitive appraisal of a situation comes first, which leads to some action, and then the emotional feelings follow last. 5. Is Physiological Arousal Necessary for Emotions? a. People with severe spinal cord injuries, who cannot move or feel from the damaged area downwards still report that they feel emotions at the same rate/level as before the injury. This suggests emotions do not require feedback from muscle movements. b. Pure autonomic failure: Uncommon condition in which output from the autonomic nervous system to the body fails, either completely or almost completely. People with this disorder have no changes in autonomic response to psychological or physical stress. These people report having the same emotions as anyone else, although the emotions are much less intense. The decreased emotional feeling is consistent with predictions of the James-Lange theory. c. A study of people who have received BOTOX suggest that they experience weaker than usual emotional responses to videos, which implies that body changes (i.e., facial expressions and movements) are important for feeling an emotion. 6. Is Physiological Arousal Sufficient for Emotions? a. b. Panic attack: A condition marked by extreme sympathetic nervous system arousal, which is sometimes brought on by the occurrence of rapid breathing and a racing heartbeat. Such symptoms, when spontaneously occurring, lead people to believe that a panic attack is about to happy, and thus trigger the panic attack. People forced to smile rated a comic strip as funnier than people not manipulated into expressing a smile. c. However, a smile is now necessary for happiness. People with a rare disorder that does not allow them to smile, Möbius syndrome, still experience happiness and amusement. C. Brain Areas Associated with Emotion 1. Attempts to Localize Specific Emotions a. Limbic System: A forebrain area that forms a border around the brainstem, traditionally regarded as critical for emotion. b. Brain areas responsible for different types of emotion have been investigated and found to be highly variable. One of the best-localized emotions is disgust, which has been shown to be in the insular cortex. The same part of the brain reacts to something that tastes badly. 2. Contributions of the Left and Right Hemispheres a. Activity in the left hemisphere is termed as Behavioral Activation System (BAS), marked by low to moderate autonomic arousal, which could be characterized as either happiness or anger. b. Activity in the frontal and temporal lobes of the right hemisphere is associated with the Behavioral inhibition System (BIS), which increases attention and arousal, inhibits action, and stimulates emotions such as fear and disgust. c. People with great activity in the frontal cortex of the left hemisphere tend to be happier, more outgoing, and more fun-loving. People with greater right-hemisphere activity tend to be socially withdrawn, less satisfied with life, and prone to unpleasant emotions. d. The right hemisphere appears to be more responsive to emotional stimuli than the left. The right hemisphere is especially activated by unpleasant emotions. D. Functions of Emotion 1. The adaptive advantages of some emotions are clear: Fear alerts us to danger and disgust helps us avoid illness-inducing substances. They may also help us make quick “gut” decisions. 2. Emotions and Moral Decisions a. When making moral decisions, we consider how the outcomes will make us feel. b. The trolley dilemma, the footbridge dilemma, the lifeboat dilemma, and the hospital dilemma are all examples where you can save five people (sometimes including yourself) by killing one person. c. Most people say it is right to pull the switch in the trolley dilemma, while fewer say yes in the footbridge and lifeboat dilemmas. d. 3. When we are making a decision about right and wrong, we seldom work it out rationally. Most people go with what feels right and then think of a logical justification afterwards. Decision Making After Brain Damage that Impairs Emotions a. If confronted with the trolley car dilemma, people with prefrontal damage are more likely than average to choose the utilitarian option of killing one to save five, even in situations where most people find the choice emotionally unacceptable. b. The most famous example is of Phineas Gage, who suffered massive damage to his prefrontal cortex and survived. Reports about his behavior and temperament after the accident suggested that he became impulsive and made poor decisions. c. Antonio Damasio, who also suffered prefrontal cortex damage, expressed almost no emotions. It resulted in bad decision-making that cost him his job, his marriage, and his savings. d. Damage to the ventromedial prefrontal cortex results in the loss of a sense of guilt. People with damage to the prefrontal cortex or the amygdala are slow in processing emotional information. II. Attack and Escape Behaviors A. Attack Behaviors 1. What you are doing or about to do affects how you feel. Research shows that a bout of anger will often prime a more intense and rapid bout of anger, if provoked, at a later point. 4. Heredity and Environment in Violence a. Research shows that people who were abused as children, people who witnesses abuse of parents, and people who live in violent neighborhoods are at greater risk of violence themselves. b. Environmental factors, including exposure to lead and fetal exposure to nicotine, may lead to violence in individuals. c. One study found that monozygotic twins were more likely to resemble each other in adult crimes and aggressive behaviors than were dizygotic twins, and adopted children resemble their biological parents. d. No single gene accounts for aggression. Instead, there is an interaction between heredity and environmental factors. e. Violence is particularly enhanced in people with both a genetic predisposition and a troubled early environment. Low MAOA activity and serious maltreatment in childhood results in significantly higher antisocial behavior. 4. Hormones a. Male aggressive behavior and increased striving for social dominance depends heavily on testosterone. In humans, men with higher levels of testosterone have, on average, slightly higher rates of violent activities and criminal behaviors than do other men. b. Aggression tends to be highest when testosterone levels are high and cortisol levels are low (as cortisol increases fear). c. A recent study found young women injected with testosterone were less accurate at identifying angry facial expressions. Other studies show that testosterone increases the response of the amygdala to angry facial expressions. 5. Serotonin Synapses and Aggressive Behavior. a. Nonhuman Animals Increased aggressive behavior was seen in decreased serotonin turnover in mice. Serotonin turnover can be inferred by measuring the concentration of 5-hydroxyindoleacetic acid (5-HIAA), a serotonin metabolite. Luigi Valzelli found that social isolation induced a drop in serotonin turnover in the brains of male mice, an effect that further increased the possibility of aggressive behavior toward other males. These effects are not found in female mice. In a study of 2-year-old male monkeys, researchers found that monkeys with the lowest serotonin turnover had the highest amount of aggressive behaviors. Moreover, these monkeys died before the age of 6, while monkeys with higher serotonin turnover were alive at 6. b. Humans Studies have found that lower-than-normal serotonin turnover is present in those convicted of violent crimes as well as those who committed or attempted suicide by violent means. Follow-up studies on people released from prison have found that those with lower serotonin turnover had a greater probability of further convictions for violent crimes. Neurons synthesize serotonin from tryptophan (an amino acid found in proteins). A diet high in other amino acids, but low in tryptophan, impairs the brain’s ability to synthesize serotonin. One study found an increase in aggressive behavior in young men a few hours after eating a diet low in tryptophan. Less active forms of the genes for the enzyme tryptophan hydroxylase (which convert tryptophan into serotonin) may be responsible for the low serotonin levels and increased aggression. c. How Do We Explain Serotonin Effects? High levels of serotonin inhibit a variety of impulses, and low levels remove inhibitions. Serotonin’s role in aggression is complicated: Some people with low serotonin levels become depressed, not aggressive. However, the brain has been found to release serotonin during aggressive actions. In addition, low serotonin activity prior to aggravation magnifies the response when serotonin is suddenly released at the start of an aggressive encounter. B. Fear, and Anxiety 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. Fear, Anxiety, and the Amygdala a. Startle reflex: Response one makes to a sudden, unexpected loud noise. People with post-traumatic stress disorder show a much enhanced startle reflex. Studies of Rodents a. The amygdala enhances the startle reflex by sending axons to the hypothalamus (for controlling autonomic fear responses) and by relaying information to the midbrain, which in turn sends axons to the pons, triggering the startle reflex. b. Toxoplasma gondii is a protozoan that exploits the consequences of amygdala damage. The parasite enter a rat and migrates to the brain, where it damages the amygdala. The rat fearlessly approaches a cat, guaranteeing that the parasite will find its way back into a cat (where it reproduces and excreted in the feces). c. If a rat receives shocks after fear conditioning, it learns to fear the stimulus, as well as the cage, new cages, and new situations. The same is true of humans. d. The amygdala is important for knowing what to fear. When people are attacked or have traumatic experiences, they become more fearful in a wide variety of situations. This long-term, generalized emotional arousal depends on a brain area called the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis. Studies of Monkeys a. Klüver-Bucy syndrome: Tameness and placidity in monkeys following damage or removal of the amygdala. Monkeys with amygdala lesions have decreased fear and are more likely to approach an object they normally avoid, while other monkeys are withdrawn and fearful. b. Those with a more vigorously reactive amygdala tend to show the greatest fear in response to a noise or an intruder. Response of the Human Amygdala to Visual Stimuli a. Studies using fMRI have demonstrated that the amygdala is activated in response to emotional expressions, especially fear and anger. It also responds, to a lesser extent, to faces showing happiness or sadness. b. The amygdala responds most strongly when a facial expression is a bit ambiguous or difficult to interpret. Presumably, arousal indicates that it is working harder to make sense of the stimulus. Individual Differences in Amygdala Response and Anxiety a. In a study of college students, researchers found that amygdala activity correlated highly with the number of unpleasant emotions they had recorded. Presumably, unpleasant emotions are biologically predisposed. b. Soldiers with the greatest amygdala response to unpleasant faces later reported the greatest amount of combat stress. c. The amygdala is strongly associated with fear responses. The interpretation is that people with a highly reactive amygdala are likely to perceive dangers and therefore to support strong protection against those dangers. 6. Damage to the Human Amygdala a. Some people with damage to the amygdala continue to experience the cognitive aspect of emotions (they can classify emotional photos as pleasant or unpleasant) but not the feeling aspect. b. Urbach-Wiethe disease is a genetic disorder that causes gradual atrophy of the amygdala. In one example, an individual known as SM views scary movies and experiences only excitement, is curious when touching exotic and venomous snakes, and is happy when going through a haunted house. In addition, she was held up at gun point and knife point and only remembers being angry, not scared. Her preferred talking distance was ½ that of normal people, and she did not show discomfort when an unknown man approached her so close that their noses touched. c. Such people fail to recognize the emotional expressions in faces, especially expressions of fear and disgust. Instead of making eye contact, she always looks at people’s noses. When asked to look at the eyes, those like SM quickly recognize fear. d. This research suggests that the amygdala may not be responsible for feeling fear, as much as it is responsible for detecting emotional information and directing other brain areas to pay attention to it in the proper way. C. Anxiety Disorders 1. Anxiety disorders are characterized as such when the major symptoms is increased anxiety. They include panic disorder, generalized anxiety disorder, and phobias. 2. Panic disorder: A type of anxiety disorder characterized by frequent periods of anxiety and occasional attacks of rapid breathing, increased heart rate, sweating, and trembling. More common in women than men, and more common in adolescent and young adults than in older adults. 3. Pharmacological Relief from Anxiety. a. CCK (cholecystokinin) is one of the main excitatory neuromodulators in the amygdala. Injections of CCK-stimulating drugs into the amygdala enhance the startle reflex. Injections of CCK type B receptor blockers will block this anxiety. b. GABA (gamma amino butyric acid) is the main inhibitory neurotransmitter found in the amygdala. Injections of GABA blockers can induce outright panic. c. Benzodiazepines: Commonly used class of anti-anxiety drugs. The benzodiazepines include diazepam, chlordiazepoxide, and alprazolam. These drugs bind to a receptor site on the GABAA receptor, which causes the receptor to change shape, allowing GABA to attach more easily and bind more tightly to it. d. 4. Benzodiazepines exert their antianxiety effects in the amygdala and hypothalamus, midbrain, and several other areas. They produce a variety of effects, including the possibility of addiction. Relearning as Relief from Anxiety a. Anti-anxiety drugs provide temporary relief and are not intended for use with chronic anxiety, like that which results from trauma. b. To help relieve chronic anxiety, clinical psychologists generally use exposure therapy to gradually expose the individual to the feared object. However, although extinction training suppresses the original learning with new learning, it does not eliminate it altogether. Adults seldom fully extinguish a learned reaction. c. In general, it is easier to extinguish a learned response immediately after original learning than it is later. After time has passed, the learning becomes consolidated (has a stronger effect). d. Propranolol: A drug that interferes with protein synthesis at certain synapses in the amygdala. This drug weakens the emotional response that occurs after a feared stimulus is presented, and the result is a persisting decrease in fear intensity. III. Stress and Health A. Behavioral medicine: Emphasizes effects of diet, smoking, exercise, and other behaviors on health. B. Concepts of Stress 1. Stress: The nonspecific response of the body to any demand made upon it. Events that are interpreted as threatening to an individual and which elicit physiological and behavioral responses. 2. General Adaptation Syndrome: A generalized response to stress that includes the following three stages: alarm (characterized by increased activity of the sympathetic nervous system), resistance (adrenal cortex secretes cortisol and other hormones that enable the body to maintain prolonged alertness, fight infections, and heal wounds), and exhaustion (the individual is tired, inactive, and vulnerable because the nervous system and immune systems no longer have the energy to sustain their heightened responses). 3. Changes in one’s life can also induce stress, such as getting fired or promoted. C. Stress and the Hypothalamus-Pituitary-Adrenal Cortex Axis 1. Stress activates both the autonomic nervous system and the HPA axis (hypothalamus, pituitary gland, and adrenal cortex). In fact, prolonged stress increasingly activates the HPA axis. 2. Stress activates the hypothalamus, which sends messages to the anterior pituitary gland to secrete adrenocorticotropic hormone (ACTH); this hormone stimulates the adrenal cortex to secrete cortisol, which increases blood sugar levels and enhances metabolism. 3. 4. 5. Brief and moderate stress improves attention and memory formation, improves performance on relatively simple tasks, impairs performance that requires complex, flexible thinking, and enhances the activity of the immune system. The Immune System a. Immune system: Comprised of cells that protect the body against intruders such as bacteria and viruses. An autoimmune disease is the result of the immune system attacking normal cells. 1. Leukocytes: White blood cells that are produced in the bone marrow before migrating to the thymus gland, spleen, and peripheral lymph nodes. Leukocytes patrol the blood and other body fluids, looking for intruders. 2. There are several types of leukocytes, including B cells: Leukocytes that mature in the bone marrow and produce specific antibodies to attack an antigen. 3. Antibodies: Y-shaped proteins that circulate in the blood and attach specifically to one kind of antigen. The body develops antibodies against antigens that it has encountered in the past. 4. Antigens (antibody-generator molecules): Proteins located on a cell surface. When leukocytes discover cells with antigens different from the rest of the body, they attack those cells. 5. T cells: Leukocytes that mature in the thymus. T cells are of two types: cytotoxic T cells, which directly attack intruder cells, and helper T cells, which stimulate other T cells or B cells to multiply more rapidly. 6. Natural killer cells: Blood cells that attach to cells infected with viruses and certain kinds of tumor cells. 7. Cytokines: Chemicals released by the immune system that cross the blood-brain barrier and influence neuronal function. Effects of Stress on the Immune System a. Psychoneuroimmunology: Deals with the ways in which experiences, especially stressful ones, alter the immune system, and how the immune system in turn influences the central nervous system. b. In response to a stressful experience, the nervous system activates the immune system to increase its production of natural killer cells and the secretion of cytokines. The elevated cytokine levels help combat infections, but also trigger the brain to produce the same symptoms as if one were ill. c. In prosperous countries, the immune system responds more often to events such as taking exams in college, seeing photos of sick or injured people, or giving a public lecture. d. Results of one study show that people who reported a brief stressful experience were at no more risk for catching a cold that were people who reported no stress. However, for those who reported stress lasting longer than a month, the longer it lasted, the greater the risk of illness. e. High cortisol levels impair memory and increase the vulnerability of neurons in the hippocampus, so that toxins or overstimulation will kill the neurons. D. Stress Control 1. In humans, resilience in the face of stress correlates with stronger connections between the amygdala and the prefrontal cortex. 2. In order to control stress, people have developed techniques like breathing routines, exercise, meditation, and distraction. Social support is a powerful method for coping with stress. Brain scans correspond to people’s selfreports that social support from a loved one helps reduce stress. E. Post-traumatic Stress Disorder 1. PTSD: Psychiatric disorder that occurs in some people who have had a traumatic experience of being severely injured or threatened or seeing other people harmed or killed. 2. Symptoms of PTSD, which last at least a month after the experience, include frequent distressing recollections (flashbacks) and nightmares about the traumatic event, avoidance of reminders of it, and exaggerated arousal in response to noises and other stimuli. 3. There are differences in vulnerability to PTSD. Victims have a smaller than average hippocampus and have lower than normal cortisol levels. F. CHAPTER 13 THE BIOLOGY OF LEARNING AND MEMORY Chapter Outline I. Learning, Memory, Amnesia, and Brain Functioning A. Localized Representations of Memory 1. Classical Conditioning: After repeated presentations (although a strong stimulus will work with only one pairing) of a conditioned stimulus (CS), which initially elicits no response, with an unconditioned stimulus (UCS), which automatically elicits an unconditioned response (UCR), the subject begins responding to the CS because they have come to associate it with the UCS. For example, Pavlov classically conditioned a dog to respond to have a salivating response to a bell after continuously pairing the bell with the dog’s food. 2. Instrumental Conditioning: Behavior is followed by a reinforcer (which increases the future probability of a response) or punishment (which suppresses the frequency of a response). Lashley’s Search for the Engram a. Engram: Physical representation of learning. b. Karl Lashley’s work on learning (using cortical lesions in varying locations within the brains of rats) led him to propose two principles about the nervous system. c. Equipotentiality: All parts of the cortex contribute equally to complex behaviors like learning; any part of the cortex can substitute for any other. d. Mass action: The cortex works as a whole, and the more cortex the better. e. Lashley’s work was based on the assumption that the cerebral cortex was the best place to search for an engram and that all memories are physiologically the same. Researchers that followed found neither assumption was necessary true. f. Years later, Richard F. Thompson located an engram of memory in the cerebellum. g. Lateral interpositus nucleus (LIP): An area essential for learning. Damage to this area of the cerebellum leads to permanent loss of a classically conditioned eyeblink response in rabbits. Temporary suppression of the area led to zero effectiveness of classical conditioning training. B. Types of Memory 1. Short-Term and Long-Term Memory a. Short-term memory: Memory of events that have just occurred. b. Long-term memory: Memory of events from previous times. c. Short-term and long-term memory differ in capacity. Short-term memory holds no more than seven items, while long-term memory is vast and more difficult to estimate. 3. d. 2. Short-term memory depends on rehearsal and long-term memory does not. e. With short-term memory, once you forget something, it is lost. Longterm memories may be recalled with hints and reconstructed. f. Information initially entered into short-term storage can be consolidated into long-term memory. Our Changing Views of Consolidation a. Many short-term memories are not simply temporary stores on their way to being long-term memories. Once something changes (like the score to a hockey game), it doesn’t turn into a long-term memory. b. The time needed for consolidation varies enormously. Memorizing interesting facts will take less time than memorizing boring ones. Emotionally significant memories form quickly. Small to moderate amount of cortisol activate the amygdala and hippocampus, where they enhance the storage and consolidation of recent experiences. c. It was previously thought that once formed, long-term memories were permanent. However, it is now clear that consolidated memories are not always permanent. They can change, fade, and vary in detail. d. If a reminder is followed by a similar experience, the memory is reconsolidated. New experiences during the reconsolidation process can modify the memory. 3. Working Memory a. Working memory: Temporary storage of memories about a task that one is attending to at the moment. b. Delayed-response task: Memory task in which a subject is given a signal to which it must give a learned response after a delay. A common test for working memory. c. Damage to the prefrontal cortex impairs performance on working memory tasks, and the deficit can be amazingly precise. d. Older people have impairments of working memory, probably because of changes in the prefrontal cortex. C. The Hippocampus 1. Amnesia: Memory loss. Damage to the hippocampus produces a powerful kind of amnesia. 2. People with Hippocampal Damage a. Patient H. M. had his hippocampus and surrounding brain tissue removed from both hemispheres in 1953 to treat his severe epilepsy. b. Anterograde and Retrograde Amnesia i. H.M. suffered moderate retrograde amnesia (loss of memory for events that occurred shortly before brain damage) and severe anterograde amnesia (loss of long-term memories for events that happened after brain damage) as a result of the bilateral hippocampal removal. c. Intact Working Memory i. H.M.’s short-term memory or working memory remained intact. d. e. f. Impaired Storage of Long-Term Memory i. The surgery severely impaired H. M.’s ability to form long-term memories. Even newer words to the English language, like Jacuzzi and granola, were regarded as nonsense. ii. When distracted, he would underestimate his own age by 10 years or more. Severe Impairment of Episodic Memory i. He showed no ability to form episodic memories (memories of a single event). He could describe facts that he learned before the operation but could not recount personal events. ii. He did retain the ability to weakly retain semantic (factual) memories. Memory loss also affected his ability to describe the future. Better Implicit than Explicit Memory 3. i. Nearly all patients with amnesia show better implicit memory (influence of a recent experience on behavior, even if one does not realize that he or she is using memory at all) than explicit memory or declarative memory (deliberate recall of information that one recognizes as a memory). g. Intact Procedural Memory i. Procedural memory: the development of motor skills and habits; a special kind of implicit memory. H.M. acquired new skills without apparent difficulty. ii. People with amnesia usually have normal working memory, severe anterograde amnesia for declarative memory, some retrograde amnesia, better implicit than explicit memory, and nearly intact procedural memory. Theories of the Function of the Hippocampus a. The Hippocampus and Declarative Memory i. People with hippocampal damage acquire new skills but have enormous trouble learning new facts. This leads researchers to believe that the hippocampus is critical for declarative memory, especially episodic memory. ii. Delayed matching-to-sample task: Task used to measure declarative memory in animals. In this procedure, animals see an object (the sample) and after a delay get to choose between two objects, one of which matches the sample. iii. Delayed nonmatching-to-sample task: The procedure is the same except that the animal must choose the object that is different from the sample. iv. Hippocampal damage impairs performance on both delayed matching-to-sample and delayed nonmatching-to-sample tasks. b. The Hippocampus and Spatial Memory i. In rats, many hippocampal neurons are tuned to particular spatial locations. In human cab drivers, imaging data has shown that the hippocampus is activated when answering spatial questions and they have a larger than normal posterior hippocampus. ii. Radial Maze: Maze with eight or more arms used to test spatial memory in animals. Damage to the hippocampus impairs performance on this task. iii. Morris Water Maze: Procedure where an animal has to find a hidden platform, usually under murky water. This procedure is used to test spatial memory in animals and, like the radial maze, performance is negatively impacted by hippocampal damage. iv. In certain closely related species of birds, the larger the hippocampus, the better their performance on spatial memory tasks. c. Hippocampus and Contextual Memory i. The hippocampus is important for remembering details and context. Recent memories have significant detail and depend on the hippocampus. ii. Older memories have fewer details and are less reliant on the hippocampus. iii. Recalling recent memories activates the hippocampus; recalling older memories may not require the involvement of the hippocampus. D. The Basal Ganglia 1. The basal ganglia is responsible for implicit learning or habit learning, which is gradual. 2. People with amnesia from hippocampal damage perform randomly on the weather task (when asked what the weather will be like based on images). However, if they continue for long enough, they show gradual improvement based on habits supported by the basal ganglia. 3. This suggests that the hippocampus is more important for declarative memory and the basal ganglia is more important for procedural memory. Psychologists no longer believe in a strict separation between the tasks of the two structures as nearly all tasks activate both areas. E. Other Types of Amnesia 1. Korsakoff's Syndrome a. Korsakoff’s Syndrome or Wernicke-Korsakoff’s syndrome: Brain damage caused by prolonged thiamine deficiency (this disorder is most commonly seen in chronic alcoholics). b. Thiamine deficiency leads to brain cell loss in the mammillary bodies of the hypothalamus and the dorsomedial nucleus of the thalamus, which projects to the prefrontal cortex. c. Korsakoff's patients show apathy, confusion, and have trouble reasoning about their memories. Patients with Korsakoff’s syndrome also have both anterograde and retrograde amnesia. d. Confabulation: Making up an answer to a question and accepting the invented answer as if it were true (a common symptom of Korsakoff's syndrome). 2. Alzheimer’s Disease a. Alzheimer's disease: A dementia that becomes more prevalent with advancing age. Symptoms include short-term and long-term memory loss, confusion, restlessness, hallucinations, and disturbances of eating and sleeping. b. People with Alzheimer’s disease have better procedural than declarative memory and better implicit than explicit memory. c. People with Down syndrome (a type of mental retardation caused by having three copies of chromosome 21) usually get Alzheimer's disease if they survive into middle age. This led researchers to discover a gene on chromosome 21 linked with early-onset Alzheimer’s disease, but this form of the disease only accounts for 1% of total cases. d. Abnormal genes located on several different chromosomes can lead to an accumulation of amyloid- deposits in the brain. Deposits of amyloid cause neuronal degeneration in the brain, and the dying axons and dendrites form plaques in many areas of the cerebral cortex and hippocampus, as well as other brain areas. e. The tau protein also accumulates and produces tangles, structures formed from degeneration within neurons f. Curcumin, a component of the turmeric spice, has been shown to inhibit amyloid- in animals. Research on human applications has just begun. g. What Patients with Amnesia Teach Us i. People do not lose all aspects of memory equally. ii. People have several somewhat independent kinds of memory that depend on different brain areas. D. Other Brain Areas in Memory 1. Almost all cortical and subcortical structures are involved in some aspect of memory. 2. The amygdala is important for fear learning. 3. Parietal lobe damage affects the ability to associate one type of information with another. 4. Damage to the anterior and inferior temporal lobes results in semantic dementia, in which semantic memories are impaired. 5. Damage to the prefrontal cortex impairs the ability to learn about rewards and punishments. II. Storing Information in the Nervous System A. Learning and the Hebbian Synapse 1. Hebbian synapse: A synapse that increases in effectiveness because of simultaneous activity in the presynaptic and postsynaptic neurons. B. Single-Cell Mechanisms of Invertebrate Behavior Change 1. Aplysia as an Experimental Animal a. Aplysia is a marine invertebrate related to the common slug, often used for physiological studies of learning. Aplysia have fewer neurons than any vertebrate, and many are large and easy to study. A commonly studied behavior in the Aplysia is the gill withdrawal response. 2. Habituation in Aplysia a. Habituation: A decrease in response to a stimulus that is presented repeatedly and is accompanied by no change in other stimuli. Habituation in Aplysia reflects a change in the synapse between the sensory neuron and a motor neuron. 3. Sensitization in Aplysia a. Sensitization: An increase in response to a mild stimulus after an intense stimulus has been presented. Sensitization in Aplysia depends on the release of serotonin by a facilitating interneuron onto the synapses of many presynaptic sensory neurons; this process ultimately blocks potassium channels and thereby prolongs the release of transmitter from that neuron. C. Long-Term Potentiation in Vertebrates 1. Long-term potentiation (LTP): Increased responsiveness to axonal input as a result of a previous period of rapidly repeated stimulation. LTP has three properties that make it an attractive candidate for the cellular basis of learning and memory: a. Specificity: Only activated synapses become strengthened. b. Cooperativity: Nearly simultaneous stimulation by two or more axons produces LTP; stimulation by just one axon produces it weakly. c. Associativity: Pairing a weak input with a strong input enhances later responses to the weak input. 2. Long-term depression (LTD): A prolonged decrease in responsiveness to synaptic input after repeated pairing with some previous input that is generally of low frequency. LTD occurs in the cerebellum and hippocampus. 3. Biochemical Mechanisms a. AMPA and NMDA Synapses i. In a few cases, LTP depends on changes at GABA synapses. ii. Most cases of LTP depend on changes at glutamate receptors. iii. The AMPA receptor and the NMDA receptor are both usually excited by the neurotransmitter glutamate, but can respond to drugs abbreviated AMPA and NMDA respectively. iv. Usually glutamate produces neither excitatory nor inhibitory effects at NMDA receptors because magnesium blocks ion channels located on this receptor. v. About the only way to activate NMDA receptors is first to repeatedly stimulate nearby AMPA glutamate receptors, thereby depolarizing the dendrite. Depolarization repels the magnesium ions and allows glutamate to open NMDA channels so that sodium and calcium ions can enter the cell. vi. Calcium ions induce the expression of otherwise inactive genes, which produce proteins that alter the activities of more than a hundred other known chemicals within the dendrites. This increases the future responsiveness of these glutamate receptors. vii. Calcium enhances the responsiveness to glutamate by activating a protein called CaMKII, leading to the following effects: The dendrite may build more AMPA receptors or move them into a better position. Neurons make more NMDA receptors. The dendrite may make more branches, thus forming additional synapses with the same axon. The AMPA receptors become more responsive than before. viii. The effects of CaMKII and CREB are magnified by BDNF, a neurotrophin similar to nerve growth factor. ix. Once LTP has been established, it no longer depends on NMDA synapses. Drugs that block NMDA prevent the establishment of LTP, but they do not interfere with the maintenance of LTP. b. Presynpatic Changes i. LTP causes presynaptic changes through the release of a retrograde neurotransmitter from the postsynaptic cell. These changes include reduced threshold for producing action potentials, increased neurotransmitter release, expanded size of the presynaptic axonal membrane, and release of neurotransmitter from more sites on the axon. 4. Consolidation, Revisited a. fMRI studies found that progressively older events produced more activity in the cerebral cortex and less in the hippocampus and amygdala. This demonstrates a shift to the cerebral cortex both over a period of one day and over a period of many years. D. Improving Memory 1. The relationship between LTP and learning is unknown at this time, but studying the biochemistry of LTP has improved our understanding of what could impair or improve memory. 2. LTP depends on production of several proteins, and enhancing production of these proteins enhances memory in rodents. 3. Moderate doses of stimulant drugs enhance learning by increasing arousal. Caffeine and methylphenidate are both examples. CHAPTER 14 Cognitive Functions Chapter Outline I. Lateralization of Function A. The Left and Right Hemispheres 1. The brain has two hemispheres; each hemisphere controls the contralateral (opposite) side of the body. For example, the right hemisphere is connected to sensory receptors and muscles mainly on the left half of the body (the opposite holds true for the left hemisphere). Both hemispheres control the trunk muscles and facial muscles. Taste and smell are uncrossed (each hemisphere gets taste information from its own side of the tongue. 2. Corpus Callosum: A set of axons that allows the two hemispheres to exchange information with one another. The anterior commissure, the hippocampal commissure, and a couple of other small commissures also help in the exchange of interformation. 3. Lateralization: Refers to the behaviors and cognitive abilities that each hemisphere specializes in. For example, language ability is primarily localized in the left hemisphere. B. Visual and Auditory Connections to the Hemispheres 1. Light from the right visual field (what is visible at a particular moment) shines onto the left half of both retinas; this information is then relayed to the left hemisphere. The right half of each retina connects to the right hemisphere, which sees the left visual field. 2. Optic chiasm: Point where half of the axons from each eye cross to the opposite side of the brain. 3. Each ear receives sound waves from one side of the head, but each sends the information to both sides of the brain. If the two ears receive different information, each hemisphere pays more attention to the ear on the opposite side. C. Cutting the Corpus Callosum 1. Severing the corpus callosum prevents the sharing of information between the two brain hemispheres. 2. Epilepsy: Condition characterized by repeated episodes of excessive synchronized neural activity (i.e., seizure). Most people with epilepsy (90%) use drugs to suppress their seizure activity. 3. If seizure activity is not controlled by drug therapy, some people have surgery to remove the focus (point of origin of the seizure). Alternatively, epileptics sometimes have their corpus callosum severed to prevent seizure activity from crossing from one hemisphere to the other. 4. 5. 6. 7. 8. Evidently, epileptic activity rebounds back and forth between the hemispheres and prolongs seizures. Without the bounce back effect, a seizure may not develop at all. These individuals are often referred to as split-brain people. Split-brain people can point to objects with their left hand (but not with their right hand) if visual information is presented from the left visual field to their right hemisphere. Information presented in the right visual field (thus going to the left hemisphere) allows patients to name or describe what they see. The left hemisphere is dominant for speech production in 95% of righthanded individuals and for approximately 80% of left-handed individuals. Speech comprehension is less lateralized. The two hemispheres of a split-brain person can process information independently of each other. This allows split-brain people to do tasks that some find difficult, like drawing circles with both hands simultaneously with one hand going slightly faster than the other. Split Hemispheres: Competition and Cooperation a. In the first weeks after surgery, split-brain people find that the hemispheres act like separate people sharing one body. For example, 7. 8. D. 1. 2. 3. one person repeatedly took items from the grocery shelf with one hand and returned them with the other. b. Later, the brain eventually learns to use smaller connections between the left and right hemispheres to avoid conflicts between them. c. Sometimes, the hemispheres learn to cooperate. This is tested using words that are flashed simultaneously on both sides of the visual field. Some split-brain people will combine the two words into one concept. The Right Hemisphere a. The right hemisphere is better than the left at perceiving the emotions in people’s gestures and tone of voice. b. People with right-hemisphere damage speak with less inflection and expression, plus they often have trouble interpreting the emotions that other people express through their tone of voice. c. Research findings suggest that the right hemisphere is more adept than the left at comprehending spatial relationships. d. The left hemisphere is more focused on details and the right hemisphere is better at perceiving overall patterns. Hemispheric Specializations in Intact Brains a. Differences in hemisphere specialization can be demonstrated in people without brain damage, but most of these differences are small. b. For example, research shows that you will be more accurate if you smelled the two substances with the same nostril, and therefore the same hemisphere. Development of Lateralization and Handedness Anatomical Differences Between the Hemispheres a. Planum temporale: A section of the temporal cortex that is larger in the left hemisphere in approximately 65% of the population. This difference in size is apparent at age 3 months in humans. Children with the biggest ratio of left to right planum temporale performed best on language tests. b. Smaller but still significant differences are found between left and right hemispheres of chimpanzees, bonobos, and gorillas. This is evidence that specialization in the human brain is built upon specializations already present in apelike species. Maturation of the Corpus Callosum a. The corpus callosum gradually grows and thickens during childhood and adolescence. b. The corpus callosum matures slowly over the first 5 to 10 years of human life. Because the neurons connected by the corpus callosum take years to develop their mature adult pattern, the behavior of young children sometimes resembles that of split-brain people. c. In one study using fabrics, five-year olds did equally well with one hand or with two, while three-year olds made 90% more errors with two hands that with one. Development Without a Corpus Callosum a. People born without a corpus callosum can perform some tasks that split-brain people fail, possibly due to larger-than-normal hemispheric connections developing elsewhere in the brain. For example, they can describe what they feel with either hand and what they see in either visual field. The following two commissures are often larger than normal in people born without a corpus callosum: a. Anterior commissure: Connects the two hemispheres around the anterior parts of the cerebral cortex. b. Hippocampal commissure: Connects the left hippocampus to the right hippocampus. 4. Most people (90%) are right-handed, and in 95% of this group, the left hemisphere is dominant for speech. Left-handed people are more variable. Most left-handers have a left hemisphere dominance for speech, but some have right hemisphere dominance or a mixture of left and right. E. Avoiding Overstatements 1. One should not conclude from this research that they are not good at certain things because they are either “left-brained” or “right-brained”. It is doubtful that any individual habitually relies mostly on one hemisphere. II. Evolution and Physiology of Language A. Human language stands out from others because of its productivity, its ability to produce new signals to represent new ideas. B. Nonhuman precursors to language 1. Common Chimpanzees a. Common chimpanzees cannot learn to talk, but can learn some language skills using American Sign Language or other visual systems. Their use of language- related symbols differs from human language in many ways. b. The chimpanzees seldom used the symbols in new original combinations. They lack productivity. c. The chimpanzees used their symbols almost always to make a request, only rarely to describe. d. Chimpanzees show moderate understanding and can answer “What” and “Who” questions accurately. 2. Bonobos a. Bonobos (Pan paniscus) given language training used symbols in several ways that more resemble humans than common chimpanzees. They understand more information than they produce. They use symbols to name and describe objects even when they are not requesting them. They request items that they do not see. They occasionally use the symbols to describe past events. They frequently make original, creative requests. b. The reason for the better language skills in the bonobos is unknown, but three reasons have been suggested: Bonobos have more language potential than common chimpanzees. The bonobos trained so far have been very young, unlike the chimpanzees in other studies. The bonobos trained so far have learned by observation and imitation rather than formal language training. 4. Nonprimates a. Alex, an African gray parrot, could say a variety of words in conjunction with specific objects. Alex's language abilities caused many to rethink some assumptions about what sort of brain development is necessary for language. b. These studies indicate that human language evolved from precursors present in other species. C. How Did Humans Evolve Language? 1. Most theories fall into two categories: a. The first is that we evolved it as a byproduct of overall brain development. b. The second is that we evolved it as a specialization. 2. Language: Byproduct of Intelligence, or Specialized Adaptation? a. People with Normal Intelligence but Impaired Language i. Presumably because of a dominant gene, 16 (out of 30) people of normal intelligence within one family have severe language deficiencies. ii. Cases such as this suggest that genetic conditions that affect brain development can impair language without impacting other aspects of intelligence. b. People with Mental Retardation but Relatively Spared Language i. Williams syndrome: A rare disorder in which retarded individuals have skillful use of language, but limited abilities in other regards. This disorder is caused by a deletion of several genes from chromosome 7. c. Language as a Specialization i. An alternate view of the evolution of language is that language evolved as an extra brain module, called a language acquisition device. This is a built-in mechanism for acquiring language. This idea is supported by the fact that children learn language with amazing ease and that children learn language despite the fact that they do not hear enough examples to learn the grammatical structure of language (this is called the poverty of the stimulus argument). ii. Researchers have begun to explore this genetic explanation. Looking at the family with severe language deficiencies, researchers found a nutation on the genre designated FOXP2, which has an impact on the development of the jaw and throat and is essential for speech. 3. A Sensitive Period for Language Learning a. While testing the hypothesis that there is a sensitive period for language acquisition early in life, researchers found that adults are better than children at memorizing the vocabulary of a second language. However, children have a great advantage on learning the pronunciation and grammar. b. Research shows that the younger language acquisition starts, the better. People who start learning a second language beyond age 12 or so almost never reach the level of a true native speaker. c. Those who grow up in a bilingual home show substantial bilateral brain activity during speech, for both languages. In addition, their temporal and frontal cortex grow thicker than average. d. Language has a critical period; if you don’t learn language when you are young, you will forever be language-disadvantaged. D. Brain Damage and Language 1. Broca’s Aphasia a. Aphasia: Severe language impairment. b. Broca’s area: Small part of the frontal lobe of the left cerebral cortex that when damaged, leads to language impairments. c. Broca's aphasia or nonfluent aphasia: A language impairment whose most prominent symptom is a deficit in language production. Caused by damage to Broca’s area and surrounding areas. d. Patients suffering from Broca’s aphasia speak meaningfully, but omit pronouns, prepositions, conjunctions, and qualifiers from their own speech; they also have trouble understanding these same kinds of words. e. Difficulty in Language Production i. Broca’s aphasia relates to language, not just vocal muscles. English speakers with Broca’s aphasia speak most pronouns, prepositions, conjunctions, auxiliary verbs, quantifiers, and tense and number endings. ii. The problem seems to be with word meanings, not just pronunciation. f. Problems in Comprehending Grammatical Words and Devices i. People with Broca’s aphasia have trouble understanding the same kinds of words that they omit when speaking. They also misunderstand complex sentences. ii. However, they generally recognize when something is wrong in a sentence and have some knowledge of grammar. g. Broca’s Area One Step at a Time i. In some studies, physicians expose someone’s brain to explore options for treating severe epilepsy. In a few cases, researchers implanted electrodes to record activity in Broca’s area while the person listened to sentences or processed them in other ways. ii. The cells that responded first made the same response regardless of what, if anything, the person was supposed to do with the word. iii. A second group of cells responded a bit later, and responded more strongly if the instruction was to change the tense. iv. A third group, with the latest response, was active in preparation for saying the word. v. This research suggests Broca’s area goes through at least three stages in controlling speech. 2. Wernicke’s Aphasia (Fluent Aphasia) a. Wernicke's aphasia or fluent aphasia: Damage to Wernicke’s Area, near the auditory part of the temporal cortex, leads to difficulty in comprehending the verbal and written communications of others. Although patients can still speak smoothly, their speech content is often nonsensical. They also have anomia (difficulty recalling the names of objects). b. Typical characteristics of Wernicke’s aphasia are: articular speech, difficulty finding the right word, and poor language comprehension. c. People with Wernicke’s aphasia have anomia, difficulty recalling names of objects. d. Language requires the activation of many different areas other than the frontal cortex (Broca’s area and surrounding regions) and the temporal cortex (Wernicke’s area). E. Music and Language 1. Music and language have many parallels, suggesting that they may have arisen together a. Trained musicians and music students tend to be better than average at learning a second language. b. In both language and music, we alter the timing and volume to add emphasis or to express emotion. c. English speakers average about .5 to .7 seconds between one stressed syllable and another in speech, and prefer music with about .5 to .7 seconds between beats. d. Greek and Balkan languages have less regular rhythms than English, and much of the music written by speakers of those languages has irregularly spaced beats. F. Dyslexia 1. Dyslexia: Inability to read despite adequate vision and intelligence. Dyslexia is more common in boys than girls and has been linked to at least four genes that produce deficits in hearing or cognition. 2. As a rule, a dyslexic person is more likely to have a bilaterally symmetrical cerebral cortex (i.e., the planum temporale and other structures are the same size on the left and right hemisphere). 3. Different researchers have hypothesized different explanations for dyslexia including: a. Dyslexia reflects a subtle hearing impairment. b. c. d. Dyslexia is caused by a problem detecting the temporal order of sounds. Dyslexia is caused by a problem converting vision to sound or vice versa, as if one part of the brain were poorly connected to another. Dyslexia is a function of attentional differences. III. Consciousness and Unconscious Processes and Attention 1. If a cooperative person reports the presence of one stimulus, but not of a second, they were conscious of the first but not the second. 2. Consciousness is roughly equivalent to attention. 3. Can be driven by features of the stimulus, such as brightness, motion, or size, or due to “top-down” processes. 4. Inattention blindness: You are conscious of only a few things in your visual field at a time. If things change slowly or during a blink, you are unlikely to notice the change. A. The Mind-Brain Relationship 1. The mind-brain problem: What is the relationship between the mind and the brain? 2. The most widespread view among nonscientists is dualism, the belief that mind and body are different kinds of substance that exist independently. 3. The alternative is monism, the belief that the universe consists of only one kind of substance. 4. Various types of monism are: a. Materialism: the view that everything that exists is material, or physical b. Mentalism: the view that only the mind really exists and that the physical world could not exist unless some mind were aware of it c. Identity position: the view that mental processes and certain kinds of brain processes are the same thing B. Brain Activity Associated with Consciousness 1. Researchers used fMRI images to record brain activity in a young woman who was in a persistent vegetative state. When she was told to imagine playing tennis, the fMRI showed increased activity in motor areas of her cortex, similar to that of healthy volunteers. 2. One problem in solving the mind-body problem is we cannot observe consciousness. 3. Researchers use the operational definition: If a cooperative person reports awareness of one stimulus and not another, then he or she was conscious of the first and not the second. 4. The next step is to present a given stimulus under two conditions. For example, in “flash suppression,” you may be unable to see a stationary dot while other dots are flashing around it. 5. Experiments using Masking a. Masking: a brief visual stimulus is preceded and followed by longer interfering stimuli. b. Backward Masking: the same method as above but only the later stimulus is presented. c. Data on masking studies shows that consciousness of a stimuli depends on the amount and spread of brain activity. 6. Experiments Using Binocular Rivalry a. Binocular rivalry: Gradual changes in perception when two different stimuli are presented to the two eyes b. Consciousness seems to be all or none; you cannot be partially conscious of a stimulus. 7. The Fate of an Unattended Stimulus a. Much of brain activity is unconscious and can influence behavior. b. Brain attends to some things even if you are not conscious of the stimulus. 8. Consciousness as a Threshold Phenomenon a. One study suggests that consciousness is a yes/no phenomenon. When shown blurry words on a screen, participants almost never said they were partly conscious of something. They always rated words as 0 or 100 in terms of how conscious they were of it. 9. The Timing of Consciousness a. Phi phenomenon: If you see a dot in one position alternating with a similar dot nearby, it will seem that the dot is moving back and forth. b. Later perceptions alter earlier perceptions. C. Attention 1. Attention is closely aligned with consciousness. 2. Inattentional blindness or change blindness: If something in a complex scene changes slowly, you probably will not notice it unless you are paying attention to the particular item that changes. 3. Brain Areas Controlling Attention a. Bottom-up process: a reaction to a stimulus b. Top-down process: intentional. For example, when you look for someone in a crowd. c. Stroop effect: the difficulty of ignoring words and saying the color of ink 4. Unilateral Neglect d. Spatial neglect: a tendency to ignore the left side of the body and its surroundings, including visual, auditory, and touch stimuli after damage to the right hemisphere. a. If the damage to the right hemisphere is to the inferior part of the parietal cortex, the person tends to neglect everything to the left of their own body. People with damage to the superior temporal cortex neglect the left side of objects, regardless of their location. b. Spatial neglect can be reduced by doing manipulations to increase attention to the left side, such as giving instructions to attend to the left side or having the person look left while at the same time feeling something with the left hand. CHAPTER 15 MOOD DISORDERS AND SCHIZOPHRENIA Chapter Outline I. Mood Disorders A. Major Depressive Disorder 1. Major Depression: According to the DSM-IV, people with major depression feel sad, helpless, and lacking in energy and pleasure for weeks at a time. Individuals with major depression also feel worthless, have trouble sleeping, cannot concentrate, get little pleasure from sex or food, may contemplate suicide, and in many cases, can hardly imagine being happy. 2. Studies show that people with major depression reacted normally to sad or frightening depictions, but seldom smiled at comedies or pleasant pictures. In similar studies, people with depression show a decreased response to a likely reward. 3. Approximately 5% of adults in the U.S. have a “clinically significant” depression. 4. Childhood depression is equally common for boys and girls, but beyond age 14, depression is more common in females. 5. Although some people suffer from long-term depression, it is more common to have episodes of depression separated by periods of normal mood. Generally, the first episode is longer than subsequent episodes. The more one experiences an episode, the easier it is to start another one. 6. Genetics a. Evidence of genetic or other biological predispositions to depression exist. b. People with early-onset depression (before age 30) have a high probability of other relatives with depression, anxiety disorders, ADHD, alcohol or marijuana abuse, obsessive compulsive disorder, bulimia, migraine headaches, and irritable bowel syndrome. c. People with late onset depression (especially after 45 to 50) have a high probability of relatives with circulatory problems. 7. Other Biological Influences a. A few cases of depression are linked to viral infections, like Borna disease, which affects farm animals. One third of those with severe depression or bipolar disorder have Borna, which suggests it predisposes people to those illnesses. b. About 20 percent of women experience postpartum depression (depression after giving birth) and most recover quickly. 8. Abnormalities in Hemisphere Dominance a. Most people suffering from depression have decreased activity in the left hemisphere and increased activity in the right prefrontal cortex. B. Antidepressant Drugs 1. Drugs used for the treatment of depression and other mood disorders. 2. Types of Antidepressants a. Tricyclics: Prevent the presynaptic neuron from reabsorbing catecholamines or serotonin after releasing them (this allows the neurotransmitter to remain longer in the synaptic cleft thus stimulating postsynaptic receptors). b. Selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs): These drugs are similar to tricyclics, but are specific to the neurotransmitter serotonin. The most popular drug in this class is fluoxetine (Prozac). c. Serotonin norepinephrine reuptake inhibitors (SNRIs): Block the reuptake of serotonin and norepinephrine. d. Monoamine oxidase inhibitors (MAOIs): Block the enzyme monoamine oxidase (MAO) from metabolizing catecholamines and serotonin into inactive forms. e. Atypical antidepressants: A miscellaneous group of drugs with antidepressant actions and mild side effects, including bupropion (Wellbutrin), which inhibits reuptake of dopamine and to some extent norepinephrine. 3. How Do Antidepressants Work? a. People with depression have approximately normal levels of release of neurotransmitters. Some studies show that people with depression have an increase in serotonin release. b. Although some patients respond to one drug and not another, we have no clear evidence that any antidepressant drug produces any different effects from any other. c. Antidepressant drugs produce their effects on neurotransmitters in the synapses within minutes to hours but it takes weeks before patients experience mood elevation. 4. How Effective Are Antidepressants? a. Depression occurs in episodes so even those with untreated depression recover within a few months. b. The best comparisons of the benefits of drugs are comparisons between those who are treated with medication and those who are administered a placebo. Placebo results often overlap with drug group results. One study suggests that for people with mild to moderate depression, there is no clear benefit of a drug over a placebo. Furthermore, even for those with severe depression, antidepressants don’t always work. c. Another type of treatment is psychotherapy. While drugs work better for dysthymia, therapy works better for those who suffered abuse or neglect during early childhood. For such cases, antidepressants are usually ineffectual. Psychotherapy is also more likely to have long term benefits, reducing the likelihood of relapse. d. e. On average, people receiving both treatments improve more than those receiving one treatment alone. Electroconvulsive Therapy (ECT) i. Electroconvulsive therapy (ECT): Inducing seizures with an electric shock to the head. ECT is usually applied every other day for about two weeks. ii. Invented by Ladislas Meduna in the 1930’s for schizophrenia, the treatment was mostly ineffectual on schizophrenia and became overused in the 1950’s on patients without their consent. iii. One common side effect was memory loss for the few months following the shock. iv. About half of those who respond well to ECT relapse into depression within six months unless they are given antidepressant drugs or other therapies to prevent it. TMS has similar effects on depression symptoms. f. Altered Sleep Patterns i. Most depressed people have sleep problems, which precede mood changes. ii. Most depressed people enter REM sleep within 45 minutes after going to bed compared to about 80 minutes for non-depressed people and they have more than the usual number of eye movements during REM sleep. This pattern resembles someone who has traveled west for a few time zones. iii. One way to treat depression is to have the depressed person stay awake all night. Another method is to alternate the sleep schedule. 5. Other Therapies a. Regular, non-strenuous exercise increases blood flow to the brain and provides other benefits that are especially helpful to people with depression. C. Bipolar Disorder 1. Depression can be unipolar or bipolar. People with unipolar disorder vary between depression and normality. People with bipolar disorder (formerly known as manic-depression disorder) alternate between episodes of depression and mania (characterized by restless activity, excitement, laughter, self-confidence, rambling speech, and loss of inhibitions). 2. Bipolar I disorder: A type of bipolar disorder where the person has fullblown episodes of mania. 3. Bipolar II disorder: A type of bipolar disorder where the person has much milder manic phases, called hypomania. 4. Genetics a. There is a strong hereditary basis for bipolar disorder, as shown by twin studies. Two genes appear to increase the probability of bipolar II 5. disorder. They have also demonstrated that some of the same genes that predispose major depression also predispose bipolar disorder. Treatments a. Lithium salts are the most effective therapy for bipolar disorder, but how it works remains unknown. Other drug treatments include anticonvulsant drugs such as valproate and carbamazepine. Encouraging bipolar patients to keep a consistent sleep schedule may reduce the intensity of the mood swings. D. Seasonal Affective Disorder 1. Seasonal affective disorder (SAD): Depression that reoccurs seasonally, usually in the winter. 2. SAD is most common in regions closest to the poles, where the nights are very long in winter and very short in summer. 3. It is possible to treat SAD by exposing the person to very bright lights for about an hour either early in the morning or in the evening. II. Schizophrenia A. Diagnosis B. Schizophrenia was originally called dementia praecox. Eugen Bleuler came up with the term schizophrenia in 1911, which has been preferred ever since. C. Schizophrenia: A disorder characterized both by deteriorating ability to function in everyday life and by some combination of the following: 1. Delusions: Unjustifiable beliefs, such as “beings from outer space are controlling my actions” 2. Hallucinations: False sensory experiences, such as hearing voices when alone 3. Disorganized speech: rambling or incoherent 4. Grossly disorganized behavior 5. Weak or absent signs of emotion, speech, and socialization D. Positive symptoms: Behaviors that are present that should be absent. Positive symptoms fall into two clusters that do not correlate strongly with each other: E. Negative symptoms: Behaviors that are absent that should be present, such as deficits of social interaction and emotional expression. F. Cognitive symptoms: Limitations of thought and reasoning that are common in schizophrenia. G. The main problem is disordered thinking, which results from abnormal interactions between the cortex and the thalamus and cerebellum. This may lead to the hallucinations, delusions, and other symptoms. 1. Demographic Data a. Schizophrenia occurs in all ethnic groups and is slightly more common in men than in women; however, it usually develops at an earlier age in men and is more severe. About 1% of people suffer from schizophrenia at any given time. b. c. Schizophrenia is 10 to 100 times more common in the United States and Europe, as compared to many third world countries. The cause of this discrepancy is unknown. The older the age of the father at time of the baby’s birth, the greater the risk of schizophrenia. H. Genetics 1. Twin Studies a. For monozygotic schizophrenic twins, there is about a 50% concordance (agreement), and a 15% concordance for dizygotic twins. b. The greater concordance in monozygotic twins does not necessarily mean a genetic cause, as a pure genetic effect would have a 100% concordance. The greater environmental similarity in monozygotic twins, as compared to dizyogotic twins, may also influence concordance rates. c. Dizygotic twins have the same genetic resemblance as siblings but greater environmental similarity, including prenatal environment. 2. Adopted Children Who Develop Schizophrenia a. One study found that 12.5% of the immediate biological relatives and none of the adopting relatives had schizophrenia. b. These results suggest a genetic basis for schizophrenia. There is also the possibility of a prenatal influence. For example, many women with schizophrenia drink and smoke during pregnancy. 3. Efforts to Locate a Gene a. One gene has consistently been linked with schizophrenia. A dozen genes appear to be more common in people with schizophrenia. b. DISC1 (disrupted in schizophrenia 1) gene controls the production of dendritic spines and the generation of new neurons in the hippocampus. c. Other genes linked to schizophrenia are important for brain development, transmission of glutamate synapses, and connections between the hippocampus and the prefrontal cortex. d. Schizophrenia may result from a combination of genetic and environmental factors. C. The neurodevelopmental hypothesis: Schizophrenia is caused in large part by abnormalities to the nervous system during the prenatal or neonatal periods. 1. Prenatal and Neonatal Environment a. Several factors could have affected the infant’s brain development, including poor nutrition of the mother during pregnancy, premature birth, low birth weight, and complications during delivery. b. If a mother is Rh-negative and her baby is Rh-positive, the baby’s Rhpositive blood may trigger an immunological rejection by the mother. The result is hearing deficits, mental retardation, and twice the usual probability of schizophrenia. c. Season-of-birth effect: the tendency for people born in winter to have a slightly greater possibility of developing schizophrenia. Some reason this may occurs is nutrition during winter, viral infections, fever, and influenza. d. Childhood infections, like toxoplasma gondii, also infect humans and leads to memory disorders, hallucinations, and delusions. Because this bacteria only reproduces in cats, people with schizophrenia are more likely than other people to have a pet cat in childhood. 2. Mild Brain Abnormalities a. On average, people with schizophrenia have less than average gray matter and white matter, and larger than average ventricles—the fluidfilled spaces within the brain. b. The strongest deficits are in the left temporal and frontal areas of the cortex. The thalamus is also smaller than normal for people with schizophrenia. c. The areas with consistent signs of abnormality include some that mature slowly such as the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. Most people with schizophrenia show deficits in memory and attention because of these deficiencies. d. Lateralization also differs, with the left hemisphere slightly larger than the right. e. The areas of the brain that most consistently show signs of abnormality in schizophrenics are the ones that mature the most slowly, such as the prefrontal cortex. f. At a microscopic level, people with schizophrenia have smaller than normal cell bodies, especially in the frontal cortex and hippocampus. g. Research has not yet determined whether these brain damages will worsen with time. 3. Early Development and Later Psychopathology a. It is currently thought that the early brain damage is done in areas that are slow to mature, such as the prefrontal cortex. For this reason, the damage produces only minor symptoms in childhood, but increasing impairments when the brain area fully matures. b. Those later developed with schizophrenia often have other problems in childhood, like deficits in attention, memory, and impulse control. D. Treatments 1. Antipsychotic Drugs and Dopamine a. Chlorpromazine (Thorazine): First drug used successfully for the treatment of schizophrenia. b. Antipsychotic drugs (neuroleptic drugs): Drugs used for the treatment of schizophrenia. These drugs work primarily by blocking dopamine receptors. Phenothiazines: A class of neuroleptic drugs that includes chlorpromazine. Butyrophenones: A class of neuroleptic drugs that includes haloperidol (Haldol). c. Dopamine hypothesis of schizophrenia: According to this hypothesis, schizophrenia results from excess activity at certain dopamine d. e. f. 2. 3. synapses. The primary evidence for this hypothesis is the type of drugs that relieve and aggravate the symptoms of schizophrenia. Substance-induced psychotic disorder: Disorder characterized by hallucinations and delusions caused by drugs such as cocaine, amphetamine, and LSD, which increase the activity of dopamine synapses. Schizophrenic people have about twice as many D2 receptors occupied by dopamine as normal people. Excess activity of dopamine cannot be the sole cause of schizophrenia. Drugs that block dopamine receptors do so almost immediately, but their effects on behavior build up gradually over 2 or 3 weeks. Role of Glutamate a. Glutamate hypothesis of schizophrenia: Idea that schizophrenia results from deficient activity at certain glutamate synapses. Because dopamine inhibits glutamate activity in many parts of the brain, much of the evidence supporting the dopamine hypothesis of schizophrenia also supports the glutamate hypothesis of schizophrenia. b. Researchers have found that the brains of schizophrenic people release lower than normal amounts of glutamate in the prefrontal cortex and hippocampus. Schizophrenics also have fewer glutamate receptors. c. Phencyclidine (PCP): A drug that blocks NMDA glutamate receptors. PCP administration produces a type of psychosis more similar to schizophrenia than drugs like cocaine, as PCP induces both negative and positive symptoms. Moreover, PCP does not produce psychosis in preadolescents and PCP produces a much more severe psychosis in people with a history of schizophrenia. d. Because increasing glutamate activity in the brain would be extremely risky, there are no drugs used to treat schizophrenia that directly stimulate glutamate activity. However, there are some experimental compounds that may someday be used to treat schizophrenia, such as the amino acid glycine, which enhances the effects of glutamate at NMDA synapses. Glycine is not an effective antipsychotic by itself but it increases the effects of other antipsychotic drugs. New Drugs a. Mesolimbocortical system: A set of neurons which project from the midbrain tegmentum to the limbic system. The mesolimbocortical system is believed to be the area in which antipsychotics have their beneficial effects. b. Tardive dyskinesia: A serious side effect of antipsychotics; this disorder is characterized by tremors and other involuntary movements. Tardive dyskinesia is caused by the prolonged blockade of dopamine receptors in the basal ganglia. c. d. Second generation (atypical) antipsychotics: New drugs (e.g., clozapine) that alleviate the symptoms of schizophrenia while seldom, if ever, producing movement problems. These drugs have less intense effects on dopamine type D2 receptors, but stronger effects at D4 and serotonin 5-HT2 receptors. Atypical antipsychotics are more effective than typical antipsychotics at relieving the positive symptoms and, to some extent, the negative symptoms of schizophrenia, but they do not improve overall quality of life more than the typical antipsychotic drugs.