Unbelievable eyewitnesses: the legal system has long relied on eyewitness testimony in the determination of guilt, but how reliable are eyewitnesses? Researcher Cara Laney takes a look at how and when eyewitness testimony can be less than believable Pub:Psychology Review Detail:Cara Laney. 15.2 (Nov 2009): p.9(5). (3378 words) From General OneFile. Full Text:COPYRIGHT 2009 Philip Allan Updates Eyewitness testimony is what happens when an eyewitness gets up on the stand and recalls for the court all the details of the crime that they saw. It is also the complicated process leading up to the witness standing in that courtroom. This includes what happens during the actual crime to facilitate or hamper witnessing, as well as everything that happens between the criminal event and the courtroom appearance. This may include the witness being interviewed by the police--perhaps several times--and numerous lawyers, describing the perpetrator to several different people, making an identification of the perpetrator and other steps. This article addresses just a few of those steps. In court, the testimony of victims and eyewitnesses is often trusted more than other kinds of evidence because the witnesses were present when the crime was committed and can describe what happened from their own memories. This kind of testimony is particularly compelling for several reasons. * It is often quite specific and detailed. * It is normally conveyed with a great deal of confidence (particularly after several interviews). * It is also often accompanied by substantial emotional weight. And yet, eyewitness testimony is often quite unreliable for a variety of reasons. Several of these reasons--as well as possible ways to prevent eyewitness errors--are discussed below. Why research eyewitness testimony? Eyewitness testimony has been a popular research topic for cognitive psychologists, social psychologists, and specialised forensic psychologists since the 1970s. Several areas of eyewitness testimony research are touched on in this article. However, these areas are really just the tip of the iceberg. The best reason to research eyewitness testimony is that this research can ultimately be used to prevent eyewitness errors. That is, this research has the potential to make a real difference to people's lives--by making the justice system work better. Classic eyewitness testimony research Loftus and Palmer (1974) showed participants short films of car accidents and then asked them questions about what they had seen. Some participants were asked, 'About how fast were the cars going when they hit each other?' Other participants were instead asked how fast the cars were going when they smashed into each other or bumped each other. Those who were given a question using smashed later estimated that the cars were going faster than those given questions using other verbs. Participants asked about cars that smashed were also more likely to report that there was broken glass in the video (which there was not). Another classic 1970s study looked at the importance of positive lineup (or identity parade) identifications. Loftus (1974; see also Loftus 1979) gave three different groups of participants slightly different versions of a case against a particular defendant. All three versions of the story included the same circumstantial evidence. The three versions varied in their eyewitness evidence. For one group, there was no eyewitness identification; just 18% of these participants voted guilty. For the second group, there was a single positive identification made by an eyewitness and 72% found this compelling enough to vote for conviction. For the third group, there was an identification but it was, critically, a poor quality, even useless, identification. In this case, the witness had poor vision and so could not actually see the perpetrator from where he was standing. That is, objectively, this identification added nothing whatsoever to the case, so this group should have voted like the first group, because objectively they had similar levels of evidence to work with. In fact, though, they voted to convict at nearly the same rate as the second group (68%). That is, even though their eyewitness evidence was useless, it had as much weight as if it were genuinely useful. Any identification made by an eyewitness seems to be meaningful, even if it really is not. How lineup identifications go wrong Lineup identifications can go wrong for a variety of different reasons, from poor initial viewing conditions (or poor vision) to too much delay between the eye-witnessed event and the identification. Other factors include: * too many perpetrators (because it is harder to identify several people than one) * too little attention paid at the time (perhaps because the eyewitness did not know that they were witnessing a crime until later) * severe stress caused by the eye-witnessed event (though some other types of emotions can actually be beneficial) Eyewitnesses' memory for the perpetrator, as well as other details of the witnessed event, can also become contaminated by information from biased or leading questions (as in the smashed car study described above) or details reported by co-witnesses, which I discuss below. Another thing that can make lineups unfair is the feedback that witnesses often get immediately after they make a selection from a lineup. This was originally demonstrated by Wells and Bradfield (1998). In their study, participants watched a clip of grainy security camera footage. Then they were asked to pick the perpetrator out of a lineup. In fact, the perpetrator was not in the lineup at all, so all participants necessarily chose incorrectly. Some participants were then told that they had correctly identified the suspect. Other (control) participants were not given this feedback. Relative to the control group, those given positive feedback were, perhaps unsurprisingly, more confident that they had chosen correctly. What is surprising, though, is that they also reported better memories for the witnessed event, faster estimates of their time to make the identification and even clearer views of the original event. This has been termed the 'postidentification feedback effect'. This effect is problematic, because it means that judgements of confidence and memory made after an identification are not trustworthy. This is a bad thing, because juries put huge stock in the confidence of eyewitnesses. An eyewitness who reports that he/she was extremely confident when he/she made her identification is believed much more than an eyewitness who says he/she was not confident. The solution to this problem is to run lineups 'double blind'. This term is borrowed from medical research, where most studies are run double blind. This means that neither the patient nor the person handing out pills knows which condition--the active drug condition or the placebo condition--the patient is in. That way, the doctor cannot accidentally give out hints that let the patient know whether they are receiving the active drug. If lineups were run double blind, with neither the lineup administrator nor the witness knowing who the suspect is, then the lineup administrator would not be able to tell the eyewitness that he had picked the suspect, because the administrator would not know. Co-witnesses As already mentioned, witnesses tend to talk to one another, which is problematic. When two, three or more people witness the same crime, they are quite likely to share their perceptions with one another immediately afterwards--for instance, while they wait for police to arrive. They may also end up bonding over the event and talk more on a later occasion. But because different witnesses are different people with different perspectives, they will see or notice different things and thus remember different things. So, when they communicate with one another about the crime, they not only reinforce common memories for the event, they also contaminate each other's memories, much as leading questions contaminate memory. What Eric did This effect was modelled in the laboratory. Gary et al. (2007) asked participants to watch a video in groups of two. Both participants sat in front of the same screen, but one participant wore glasses polarised in one direction, and the other wore classes polarised in the other direction. Two different versions of the same video were projected onto the screen the participants were watching. So, although they were both watching the same screen and believed (quite reasonably) that they were watching the same video, they were actually watching two different versions of the video. In the video, Eric the electrician is seen wandering through an unoccupied house stealing various items. A total of eight details were different between the two videos. After watching the videos, the co-witnesses worked together on 12 memory test questions. Four of these questions dealt with details that were different in the two versions of the video, so participants had the chance to influence each other. Then participants worked individually on 20 additional memory test questions. Eight of these were for details that were different in the two videos. The results from these eight questions are the most interesting. For the new questions, where no discussion occurred, participants answered an average of nearly 80% right. But for questions where they had previously discussed their answers with a fellow participant who saw something slightly different, accuracy fell to below 40%. That is, participants allowed their co-witnesses to corrupt their memories about what they had seen. False memories Several of the studies already discussed prove that memory can become distorted by new information, including information from leading questions or co-witnesses. But additional research shows that wholly false memories can be created for events that never took place. In the original false memory study, Loftus and Pickrell (1995) first called up their participants' family members to ask for some events from the participants' childhoods. The researchers then brought the participants into the laboratory and told them that they had talked to their families. They said that their families had provided details of four different childhood events. This was actually the case for three of the events, but the fourth was made up by the researchers. The researchers told the participants that they had been lost in a shopping mall as children. The researchers had, in fact, asked the families about this event too, but to make sure that it did not happen. Participants were asked to think about the four events and to describe them in a booklet in as much detail as they could remember. They were then interviewed about the events on two different occasions. By the end of the study, the participants had provided details about some 68% of the true memories and also 25% of the false memories. Participants sometimes provided substantial detail about being lost in the mall, even though it was made up by the researchers. More recent research has looked at other aspects of false memory creation, including the extent to which false memories can be emotional. There is some evidence that jurors put even more stock into emotional testimony than they put into eyewitness testimony generally. As mentioned previously, jurors seem to think that any eyewitness identification (even one from an unreliable eyewitness) is good evidence that the defendant is guilty. Jurors also seem to assume that, if the witness is extremely emotional when describing their memory, then their memory must be accurate. But this is actually an empirical question. Are emotional memories necessarily true memories or can false memories be emotional too? To find out, Laney and Loftus (2008) compared true and false memories for the same three emotional childhood events: being hospitalised overnight, catching one's parents having sex and witnessing a physically violent fight between one's parents. The researchers gave some participants false memories for these events, using a false feedback procedure, and found other participants who had true memories for the same events. The two types of memories were then compared to see which were more emotional. If an emotional memory is necessarily a true memory, then when the two types of memories are measured on a scale of emotionality, the true memories should come out far ahead of the false memories. That is, if an emotional memory is necessarily a true memory, then there should be no such thing as an emotional false memory. So, a false memory for a particular emotional event should be much less emotional than a true memory for the same emotional event. The results did not, however, show that true emotional memories were more intense than false ones. Instead, the two types of memories were virtually identical in terms of their emotionality. This means that false memories can be emotional and that the assumption that emotional memories are necessarily true memories is false. Minimising errors Eyewitness testimony research has made a variety of suggestions for ways to improve the justice system, some of which have been implemented in the real world. Examples of things that can be done and are being implemented in some places are: * use of interview techniques that minimise leading questions * double blind lineups * sequential lineups (where witnesses make 'yes' or 'no' judgements to each of a series of individuals, rather than choosing the 'best' from a group of individuals in a simultaneous lineup) * the use of live or video lineups rather than photographs (often called '6-packs' in the USA and on television shows like Law & Order and CSI) Conclusions Eyewitness testimony is powerful and convincing to jurors, even though it is not particularly reliable. Identification errors occur and these errors can lead to people being falsely accused and even convicted. Likewise, eyewitness memory can be corrupted by leading questions, misinterpretations of events, and conversations with co-witnesses. People can even come to remember whole events that never occurred. However, some of these errors can be minimised by psychologists working with the justice system. Key words Co-witnesses Eyewitness testimony Leading questions Emotional memory False memory Lineup identification Student activities Tutors, examiners, researches and textbook writers all tell us how important it is to be active in our learning Anthony Curtis gives some useful activities that will consolidate and enhance your understanding of the article you have just read. 1 Having read Cara Laney's article--which focuses on how and when eyewitness testimony can be less than believable--now work through the following questions. You may want to discuss your answers with your fellow students in small groups, or you may prefer simply to discuss the questions in a whole group plenary. 2 Cara Laney highlights a range of different reasons as to how and why eyewitness testimony may be unreliable. Before we examine some of these in more detail, it is useful just to stand back for a moment and ask yourself what is meant by the term 'eyewitness testimony'. For example, does this term refer only to accounts that are presented in a court of law? What about police interviews, for example? What forms of testimony would they include? Consider the following: * recall of people, places and events * recall of stressful and non-stressful events You will soon discover that there is much more to the term 'eyewitness testimony' than you might think. Active reconstruction of previous events often involves interrelated processes of perception, attention, memory, language and thought. Perhaps this is one reason why this applied research field is growing all the time and can be quite complex to understand in detail. 3 Cara Laney cites the work of Elizabeth Loftus on eyewitness testimony. She worked with Loftus recently while completing her PhD thesis. No doubt you know and understand these classic studies of eyewitness account by Loftus, having studied them in class. Laney's article explains some of the wider factors involved in eyewitness testimony which Loftus' early research focused on, but did not elaborate further (e.g. problems involving selective attention, stress and other co-witnesses). All this may influence or affect the 'believability' or 'trustworthiness' of witnesses in court. * Make a list of all the factors discussed by Laney in her article which may affect the accuracy of eyewitness testimony and explain, in your own words, how you consider these factors might affect the reliability of the subsequent testimony. This is important; it is not enough at A-level simply to state things like, 'And this resulted in poorer recall of events'. Instead, you need to explain how, or in what ways, specific factors produced specific kinds of effects e.g. The 'post-identification feedback effect' may distort the memory of events because participants who are given 'false', but positive, feedback following identification of suspects from a lineup were more confident, reported better memories, gave faster estimates of their times to make identifications and even reported clearer views of the event compared with a control group, who did not receive positive feedback, To maximise your AO2 marks, you can then elaborate this by saying, 'This suggests that...' or 'The implications of these findings are that...' 4 Cara Laney also writes about the powerful effect of the so-called 'co-witnesses' in influencing recall accuracy in different ways (e.g. positively, through reinforcing common memories or negatively, through leading questions, which may then contaminate individual memories). [ILLUSTRATION OMITTED] * Read and briefly summarise in your own words the study by Gary et al. (2007) involving 'Eric the electrician', showing how other people may corrupt memories of an event. How might you design a study investigating both the positive and negative effects of the presence of others on eyewitness memory (e.g. in comparison with a control group)? Focus here on what you may already know about how the presence of others may help us contaminate or consolidate factual information. * Do you think, therefore, that the presence of others is a good or a bad thing in enhancing the reliability of eyewitness testimony? Explain and justify (i.e. elaborate) your answer. 5 Cara Laney also reports that false memories can be created for events that never took place. These memories are usually emotional in nature and rooted in childhood disturbances. The study Laney cites by Laney and Loftus (2008) shows that the true memories were no more or less emotional than the false ones. This is contrary to the belief that it is only true memories about an emotional event that are genuinely emotional and this, intriguingly, suggests that emotion can attach itself to either type of memory. Given that jurors seem to attach more weight to 'emotional' eyewitness testimonies, what are the implications of the findings by Laney and Loftus? For example, should we encourage jurors not to be swayed by the emotional content of testimonies and focus instead on the evidence that is presented? 6 Read the wider recommendations provided by Cara Laney for minimising errors in the justice system, thereby improving it. What other recommendations can you provide for courts in general and jurors in particular? 7 Visit Elizabeth Loftus's website at http://faculty.washington.edu/ eloftus/and browse some of her key articles on eyewitness testimony. Many will take you to further studies that are not covered in your psychology textbook. In addition, Wikipedia provides a comprehensive summary of the wider contribution of Elizabeth Loftus to psychology at: http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Elizabeth_Loftus These two sites will give you an idea of just how significant a contribution Elizabeth Loftus has made. In 2002, Loftus came fifty-eighth on a list of the 100 most influential researchers in psychology in the twentieth century and the highest-ranked woman on the list. As a professor of psychology and of law, Elizabeth Loftus has made a unique and lasting contribution to our understanding of how psychology continues to play a crucial role in the legal system in general and the reliability of eyewitness testimony in particular. Anthony Curtis is Head of Psychology at Hartpury College, UWE, Associate Lecturer at De Montfort University and an editor of PSYCHOLOGY REVIEW. References Gary, M. et al. (2007) 'Eyewitness memory following discussion: using the MORI technique with a Western sample', Applied Cognitive Psychology, Vol. 22, pp. 431-39. Laney, C. and Loftus, E. F. (2008) 'Emotional content of true and false memories', Memory, Vol. 16, pp. 500-16. Loftus, E. F. (1974) 'Reconstructing memory: the incredible eyewitness', Psychology Today, Vol. 8, pp. 116-19. Loftus, E. F. (1979) Eyewitness Testimony, Harvard University Press. Loftus, E. F. and Palmer, J. C. (1974) 'Reconstruction of automobile destruction', Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, Vol. 13, pp. 585-89. Loftus, E. F. and Pickrell, J. E. (1995) 'The formation of false memories', Psychiatric Annals, Vol. 25, pp. 720-25. Wells, G. L. and Bradfield, A. L. (1998) '"Good, you identified the suspect": feedback to eyewitnesses distorts their reports of the witnessing experience', Journal of Applied Psychology, Vol. 83, pp. 360-76. Dr Cara Laney is a lecturer in forensic psychology at the University of Leicester. She completed her PhD at the University of California in 2006, working with Elizabeth Loftus. Her primary research area is human memory and the various interesting ways that it can go wrong. Source Citation Laney, Cara. "Unbelievable eyewitnesses: the legal system has long relied on eyewitness testimony in the determination of guilt, but how reliable are eyewitnesses? Researcher Cara Laney takes a look at how and when eyewitness testimony can be less than believable." Psychology Review Nov. 2009: 9+. General OneFile. Web. 29 Nov. 2010. Document URL http://find.galegroup.com/gps/infomark.do?&contentSet=IACDocuments&type=retrieve&tabID=T003&prodId=IPS&docId=A212276558&source=gal e&srcprod=ITOF&userGroupName=camb70211&version=1.0 Gale Document Number:A212276558