Motivation for Excellence: Results from the National Braille Challenge Study Research in the Rockies 2010: Research Summit on Braille Reading and Writing Presentation Summary Adam Wilton, MA and Cay Holbrook, PhD The University of British Columbia Introduction The research literature in educational psychology spanning the past 30 years is replete with theories and constructs of academic motivation (see Wigfield & Eccles, 2002). According to Covington (2000), “the concept of motivation stands at the centre of the educational enterprise” (p.171). The importance of achievement motivation to students’ learning outcomes cannot be overstated, yet this relationship may not be widely understood by professionals working in educational settings. While this relationship is of general importance to all students, some populations of students are at risk for lower motivation for academic success. This may be true for students with severe visual impairments, who may more vulnerable to external loci of academic control than sighted populations (Spenser, Head, VanDusen, & Chalfant, 1997; Tuttle & Tuttle, 2004). Students with external loci of control do not see academic outcomes as contingent on their knowledge, effort, skills, etc. and may not be motivated to take on new intellectual challenges. It is therefore critical that professionals working with students with visual impairments have a sophisticated understanding of the construct of achievement motivation, as well as its underlying component constructs. The current research examines several of these component constructs in detail, contextualizing them in a special program designed to support students’ motivation for academic achievement in braille reading and writing – the Braille Challenge. Relevant Constructs from the Achievement Motivation Literature Despite the wide application of the term across educational contexts, “achievement motivation” is not a unitary construct (Wigfield & Eccles, 2002). There are numerous constructs that comprise the student’s “motivation” to engage in academic pursuits (Wentzel & Wigfield, 1998). In an effort to better understand the achievement motivation of students with visual impairments in context, several of these constructs are important to the current study. Two of these constructs (academic locus of control and self-monitoring) are outlined in the following section. Academic Locus of Control. The locus of control construct has traditionally been of interest to researchers and professionals in education (Kalechstein & Nowicki, 1997). Generally stated, locus of control refers to the individual’s “perceptions of control over access to reinforcements” (Anderson, Hattie, & Hamilton, 2005, p.517). If reinforcement is not seen as “contingent on the subject’s own behaviour, then it will not increase the subject's expectancy that a particular behaviour or event will be followed by a reinforcement in the future” (Stipek & Weisz, 1981, p.102). When applied to student learning, the academic locus of control refers to the student’s belief that academic outcomes are contingent on personal characteristics (i.e. effort, knowledge, ability) (Kalachstein & Nowicki, 1997). Research with sighted learners has consistently shown that an internal academic locus of control is positively related to academic success (Findley & Cooper, 1983; Kalachstein & Nowicki, 1997). While there is a dearth of corresponding research with samples of students with visual impairments, similar conclusions are drawn from research involving this population (i.e. Parsons, 1987). Self-Monitoring. Several researchers highlight the importance of the student’s ability to actively monitor his or her own progress as he or she works towards achieving academic goals, for both typically-developing and disabled populations (Lee, Palmer, & Wehmeyer, 2009; Rock, 2005). Specifically, self-monitoring has been identified as an important component of self-determination instruction for students with visual impairments – self-determination being a component area of the Expanded Core Curriculum for these students (Agran, Hong, & Blankenship, 2007; Hatlen, 2009). Overview of the Braille Challenge The Braille Challenge, administered and sponsored by the Braille Institute of America, is a skill competition open to students in grades one through twelve from across the United States and Canada who use braille as their primary literacy medium. In 2010 overall participation was 723 students, up from 450 in 2007 (Niebrugge, 2010). This year, students came from 34 regional sites and represented 43 US states and 4 Canadian provinces. The growing popularity of the Challenge is made evident by a comparison to 2005 figures – when 6 regional sites hosted preliminary events (Niebrugge, 2010). This popularity is also demonstrated by the fact that according to national figures in the US, it is estimated that 13% of school-age braille readers participate in the Challenge. Programming at the Braille Challenge The Braille Challenge consists of two stages of competition (preliminary and national). Preliminary challenges are held in one of two formats. They can be organized as regional events, bringing together students with visual impairments from local communities. Preliminary challenges are held individually or in small group settings, providing that a teacher of students with visual impairments proctors the event. Once students’ performance has been scored, top students are invited to the National Braille Challenge in Los Angeles, California. For the 2010 National Challenge, 60 finalists have been invited to participate from the original pool of 723 contestants. The Challenge is composed of 5 discrete competitions (Niebrugge, 2010). Within these competitions, activities are unique and do not remain fixed across yearly Challenges. Each competition focuses on a particular skill area critical for optimal use of braille as a literacy medium: Reading comprehension (All grade levels) Speed and accuracy (Grades 5-12) Proofreading (All grade levels) Spelling (Grades 1-4) Charts and graphs (Grades 5-12) Survey Development and Sampling In an effort to examine the relationship between these motivation constructs and participation in the Braille Challenge more closely, the parent/guardian survey was guided by the following research questions (corresponding hypotheses are included): R1: Are there any motivational characteristics common to students who participate in the Braille Challenge competition, as rated by their parent/guardian? H1: Participants in the Braille Challenge will demonstrate a similar constellation of positive motivational characteristics, as rated by their parent/guardian. R2: What is the perceived impact of the child’s participation in the Braille Challenge on motivational outcomes, as rated by parents/guardians? H2: Parents/guardians will indicate that there is a significant positive relationship between their child’s motivational outcomes and their child’s participation in the Braille Challenge. The purpose of the teacher survey was to understand if or how teachers incorporate the Braille Challenge into their classroom practice. The teacher survey was guided by the following research questions (corresponding hypotheses are included): R3: What influence does a student’s participation in the Braille Challenge have on a teacher of students with visual impairment’s classroom practice? H3: Teachers of students with visual impairments will integrate, promote and capitalize on their students’ participation in the Braille Challenge in the context of their own braille instruction. R4: What is the perceived impact of the students’ participation in the Braille Challenge on motivational outcomes, as rated by teachers? H4: Teachers of students with visual impairments will indicate that there is a significant positive relationship between their students’ motivational outcomes and their students’ participation in the Braille Challenge. Survey Design and Distribution The parent/guardian survey contains 17 multiple choice and scalar questions, and took an average of 4:20 to complete. A hyperlink to the online survey was sent via electronic mail to 438 valid email addresses. From this original mailing, 57 completed surveys were obtained, resulting in a return rate of 13.0%. The teacher survey contains 15 multiple choice and scalar questions, and took an average of 3:30 to complete. A hyperlink to the online survey was sent via electronic mail to 338 valid email addresses. From this original mailing, 65 completed surveys were obtained, resulting in a return rate of 19.2%. Study Participants Relevant information pertaining to parent/guardian participants was gathered in the context of their child’s participation in the Braille Challenge. Table 1 reports on the distribution of grade levels at which these children first participated in the Challenge. Graph 1 displays these data. Please note that all data outlined in this document are displayed in both table and graph format to ensure their accessibility. Grade Level 1 Percentage 25% of Sample 2 3 4 5 39% 39% 45% 29% 6 7 8 9 11% 11% 11% 9% 10 11 12 7% 5% 1% Table 1: Distribution of initial Braille Challenge participation by students – Reported by parents/guardians Graph 1: Distribution of Initial Braille Challenge Participation of Students – Reported by Parents/Guardians These data indicate that the majority of parents/guardians report first participation in the Braille Challenge early in their child’s educational career. It is also reasonable to assume that many of these students continue to participate, since over 50% of finalists in 2009 were participating in at least their second National Challenge (Niebrugge, 2010). Therefore, this sample of parents/guardians is reasonably well informed about the Braille Challenge and is well positioned to comment on its influence on child outcomes. Based upon parent/guardian responses, 45% of children had participated in the National Braille Challenge in Los Angeles. Thus, the sample contains roughly equal numbers of parents/guardians whose child has only participated at one stage of the competition. This distinction enables distinctions to be made between parent/guardian perceptions of children who have, and have not, advanced to the National Challenge. Teachers indicated their experience with the Challenge as measured by the number of participating students, as well as their years of service as a teacher of students with visual impairments. Table 2 reports on the distribution of the number of students that a teacher has had participate in the Braille Challenge at any level (individually, locally, or regionally). Graph 2 displays these data. Number of 1 Students 2 Percentage 45.61% 17.54% of Sample 3 4 5 6 Do not know 17.54% 5.26% 3.51% 7.02% 3.51% Table 2: Distribution of number of students who have competed at any level – Reported by teachers. Graph 2: Distribution of Number of Students who have Competed at any Level – Reported by Teachers. Table 2/Graph 2 reveal that 45.61% of teachers have only ever had one student participate in the Braille Challenge at any level. This is not surprising, given the significant growth of the Braille Challenge in recent years (see Section I). It is also interesting to note that of the students represented above, 54% have participated in the National Challenge. Thus, in the current sample, many teachers’ experience of the Braille Challenge is that of one student participant. Results Parent/Guardian Survey The parent/guardian survey contains one item that directly assesses the priority that parents/guardians place on increased motivation as a result of their child’s participation in the Braille Challenge. “What are your goals for your child’s participation in the Braille Challenge? – To encourage and motivate my child to continue to use braille in his/her daily life.” Rating of Critically importance important Important Somewhat important Not important Percentage of sample 20.45% 6.82% 4.55% 68.18% Table 3: Distribution of parent/guardian ratings of importance of increased motivation through child’s participation Graph 3: Distribution of parent/guardian ratings of importance of increased motivation through child’s participation Academic locus of control. The parent/guardian survey contains three items that assess parents’ perceptions of their child’s locus of causality as it relates to academic endeavours. Parents/guardians indicate their level of agreement with the following statement: “My child feels that her/his achievement is highly dependent on his/her effort.” Strongly agree Percentage 54.35% of Sample Somewhat agree 28.26% Neutral 2.17% Somewhat disagree 8.7% Strongly disagree 4.35% Do not know 2.17% Table 4: Distribution of parent/guardian responses when asked about child’s academic locus of control (effort) Graph 4: Distribution of parent/guardian responses when asked about child’s academic locus of control (knowledge) Parents/guardians indicate their level of agreement with the following statement: “My child feels that her/his achievement is highly dependent on his/her knowledge.” Strongly agree Percentage 53.33% of Sample Somewhat agree 31.11% Neutral 6.67% Somewhat disagree 4.44% Strongly disagree 2.22% Do not know 2.22% Table 5: Distribution of parent/guardian responses when asked about child’s academic locus of control (knowledge) Graph 5: Distribution of parent/guardian responses when asked about child’s academic locus of control (skills) Parents/guardians indicate their level of agreement with the following statement: “My child feels that her/his achievement is highly dependent on his/her skills.” Strongly agree Percentage 53.33% of Sample Somewhat agree 31.11% Neutral 6.67% Somewhat disagree 4.44% Strongly disagree 2.22% Do not know 2.22% Table 6: Distribution of parent/guardian responses when asked about child’s academic locus of control (skills) Graph 6: Distribution of parent/guardian responses when asked about child’s academic locus of control (skills) Self-monitoring. One question explicitly evaluates parent/guardians’ perception of their child’s ability to actively engage in self-monitoring in academic contexts. Parents/guardians indicate their level of agreement with the following statement: “My child independently monitors his/her own progress towards achieving his/her own goals.” Strongly Somewhat Neutral Somewhat Strongly Do not agree agree disagree disagree know Percentage 31.91% 38.3% 10.64% 4.26% 12.77% 2.13% of Sample Table 7: Distribution of parent/guardian responses when asked about child’s self-monitoring Graph 7: Distribution of parent/guardian responses when asked about child’s self-monitoring Teacher Survey The teacher survey contains one item that directly assesses the priority that teachers place on increased motivation as a result of students’ participation in the Braille Challenge. “What are your general expectations for your students’ participation in the Braille Challenge? To encourage and motivate my student to continue to use braille in his/her daily life.” Rating of Critically importance important Important Somewhat important Not important Percentage of sample 35.59% 8.47% 0% 55.93% Table 8: Distribution of parent/guardian ratings of importance of increased motivation through student participation Graph 8: Distribution of teacher ratings of importance of increased motivation through student participation Integration into Practice. There is one question on the teacher survey that explicitly evaluates teachers’ perceptions of their integration of motivational aspects of the Braille Challenge into their classroom practice. “When preparing my student(s) to participate in the Braille Challenge, I generally use the prospect of participating in the Challenge to motivate my student(s) to ready themselves.” Always Mostly Some of the time Rarely Never Percentage 45.45% of sample 32.73% 18.18% 0% 3.64% Frequency Table 9: Frequency of teacher use of participation in the Braille Challenge as a motivational tool Graph 9: Frequency of teacher use of participation in the Braille Challenge as a motivational tool Conclusions These conclusions are based, only in part, on the data presented in this summary. More complete analyses will be outlined at the conference presentation. Taken together, the data outlined in the previous section largely validate the research hypotheses listed earlier in this summary. According to parent/guardian perceptions, there are some motivational characteristics (i.e. academic locus of control and self-monitoring) that are common to participants in the Braille Challenge. However, some characteristics (academic locus of control) are rated as being more present among Challenge participants than others (self-monitoring) (H1 partially supported). Parents support the assertion that participation in the Braille Challenge has a positive influence on the child’s motivation for academic success (H2 supported). Teachers of students with visual impairments work to integrate motivational features of the Braille Challenge into their own educational practice (H3 partially supported). Teachers support the assertion that participation in the Braille Challenge has a positive influence on the student’s motivation for academic success (H4 supported). References Agran, M., Hong, S., & Blankenship, K. (2007). Promoting the self-determination of students with visual impairments: Reducing the gap between knowledge and practice. Journal of Visual Impairment and Blindness, 101, 453-464. Anderson, A., Hattie, J., & Hamilton, R.J. (2005). Locus of control, self-efficacy, and motivation in different schools: Is moderation the key to success? Educational Psychology, 25, 517-535. Covington, M.V. (2000). Goal theory, motivation, and school achievement: An integrative review. Annual Review of Psychology, 51, 171-200. Findley, M.J., & Cooper, H.M. (1983). Locus of control and academic achievement: A literature review. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44, 419-427. Hatlen, P. (2009). The opportunity to be equal, the right to be different. Austin, TX: The Texas School for the Blind and Visually Impaired. Kalenchstein, A.D., & Nowicki, S. (1997). A meta-analytic examination of the relationship between control expectancies and academic achievement: an 11-year follow-up to Findley and Cooper. Genetic, Social, and General Psychology Monographs, 123, 29-57. Lee, S., Palmer, S.B., & Wehmeyer, M.L. (2009). Goal setting and self-monitoring for students with disabilities: Practical tips and ideas for teachers. Intervention in School and Clinic, 44, 139-145. Parsons, S. (1987). Locus of control and adaptive behaviour in visually impaired children. Journal of Visual Impairment and Blindness, 81, 29-32. Niebrugge, N. (2010, April). What the analysis of the Braille Challenge surveys & scores tell us about school-age braille readers. Presented at the 51st Annual Conference of the California Transcribers and Educators for the Blind and Visually Impaired, Los Angeles, CA. Rock, M.L. (2005). Use of strategic self-monitoring to enhance academic engagement, productivity, and accuracy of students with and without exceptionalities. Journal of Positive Behavior Interventions, 7, 3-17. Spencer, R.A., Head, D.N., Van Dusen, M., & Chalfant, J.C. (1997). Response patterns of children with visual impairments on measures of internalized self-responsibility. RE:view, 29, 121-127. Stipek, D.J., & Weisz, J.R. (1981). Perceived personal control and academic achievement. Review of Education Research, 51, 101-137. Tuttle, D.W., & Tuttle, N.R. (2004). Self-Esteem and Adjusting with Blindness: The Process of Responding to Life’s Demands. 3rd Ed. Springfield, Il: Charles C. Thomas Publishing Ltd. Wentzel, K.R., & Wigfield, A. (1998). Academic and social motivational influences on students’ academic performance. Educational Psychology Review, 10, 2, 155-175. Wigfield, A., & Eccles, J.S. (2002). Development of Achievement Motivation. Ann Arbor, MI: Academic Press.