Part 10 SC, after recovery, can generate saccades even in the absence of the frontal eye fields, having its own visual inputs Subcortical saccadic circuit Horizontal and vertical/torsional saccadic systems are closely interconnected Share the same cortical areas, cerebellar areas and the superior colliculus Clear anatomical parsing of the horizontal and vertical/torsion saccadic pathways after the SC Due to the anatomical separation of the horizontal and vertical/torsion EBN/IBNs The SC is the element that contains the spatio-temporal coding for saccades SC command is further refined by the local feedback loop and cerebellar innervation Contralateral saccadic commands from the SC, parsed in horizontal and vertical/torsional commands, cross and are transmitted to the ipsilateral horizontal and vertical/torsional LLBNs LLBNs then connect to the ipsilateral MLBs, also called EBNs OPNs act as gates between the two groups with a very strong inhibition on the EBNs Excitatory burst neurons For horizontal saccades: located in the PPRF For vertical/torsion saccades: located in the riMLF The SC consists of 7 layers Dorsal layers are “visual” with the firing related to the appearance of a target in the cell’s receptive field No response to the movement generated by the stimulus Precise retinal map coming directly from the contralateral retina and the contralateral striate cortex (V1) Conjugate rotations of the eyes + head movement are elicited when the head is free and the elicited movement large There is a motor map in the motor (intermediate) layers Each location is associated to a precise motor vector (size and direction of the movement) The rostral pole of the SC has cells that fire tonically during fixation If stimulated, they suppress the execution of an impending saccade Seems to be a reciprocal inhibition between the rostral and the caudal areas of the SC By interrupting, through the OPNs, the flow from the LLBNs to the EBNs they can stop the movement while occurring Massive bilateral SC lesions (after an acute phase of profound deficits) show a remarkable recovery Suggests that probably a combination of FEF and cerebellar adaptations are able to compensate for the loss of the SC Most significant residual effects Hypometric movements (smaller than needed) with higher number of corrective saccades Decrease in the frequency of scanning saccades while looking at a visual scene Complete loss of express saccades during the gap paradigm The SC is responsible for the fast acquisition of the new target in the gap paradigm There is a “decision making” between the visual and motor layers There is a selection process with a “winner take all” between the visual and motor layers with cortical involvement, most likely from the FEF through the basal ganglia The motor layers are not just a carbon-copy of the visual layers Basal Ganglia Substantia nigra pars reticulate (SNpr) Inhibitory connections to the intermediate layers of the SC Main input probably from the frontal and supplementary eye fields through the caudate nucleus Cells have elevated static firing rate; normally slow down significantly in correspondence to the presentation of a visual target SNpr cells two main functions with respect to the saccadic system Exerts tonic inhibition on the SC cells with a pause associated to the behavior they “represent” Inhibition is GABAergic Projection is selective; there is a match between the movement fields of the SNpr cells and the movement field of the SC cells SNpr cells related to that specific saccadic size/direction stop allowing the appropriate location of the SC to fire Play a role in the initiation of voluntary saccadic eye movements Basal ganglia circuit is an active behavioral gateway between cortex and SC Chemical inhibition of these areas decreases the latency of the eye movements with a stronger bias towards reflex-like behavior Their function is to selectively release a neuronal target from tonic inhibition An absence of this release action (a maintenance of an elevated tonic inhibitory action even a movement is desired) is going to interfere with the desired movement, up to its cancellation Loss of dopamine in Parkinson’s generates an abnormal increase in the tonic activity of these structures Makes the generation of the selective pause that allows the movement and the movement itself much harder Caudate nucleus is an integration center of information from FEF, SEF, DLPC, the IML lamina of the thalamus, SNpc Major projections to the SNPr and globus pallidus Pre-saccadic activity shows a strong dependence on memory, ecpectation, attention, and reward Lesion of this area affects mainly memory saccades Intramedullary lamina is believed to be a source for an efferent copy of the eye movement to the cortical eye fields Stimulation of the FEF elicits saccades of a given size and direction, similarly to the SC with a latency of 30-45 ms Movement usually oblique or horizontal Pure vertical saccades achieved only by bilateral stimulation Different subpopulations of cells code the visual stimulus, the movement, or both The activity is not coding the saccadic dynamics or motor error and does not act in normal conditions as a spatio-temporal transformer like the SC Heavily involved in the generation of memory saccades Acute pharmacological lesions cause an immediate oculomotor scotoma (the abolition of any kind of saccades in the area affected by the lesion) Main effects are increased saccadic latency and inaccuracy of visual and memory saccades coded in that area (relatively minor effects) Main feature of the supplementary eye fields is that it seems to code complex (learned) saccadic behaviors, like anti-saccades and sequences of saccades Lesions can inhibit a subject from executing a learned sequence of saccades Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex seems to be involved in the holding of the memory of where a memory saccade is going to be directed; similar effect for anti-saccades Damage is mainly affecting memory saccades and anti-saccades FEF, SEF, and DLPC can be seen as cortical supervisor/decision makers of the saccadic system Capable of generating complex motor behaviors Decisions are transmitted to the basal ganglia and the SC, eventually overwriting (when needed) the reflexive behavior of the SC Parietal areas are NOT directly involved in the generation of saccadic eye movement Responsible for the spatial reorganization for the sensory space when you move Posterior parietal cortex contains cells that respond to visual stimuli and discharge after saccades have been made Also critical in the programming of complex movements, like saccadic gaze (eye + head) shifts toward selected targets, eye, head, and hand coordination that require a spatial frame of reference Acute unilateral posterior parietal lesions cause contralateral inattentions Bilateral lesions are associated with Balint’s syndrome Area LIP is a critical for a number of eye movements Discharges before a saccade and there is coding of eye position Their visual response field shifts in order to anticipate the change in visual frame reference associated with the incoming eye movement They seem to code the info where to go next in a memory saccade Main effect of lesions is a prolonged latency of visually-guided saccades during gap and overlap trials Patient is also unable to respond to double-step tasks NRTP Contains neurons that discharge in relation to a variety of eye movements Lesions cause torsional errors Dorsal vermis The amount and duration of stimulation in the dorsal vermis directly modulates the saccadic dynamics and size Plays a role in the fine tuning of the saccadic response The control is on the pulse part of the saccade (because there is no postsaccadic drift) Unilateral damage causes marked ipsilateral hypometria and mild contralateral hypermetria With longer latencies for ipsilateral saccades and abolition of express and anticipatory saccades Adaptation of saccades to modified visual correspondence is lost after damage to the dorsal vermis Fastigial Nucleus Actual adaptation center Outputs project to practically all subcortical saccadic structures Discharge ~8ms before the movement with a contralateral component and around the end of the saccade with generally an ipsilateral component Gives an additional acceleration drive to the burst neurons for contralateral saccades and an additional late brake during ipsilateral ones Bilateral lesions produce marked hypermetria in all saccades Complete cerebellectomy affects the pulse component of the saccade with hypermetria Also major post-saccadic drift (pulse-step mismatch) Cerebellum can almost completely compensate for the loss of the SC or the FEF (but not both) Saccadic abnormalities Main features quantified in a saccadic response are Gain, defined as the ratio between the size of the movement executed and the required size Duration, defined as the distance between two given velocity thresholds Peak velocity (deg/s) Post-saccadic drifts Latency Presence of saccadic intrusions: presence of involuntary saccades while the subject is fixating Pulse dysmetria Pulse lasting too much (overshoot, hypermetria) Pulse lasting too little (undershoor, hypoemetria) Pulse with a low gain but with an abnormally low peak velocity Pulse with too much gain, with an abnormally high peak velocity Memory saccades, saccades in the dark, saccades during decreased alertness, and during vergence eye movements are often slower than normal visually-elicited saccades Abnormally slow saccades are usually associated with problems in the generation or maintenance of the saccadic pulse in the saccadic bursters More central disorders (involving SC or FEF) also generate slower saccades Gaze-evoked nystagmus is usually a sign of loss of the neural integrator The saccadic system detects the error and generates another saccade and the cycle repeats: nystagmus In the pulse-step mismatch there is still a neural integrator but the integrator gain is altered Two other common aspects are difficulties in generating a saccade or the generation of saccades when not wanted (saccadic intrusions) Significantly longer latencies or no saccades are usually signs of cortical problems Saccadic intrusions mean the inability of a subject to suppress unwanted saccades When the overshoot is very large, the subject is not able to reach the target and engages in an oscillatory pattern called macrosaccadic oscillations Macrosaccadic oscillations are a typical expression of huge hypermetria (often associated with cerebellar disease) Look at graph