Introduction The principle question asked throughout this research is whether upright faces capture visual-spatial attention. Empirical support that it does, will provide an explanation for the Face-Detection Effect {Purcell, 1986 #27;Purcell, 1988 #24;Shelley-Tremblay, 1999 #121}. The Face-Detection Effect refers to the phenomenon whereby upright faces were faster to be localised than its inverted and scrambled counterparts. If spatial attention involuntarily orients towards upright faces, it would explain why participants are quicker to be consciously aware of their presence and their spatial position. The 4 experiments reported in this chapter test the hypothesis that a face in its canonical orientation is more easily detectable than its inverted counterpart because it captures spatial attention. This is achieved by pitting this putative property of upright faces against the attentional demands of a classic visual search task. If faces capture attention, then their mere presence in the periphery of the visual search array should distract and hence, impair visual search performance; as measured by target-detection timings and accuracy. The introductory sections of this chapter will discuss 2 main theoretical themes – namely, attentional capture and the attentional “spotlight” – as researched using the classical experimental design of visual search tasks. First though, the visual search paradigm will be described alongside the two different patterns of searches that can occur depending on the elements used in a search array; that is, parallel and serial visual search. Attentional capture is said to explain parallel searches and serial search patterns can be described with the metaphor of an attentional spotlight. Hence, it will be useful to discuss attentional capture within the context of parallel search performances and the search conditions that induces it. A coverage of stimulus properties known to capture spatial attention will be discussed within the context of the visual search design. Here, parallels will also be made between these stimulus properties and how they might relate to faces. Following this, the commonlyused metaphor of an “attentional spotlight” will be discussed, given its usefulness as a theoretical construct within the attentional literature. As before, its theoretical implications to our experiments will be addressed. Visual search design The visual search paradigm was introduced in the previous chapter as a means of studying spatial attention. It is thus a useful paradigm that has been employed in all the experiments reported in this chapter that set out to study whether the upright faceconfiguration possesses attentional-capture properties. A typical visual search experiment involves requiring participants to search for a pre-specified target among a varying number (set-size) of non-target distractors (e.g. {Treisman, 1985 #162;Treisman, 1988 #122}. As mentioned previously, task-performance on in visual search experiments are measured in terms of reaction-time and accuracy, as a function of set-size. Properties of visual search can be inferred from the slope-function of these performance variables in relation to varying set-size. In this section, visual-search experiments conducted in the past and their consistent main findings will be presented in greater detail. This is to grant the reader a better appreciation of the general experimental design in this chapter. Commonly, search is labelled as highly efficient when the target can be distinguished from all non-targets by the minimum of a single feature. For example, searching for a red target embedded within an array of green distractors. In such cases, target detection times are hardly affected by set-size at all {Wolfe, 1998 #124}. Such efficient searches are sometimes described as parallel searches, whereby the target ‘pops out” by virtue of its salient visual difference to its neighbours. In contrast, search time for targets that can only be distinguished from its distractors by a combination of features, increases linearly with incrementing set-size {Wolfe, 1998 #124}. For example, searching for a red X within an array of red Os and green Xs. Searches that vary in accordance to set-size are termed, serial searches. These terms “parallel” and “serial” describe the theoretical search process for targets; that is, in ‘parallel searches’ every element in the array can be analysed for a single distinguishing feature simultaneously, whereas serial search for a target defined by a conjunction of features can only be achieved by analysing every element in the search array one by one {Treisman, 1988 #122}. There are several explanatory accounts for the qualitative difference between parallel and serial search. The feature-integration theory (FIT) is one such account. In the FIT, visual search is based on the use of topological featural maps, each responding retinotopically to the presence of a specific visual attribute e.g colour, line-orientation, etc. {Treisman, 1982 #68;Treisman, 1988 #81;Treisman, 1998 #65}. A target that is distinguishable from its surrounding on the basis of only one feature, requires the use of only one featural map. Parallel search occurs because by using just one featural map, all the items in the visual scene can be simultaneously assessed for this given target feature and the target containing the critical attribute simply ‘popsout’ on the featural map. In contrast, serial searches occur when a critical target is different from its neighbouring distractors, not by a single feature, but by possessing a unique combination of featural attributes. According to the FIT, the spatial location of the critical target cannot be solved by the use of only one featural map in such instances. The critical target is the item that possesses all the required features and hence, its position has to be topologically congruent across all the corresponding feature maps that make up the target criterion. Treisman and colleagues (1982;1988;1998) claim that to do so, it is necessary to examine each item individually and thus, search can only progress serially. There are several alternative theories to the FIT, that can also account for these qualitative differences in visual search processes (see for example: Guided search model {Wolfe, 1994 #134}; similarity theory {Duncan, 1989 #112}). Nonetheless, they are all in agreement that target-distractor similarity is an critical determinant of task difficulty in visual searches. Parallel searches occur when the target is highly distinguishable from its surrounding distractors and serial searches take place when the critical target shares visual features with its neighbours. In Experiment 1, this factor is used to control for the experimental variable of task-difficulty. Another consistent finding is that the slope of the reaction-time to set-size function of target-absent trials are often twice as steep as target-present trials {Sternberg, 1966 #114;Wolfe, 1994 #134;Wolfe, 1998 #124}. This is particularly true with serial searches and indicates that serial searches are self-terminating. With serial searches, participants search the array by by examining each element individually and in serial progression. With target-present trials, the search ends when the target is detected. This can happen at any point in the course of the serial search depending on the search ‘route’.With target-absent trials, however, the entire visual array has to be fully examined before target absence can be accurately determined. Therefore, responses latencies are expected to be longer on target-absent trials, in comparison to target-present trials. This finding is taken up and used as a controlled variable for task-difficulty in Experiment 2,3 and 4. Though a modest set-up, findings from visual search experiments have over the past 20 years made far-reaching contributions to our understanding of the processes that underlie visual cognition. We intend to use the same paradigm to investigate whether faces, presented in their normal upright orientation, capture attention. Attentional capture An alternative way of describing parallel searches is through attentional capture. The term attentional capture is associated to known visual attributes that induce reflexive and involuntary orienting of attention towards their location visual scene. For example, it is well-established that sudden changes in the visual scene i.e. abrupt visual onsets, have a special propensity to attract attention {Yantis, 1990 #115;Jonides, 1988 #116;Yantis, 1984 #117}. An instance of such a cue might be the sudden dimming or brightening of a particular location in the visual scene. One of the key properties associated with this exogenous form of attentional control is that it is involuntary. That is to say, that attention capture depends entirely on the visual stimulus’ attributes, not on the goals of the observers. Thus, visual search times decreases with abrupt-onset targets and increase with abrupt-onset distractors even when, the location of the abrupt-onset distractor is completely uncorrelated with the location of the target (e.g. {Yantis, 1990 #115}). Attentional orienting of such a nature can be described as stimulus-driven. Relevant to the thread of the discussion so far, has been a recent attribution of attentional capture to feature “singletons” (e.g. {Joseph, 1996 #120;Theeuwes, 1991 #118;Theeuwes, 1992 #119}. Feature “singletons” are described as items that possess at least one featural difference that renders it highly discriminable from its neighbours - such as target items in parallel searches. For instance, Neisser (1964) noted that curved letters e.g. C are more easily detected as a single member of a letter-string of angular letters e.g. K, and vice versa. The measure of target-distractor similarity are often based on the primitive dimensions of colour, line-orientation, motion direction – visual properties that have neurological correlates in the early visual pathway{see {Bravo, 1992 #140;Cheal, 1992 #222} for a list of “features”}. The obvious relation between attentional capture and parallel visual search performance is this common statement that highly distinctive items stand-out in the visual scene such that the amount of visual clutter of irrelevant items don’t matter. Nonetheless, the ultimate test in proving that feature ‘singletons’ can capture attention, lies in the successful demonstration that ‘singletons’can draw attention towards itself, even in the instances when they are completely task irrelevant or, when doing so is highly detrimental to primary task performance. One way of doing so is by having an irrelevant distractor as a feature singleton and to measure its attention-capturing property as an index of how much it prolongs the target detection time. For example, the time it takes to detect an oblique line (/) in an array of many Os (or an O amongsts oblique lines) is lengthened simply by having a couple of uniquely coloured distractors {Pashler, 1988 #123}: Experiment 7). Participants are explicitly instructed to ignore the irrelevant distractors at all times. This simple experiment illustrates spatial attention as a a finite resource that can be automatically recruited, against the participants intentions, by task-irrelevant albeit visually unique and salient distractors. This line of research was furthered by Theeuwes (1991, 1992) when his participants were asked to perform the easy search of a unique shape (e.g . diamond) within an array of distractors (eg. circles). A line was enscribed within each shape, target and distractors, that was either horizontal or vertical and successful detection was noted if participants were able to accurately state the orientation of the line, within the target. On half the trials, all the elements in the array were of the same colour and in the remaining trials, just one distractor was of a different colour. Participants were informed that colour was task-irrelevant. Nonetheless, the presence of the uniquely coloured distractor significantly slowed down target detection time. In a similar experiment, the test display was segmented into 4 distinct spatial quadrants and in the test phase, participants were required to search for the letter L which was embedded in an array of distractor Ts. {Joseph, 1996 #120}. Participants responded by specifying the quadrant that contained the target. Preceding the testphase, however, was a non-informative cue array that contained a “singleton” in either the same quadrant as the one containing the target in the upcoming test-array, or any one of the other non-target quadrants. Participants were told that this preceding cue array was non-informative and did not predict the target position in the test array. Nonetheless, detection times were fastest when the “singleton” in the cue array fell within the same quadrant as the test array. These experiments show that a feature singleton can reflexively orient attention to its location in space, even when this orienting does not benefit task demands. This is seems to be especially true when search in the test trial is easy, such as with parallel searches. The reason for this is because attentional capture is believed to work best when there is no top-down control in place {Theeuwes, 1991 #118}. In the first chapter, it has been suggested that upright-faces could capture attention. Against the backdrop of research concerning attentional capture by featural ‘singletons’, it seems that upright faces could be doing so in the same fashion. In Chapter 1, it has been suggested that the configural pattern of an upright face could have equivalent status as certain primary features e.g. colour, orientation. Like these primary visual features, there exists neural substrates that respond specifically to its presence {Rolls, 1984 #166;Perrett, 1982 #164;Perrett, 1984 #165}. If this is true, observers could be particularly vulnerable to attentional capture by upright faceconfigurations when conducting parallel searches. This hypothesis is put to the test in Experiment 1. Visual Attentional Spotlight The “attentional spotlight” is a convenient metaphor used to describe visual spatial attention {Treisman, 1988 #81}. The spatial extent of this “spotlight” is believed to range from 1° to 20° of visual angle (Eriksen, & Hoffman, 1973; Hughes & Zimba, 1985), depending on the task. Importantly, information processing is quicker and more efficient for items within the “spotlight” and inhibited if found, outside the “spotlight’s” perimeter {Brefczynski, 1999 #74;Posner, 1990 #110;Slotnick, 2002 #80}. In fact, retinotopic mapping can be established between the foci of attended stimuli and the topography of cortical activations, highlighting the spatial aspect of visual attention and justifying the use of this metaphor {Slotnick, 2002 #80;Brefczynski, 1999 #74}. The “spotlight” metaphor is best used to describe the guided control of visual attention in serial search tasks (e.g. {Treisman, 1988 #122}. As mentioned previously, serial search occurs when the target is embedded in a visual array of similar distractors. The more similar the target is to its surrounding distractors, the smaller the area of the “spotlight” and hence, more time will be required for target detection {Neisser, 1964 #113;Duncan, 1989 #112}. Therefore, the serial search process can described as a process in which a small attentional “spotlight” has to examine every single element in the scene one after another in order to find the specified target. This is used to account for the linear relationship between target-detection latencies and the set-size of distractors in serial searches {Wolfe, 1998 #124}. A narrow attentional spotlight, induced by high target-distractor similarity can be postulated to be more vulnerable to attentional capture by irrelevant distractors. Because the spotlight is retinotopic {Brefczynski, 1999 #74}, being attracted away from its task-relevant visual field space would severely impair task performance. The experiments reported in this chapter are designed to operate on this principle. If upright faces do capture spatial attention, it should strongly impair serial search performance of a critical task that is situated in a different visual space location to itself. Attentional capture by upright faces: serial or parallel search mechanism? The face-detection effect has only been tested in an uncluttered scene {Purcell, 1986 #27;Purcell, 1988 #24;Shelley-Tremblay, 1999 #121}. To recap, participants had to detect the presence of an image which could have been an upright, inverted or scrambled face. It was found that performance was best for upright faces in that it required the least amount of presentation time in order to be successfully detected. An offered explanation for this effect, is that upright faces capture spatial attention. However, it is not clear if it might capture spatial attention as a feature singleton might, in a visual array undergoing parallel search (cf. {Theeuwes, 1991 #118;Joseph, 1996 #120;Pashler, 1988 #123}; or by reflexively orienting a very narrow attentional spotlight - induced by target-distractor similarity – away from the critical target. Experiment 1 was designed to clarify this issue by utilising faces as a peripheral distractor with visual search arrays that either induced parallel or serial search. More importantly, the claim that upright faces could capture attention was tested by measuring their influence on visual search performance, on a central primary task. In the following experiments reported, the visual search consisted entirely of of non-face objects i.e. letters of the alphabet. This was contrived such as to avoid the conscious use of face-identification processes that could have inadvertently nullify the ability of upright face-configurations to draw attention to themselves (cf. {Kuehn, 1994 #135;Nothdurft, 1993 #136}. At all times, participants were focused on detecting a pre-specified letter of the alphabet i.e. ‘K’ and were explicitly instructed to ignore the appearance of any peripheral task-irrelevant distractors. The hypothesis predicts that visual search performance would be significantly impaired by the presence of upright faces on both parallel and serial search trials. Upright faces will have a larger impact on parallel searches than serial searches if they behave like feature singletons. The reverse will be true if upright faces orient spatial attention in a general fashion. Experiment 1: Attentional capture by Upright-face distractors on Parallel vs. Serial searches de Gelder and Rouw (2001) argues that the face-detection effect arises from an innate visual predisposition to upright face-configurations {de Gelder, 2001 #33}. This experiment sets out to operationalize this hypothesis within the context of welldocumented search processes (e.g. {Wolfe, 1994 #134}. In this experiment, participants were instructed to search for a pre-specified target in a centrally located visual array. Meanwhile, a peripheral task-irrelevant distractor appeared on half of the trials. The current hypothesis predicted that the taskirrelevant distractor should impair visual search performance by distracting attention away from the central search display, especially if it was an upright faceconfiguration. Thus, the measure to which upright faces capture spatial attention was considered to be the extent to which an accompanying task-irrelevant upright faceconfiguration impaired search performance by distracting spatial attention away from the task-relevant search array. Half of the task-irrelevant distractors that appeared were inverted face-images which acted as controls. The face-detection system that brings about involuntary orienting of attention is believed to be respond specifically to coarse-grained image information found in a face i.e. configural pattern of a face {Purcell, 1988 #24;de Gelder, 2001 #33}. Thus, the inverted-face images were not expected to draw attention away from the critical search array in the same way as an upright face image was predicted to. In addition, the influence of upright face-configurations on two different types of search processes (parallel and serial) were considered. This is to provide a qualitative description of the manner by which upright face-configurations capture attention. As explained in the preceding section, it is yet unclear whether an uprightface configuration capture attention by visual-salience, and “popping-out” of a cluttered visual scene as does a feature-singleton when parallel search processes are in action; or whether it does so by drawing a narrow attentional ‘spotlight’, that is deployed during serial searches, towards itself. Parallel searches are deemed easy and induced by low visual similarity between the target and its non-targets in the visual array. The converse is true for serial searches that are believed to be brought about by high visual similarity between targets and non-targets {Duncan, 1989 #112;Wolfe, 1994 #134}. Pitting the presence of an upright face-configuration against either of these search processes will clarify this issue. Design On each trial, participants were required to search a central visual search array for a pre-specified target. There were two levels of task-difficulty that was controlled by target-similarity between target and non-target items in the search array (see Materials section for details). The experiment consisted of 6 blocks of trials, presented in random order. Each block contained 32 trials that were equally divided for taskdifficulty. Altogether, there were 192 experimental trials on which half were presented with an accompanying task-irrelevant distractor and half without. On the trials with a task-irrelevant distractor, half the distractors were upright faces and the remaining half were inverted faces.The experiment can thus be described as a 2 (task difficulty: easy vs. difficult) x 3 (distractor-type: no distractor vs. face-distractor vs. nonface-distractor) repeated-measures design. Two dependent measures were recorded; namely, reaction time and performance accuracy on the primary visual search task. Materials Each trial was made up of a visual search display that half of the time, was accompanied by a peripheral task-irrelevant distractor. This section covers how both elements of the trial was created and paired together. To avoid confusion, the terms target and non-target will be used exclusively to refer to items of the visual search array, while the term distractor refers to task-irrelevant distractors that are either upright or inverted faces. Visual search display: Two sets of four alphabetic letters each made up the visual search array for the primary task. The two sets of letters differed from each other with regards to their visual-similarity to the target letter; that is, the letter ‘K’ which can be described as an angular letter {Neisser, 1964 #113}. Thus, one set of letters consisted solely of angular letters (target-similar: X, Y, N, V), and the other set of highly dissimilar circular letters (target-dissimilar: O, C, D, G).Each visual search array was a 2 x 4 arrangement; or 2 rows of letter-strings, each comprising 4 letters. 24 unique letter-strings were rendered for each set by applying a full permutation to the corresponding items. Following this, letter-strings containing the target item were derived by a process of simply replacing an element of a letter-string with the target letter ‘K’, By repeating this process for each non-target letter, across all the 4 possible positions, 96 unique letter-strings containing the target letter ‘K’ were derivable for each set. Each of these target letter-strings were randomly paired with a non-target letter-string made up of items from the same set, resulting in a total of 192 visual search arrays. That is to say, the entire search array would have consisted entirely of either target-similar or target-dissimilar non-targets. The visual search arrays were counter-balanced for position of the target-letter string about a central fixation cross. In addition, the position of the target letter-string within the array was also counter-balanced across the target’s position within the letter string, as well as it’s position relative to the distractor – if present. Task-Irrelevant Distractors (Upright and Inverted Faces): Still images of 24 different faces were selected from a bank of images taken of police trainees supplied by the Home Office [see Bruce et al. (1999) for more details on this set of materials]. Each image was edited using Adobe Photoshop ver. 6.0, to only reveal the face, as shown in Fig. 2.1a. The faces are looking straight ahead, with a neutral face expression. Corresponding inverted face-distractors were created by a 180° rotation of each of the 24 images, using Adobe Photoshop ver. 6.0. An example is given in Fig. 2.1b. Care is taken that each upright distractor and its inverted counterpart was equivalent with regards to brightness, contrast, size and featural detail. Fig 2.1: Examples of the task-irrelevant distractors created from photorealistic face-images: a) upright face-type distractor; b) inverted face-type distractor. Each distractor image was used twice in the experiment, a total of 96 distractors. Each image appeared in 1 of 6 specified positions, these 6 positions were fixed evenly-spaced points on an imaginary circle around the visual search array (see Fig. 2.2c). The image position was specified by a list and a latin-square was conducted such that each image was presented in all of the given 6 positions, across all the participants. Apparatus: The experiment was controlled by an experimental software package Psyscope v.1.12 on a Macintosh G4. The display was presented on a 17’ flat-screen monitor that was distanced 57cm from the head rest. A fixed head-rest was aligned to the fixation cross of the experiment and ensured that a consistent visual display size was maintained throughout the experiment. At 57cm, 1cm on the screen subtends approximately 1º visual angle on the retina. Participants 30 first-year Psychology undergraduates (age range: 18-19 years) participated in this study for course credits. All participants reported normal or corrected-tonormal vision. Procedure: On each experimental trial, participants performed a two-alternative forcedchoice (2AFC) letter detection task that involved searching a 2 x 4 array - containing 8 letters of the alphabet - for a target letter i.e. ‘K’. Each trial began with a fixation cross that lasted for 1000 msec. Following this, the critical search array was presented. The array was in the form of two letter-strings, one situated above the fixation cross and the other, below it. Only one letter-string contained the target letter ‘K’. This critical display lasted for only 200msecs and the participants’ task was to identify the letter-string containing the target with key-press response: ‘Y’ - letter-string above the fixation-cross; ‘B’ - below fixation-cross. The timings of each experimental trial are illustrated in Fig. 1 and the inter-trial interval was 500ms. Participants were required to keep both index fingers on the response keys throughout the experiment and handedness-dominance was counter-balanced across participants. Participants’ were encouraged to respond as quickly as possible, without making errors. Corrective feedback was also provided, to discourage accuracy-speed trade off. The mean reaction time (RT) and accuracy of their responses were treated as measures of their primary task performance. On half of the trials, this array was accompanied by a distractor (upright or inverted face) that was positioned in any of 6 possible positions - equidistant from and evenly spaced around the letter array. Figure 2.2 provides an example of a trial timeline. The inter-trial interval was 500ms. Fig. 2.2: a) Example of a typical visual search trial in Experimental 1 and 2. In this example, critical search array consists of target letter ‘K’ amongst visually-similar (angular) letters. Inset b) provides an example of an easy search (Experiment 1); c) possible positions of the task-irrelevant distractors. The task-irrelevant distractors subtended a circle of 4º radius and were presented in any of six positions, evenly spaced along the circumference of a circle centred 9º about the fixation cross. Letter strings were positioned 0.5º above and below the fixation cross. Each unit in the search array subtended 0.2º x 0.2º and the dimensions of the entire search array were 1.3º x 1.1º. Results: Two measures were taken for task performance on the letter-detection task; namely, accuracy and reaction time. A 2 (Task Difficulty) x 3 (Distractor-type) ANOVA was conducted on each set of dependent variable. For measures of accuracy, there were main effects of taskdifficulty (F(1,29)=184.4, p<0.001) but not of distractor-type (F(2,58)=0.096, p=0.91). Graph 2.1 gives a summary of the results across conditions. The manipulated variable of task-difficulty has been proven to be effective. Searches for the target found amongst visually-similar non-targets were less accurate than when visually dissimilar non-targets made up the array instead. In addition, there is no significant interaction was between task-difficulty and distractor-type (F(2,58)=0.21, p=0.815). This further indicates that the distractor-type has no differential effect on either type of search process. Performance accuracy (%) 100 95 90 85 80 75 easy difficult Task-difficulty levels absent inverted face upright face Graph 2.1: Comparison of accuracy scores in task-difficulty and serial order conditions across distractor-types. Error bars represent standard errors. The same pattern of results was repeated for the measure of reaction-time. Again, main effect of task-difficulty was demonstrated, but not for distractor-type. Response latency is lower for searches of the target in an array of visually-dissimilar non-targets compared to visually-similar non-targets (F(1,29)=47.5, p<0.001). As with the measure of accuracy, the type of distractors do not influence performance on the primary as measured by reaction time (F(2,58)=0.266, p=0.77). Again, there is no interaction between the variable of task-difficulty and distractor-type, indicating that the distractors do not affect either type of searches any differently from each other. A summary of the RT means are presented in Table 2.1. Easy-task Mean Reaction Time (msec) Difficult-task No distractor Inverted-face Upright-face No Inverted-face distractor distractor distractor distractor Upright-face distractor 567.3 557.4 564.2 650.0 659.9 670.9 18.7 17.9 22.2 26.4 25.7 Standard Error. 27.9 Table 2.1: Summary of mean RTs (msecs) and standard errors across experimental conditions To analyse these results in greater detail, a post-hoc analysis was conducted that collapsed the trials that contained a distractor, regardless of whether it was an upright or inverted face. Thus a 2 (task difficulty: easy vs. difficult) x 2(distractortype: absent vs. present) repeated measures Anova was conducted for this purpose. The analysis reiterated the above findings, with significant main effects of taskdifficulty for both accuracy measures (F(1,29)=182.6, p<0.001) and reaction times (F(1,29)=32.8, p<0.001). However, the mere presence of distractors failed to have any significant influence on the primary task performance at all, across both measures of accuracy (F(1,29)=0.190, p=0.67) and reaction time (F(1,29)=0.12, p=0.73). Discussion: This experiment was designed to investigate the cognitive mechanism that underlie the face-detection effect reported in Purcell and Stewart (1988). The claim is that participants are quicker to be conscious of the position of an upright facial configural pattern than control images containing the same visual elements, but in an uncommon orientation i.e. inverted faces. This is even when the images were presented so briefly that image-identification was kept at chance-level. To explain these results, some authors have suggested that visual-spatial attention might be selective for familiar visual configural patterns, in particular faces (Purcell & Stewart, 1988; de Gelder & Rouw, 2000). When processing a complex visual scene, an upright facial configuration could play the role of a highly salient cue, one that guides the “attentional spotlight” such that extra processing is involuntarily dedicated to its visual proximity. If this is so, and if visual-spatial attention can be assumed to be a limited resource, the simultaneous presentation of upright faces should effectively distract and hence, impair performance on the letter-search task in this experiment. However, our results fail to conform to this given prediction. In Experiment 1, irrespective of whether the search array was accompanied by either an upright or inverted face-distractor, performance on the primary task was the same, on both measures of accuracy and RT. In fact, task performance was completely unaffected by the presence of a distractor. The complete inefficacy of the distractors in impairing primary task performance, might provide insight into possible task confounds that might be inherent in the experimental design. The pattern of our results confirm that the letter-detection task worked as expected and employed the same attentional mechanisms as that reported in classic visual search experiments. Targets were consistently harder to detect in an array that consisted of visually similar non-targets and were more noticeable when placed amongst dissimilar non-targets {Duncan, 1989 #112;Neisser, 1964 #113}. Furthermore, these results suggest that the presence of face-distractors – both upright and inverted - do not influence the search-process at all; that is, by capturing visualspatial attention. Nonetheless, these results are arguably inconclusive as they could have been confounded by a task-solving strategy brought about by the given task-response. In this experiment, participants were asked to make a 2-force choice decision to indicate the target position; that is, above/below the fixation cross. Post-experimental discussions with the participants revealed that on occasion, the task was solved by inference. When failing to detect a target in a letter-string, participants sometimes inferred that it was to be found in the other. Thus some participants adopted the strategy of monitoring only one letter-string. Such a strategy could have diminished the influence of distractors on the visual search task, by reducing the search area to such an extent that it precluded the distractors entirely. In addition, the results suggest that the presence of distractors, failed to impair search performance completely. This is highly unlikely and raises the possibility that even in the instance that distractors were effective in impairing search performance, participants could have returned an accurate response by making an inferential decision. Another matter to note, is the high performance on searches for the target in a visually-dissimilar array i.e. easy-task. The range of accuracy scores were 94.1% to 94.9%. The results in the task-easy condition could be inconclusive due to ceiling effects. Also, it could also mean that the task-easy condition is so effortless that it is able to offset any detrimental influence that might be exerted upon it by an upright face. The next experiment is designed to take these likely confounds into account. In the following experiment, modifications were adopted to: i) prevent inferential responses, ii) reduce the overall visual display size to fall within foveal region and, iii) to minimise the possibility of a ceiling effect in primary task-performance. Experiment 2: Presence Detection task Experiment 1 failed to confirm the primary hypothesis that upright faces “capture” visual-spatial attention. Nonetheless, there existed task confounds in the previous experiment that prevented us from making any conclusive findings. Experiment 2 is a modification of the previous one, that attempts to resolve these confounding issues. The basic design of the experimental trial and timings remained the same as Experiment 1, with the following modifications. Firstly, the primary task has been modified to prevent participants from making inferential responses. For Experiment 2, participants were required to decide whether or not the target was present or absent on each trial, rather than indicate the letter-string that contained it. Therefore, unlike Experiment 1, only half of the trials in this experiment contained a target. The benefit of this modification was that participants could no longer deliver an accurate response based on inference. The only way to be truly confident about target-presence was to actually find the target. As a second modification, dissimilar non-targets are not used at all. Instead, the all the visual search arrays used in this experiment consisted only of visually similar non-targets e.g. ‘N’; that is, angular letters similar to the target letter ‘K’. As noted in the discussion section of Experiment 1, performance on the search arrays using visually dissimilar non-targets e.g. ‘C’, were exceedingly good. This resulted in high performance accuracy that hinted of ceiling effects. To avoid this, only the visual search arrays consisting of visually similar non-targets are used in Experiment 2. This also served the advantage of reducing the number of trials to a manageable level. This is because whereas previously, all trials contained a target-present search array, changing the task-response required doubling the number of trials to accomodate both target-present and target-absent trials. With the removal of trials that contained search arrays of visually dissimilar non-targets, the independent variable of task-difficulty had to be replaced by targetpresence instead. Trials that contain a target are assumed to be easier than trials that do not because target-absent trials are believed to require twice the amount of time needed by target-present trials for successful performance {Sternberg, 1966 #114;Wolfe, 1994 #134;Wolfe, 1998 #124}. This can only be explained by considering that one can only be confident of a target’s absence by examining every element in the search array. This is not true of target-present trials. Search for targets embedded amongst visual similar non-targets is believed to be serial and selfterminating on target-present trials. Hence, the participant is, on average, likely to detect the target halfway through a thorough scanning of the search array, on a targetpresent trial. With limited viewing times imposed upon the critical search array, this consistent visual search finding should be reflected by lower accuracy scores and slower reaction timings on target-absent trials. The removal of trials with dissimilar non-target means that we are now unable to test the influence of upright faces on parallel searches for a singleton. However, recent evidence indicates that serial search performance starts to resemble parallel searches with practice {Sireteanu, 1995 #138}. That is to say, that search performance is efficient and no longer influenced by an increase in the aray-size . This might be due to an enlargement of the “spotlight’s” area; or a more effective distribution of attention across the test display. In fact, certain authors suggest that serial and parallel searches are not distinct processes, but instead fall at two ends of a continuum that is modulated by learning {Bravo, 1992 #140;Townsend, 1990 #139}. To look into this, an additional variable of serial order is considered in this experiment, by comparing performance on the first 3 blocks of trials against the last 3 blocks. Given that inference is no longer a valid strategy in this experiment, any serial-order effect might be taken to support this view of a mutable transition from serial to parallel search performance that takes place with practice. If so, the second half of the experiment could be treated as the equivalent of our parallel search trials and we should expect an interaction between serial-order and distractor-type, if upright faces influence search performance of parallel searches but not serial search (or vice versa). Finally, to ensure that the presence of task-irrelevant distractors were noticed, the retinal size of the visual display was reduced in Experiment 2. This was effected by increasing the distance between the display and headrest to 96cm. By doing so, the search array and distractor now fell within the foveal region, about the fixation cross; that is, approximately 5.2° by 5.2°. Hence, the viewing conditions for the critical search array and task-irrelevant distractors were equivalent. As before, the primary hypothesis is that an upright face-image is a more effective distractor than its inverted counterpart. Therefore, the main prediction is that trials containing the former - and not the latter - will capture visual-spatial attention and hamper its application to the primary task. This should be reflected by lower accuracy rates and slower RTs in primary task performance. In addition, the first 3 blocks of experimental trials are expected to reflect serial search processes and the subsequent 3 blocks as parallel processes (if practice effects can be demonstrated). By examining the interaction between serial-order and distractor-type, we might be better able to pin-point the type of visual search that is more vulnerable to an upright face’s distractability. Design: This experiment has a 2 (serial order: first 3 blocks vs. last 3 blocks) x 2 (target presence: present vs. absent) x 3 (distractor-type: no distractor vs. facedistractor vs. nonface-distractor) repeated-measures design. Similar to Experiment 1, there are 6 blocks of 32 trials each, presented in random order and equally representative of target present and absent trials within each block; there is a total of 192 experimental trials. Half are presented with an accompanying task-irrelevant distractor and half without. On the trials with a distractor, half the distractors are upright faces and half are inverted faces. The main dependent variables are measures of primary task performance; namely, reaction timings and response accuracy. Participants: 35 first-year undergraduates with normal or corrected-to-normal vision took part in this experiment for course credits. The age-range is 18 to 20 years old. Materials: The materials in this experiment were the same as Experiment 1, with the only changes being the complete replacement of search-arrays that contained target dissimilar non-targets e.g. ‘C’, with target-absent search arrays comprising target similar non-targets. This was to facilitate for the condition of target-presence. Targetabsent visual search arrays were created by the following procedure. The search-arrays comprising non-targets similar to the target letter ‘K’ were duplicated. For this duplicate set, the target-letter was replaced by a non-target letter from the set of visually-similar letters - that was not already present in the letter-string. Thus, there were 192 search-arrays in total, half containing a target-letter ‘K’ and half without the target. Apparatus: The apparatus set-up was the same as in Experiment 1, with the exception of increasing the distance between the video display unit and the fixed chin-rest to 96cm. This modification meant that 1cm on the screen subtended approximately 0.59° on the retina. Procedure: The trial proceedings and timings remained unchanged from Experiment 1. However, instead of determining the target’s spatial position, participants were now instructed to decide if the target is present or absent. Participants were tested on a 2AFC procedure as before, and to respond with by pressing one of 2 pre-designated keys on a standard keyboard. In each trial, participants began by fixating on the central cross that was followed by the presentation of the critical search array and a possible distractor in one of 6 positions. Participants then scanned the search array for target presence and made an appropriate keypress as soon as possible. As before, participants were encouraged to make accurate and fast responses The test stimuli was briefly presented (200ms) and was followed by a central fixation cross that stayed on until a keypress response is made. Again, corrective feedback was provided. The distractors subtended a circle of 2.375º radius and were presented in any of six positions, evenly spaced along the circumference of a circle centred 5.3º about the fixation cross. The schematic arrangement was the same as that used in Experiment 1 (see Fig 2.2c). Letter strings were positioned 0.3º above and below the fixation cross. Each unit in the search array subtended 0.12º x 0.12º and the dimensions of the entire search array were 0.77º x 0.65º. The whole display fell within a circular area of diameter 5.3°, approximately the size of the foveal region. Results The dependent variables were i) response accuracy and ii) reaction timings of correct responses on the primary task. A 2 (Serial order) x 2 (Target presence) x 3 (Distractor-type) ANOVA was conducted on each set of dependent variable – accuracy and detection RT. The analysis of accuracy scores revealed statistically significant main effects for the independent variables of serial order (F(1,34)=4.58, p=0.04) and target-presence (F(1,34)=57.4, p<0.001). Performance improved with practice, resulting in more accurate responses in the latter three blocks of the experiment compared to the first three. Also, under limited viewing conditions, participants performed better with target-present rather than target-absent search arrays. Contrary to the hypothesis, there was no main effect for the variable of distractor-type (F(2,68)=2.14, p=0.13). Performance on the primary task was not substantially influenced by the condition of distractor-type. In addition, there were no significant interactions, particularly between serial-order and distractor-type (F(2,68)=1.03, p=0.36), that could have indicated a differential influence of the orientation of distractor-type on varying levels of search expertise. A repeat analysis with reaction-time measures, revealed a similar pattern of results. Statistically significant main effects were reflected for serial order (F(1,34)=39.15, p<0.001) and target-presence (F(1,34)=98.62, p<0.001), but not for distractor-type (F(2,68)=0.49, p=0.61). Again, this indicated faster responses on target-present trials and improved performance with practice. Contrary to experimental predictions, the orientation of face-distractors failed to influence primary task performance. There was a significant interaction between the independent variables of serial-order and target-presence (F(1,34)=4.618, p=0.39). Close examination of the mean reaction times (see Appendix A: Experiment 2) reveals that while response latencies were generally shorter for target-present than target-absent trials, this difference was larger for the first three blocks of trials compared to the later three blocks. This interaction demonstrates the effect that practice might exert on search performance, the transition of serial-like to parallel-like search efficiency. The summary of mean accuracy scores and RTs of the main effects are shown in Table 2.2. To summarise, responses made on the trials contained within the final 3 blocks were faster and more accurate, compared to the first 3. This was also true for trials that contained the target within their search-arrays, as opposed to those that did not. Both accuracy and RTs do not appear to vary across trials that are distinguished by the condition of distractor-type that was predicted to impair primary task performance. Serial-order Mean acc (%) Std error Mean RT (msec) Std error Target Distractor-type First Second Present Absent Absent Inverted face Upright face 65.0 1.7 68.2 1.9 77.3 1.6 55.9 2.6 68.2 1.6 66.4 1.7 65.3 2.1 827 30.0 730 21.8 688 18.9 869 30.8 776 23.2 776 24.8 784 25.0 Table. 2.1: Summary of mean accuracy and reaction times of responses and respective standard errors of visual search task, across independent variables of serial-order, target-presence and distractor type. Following this, the data for distractor-type was collapsed across upright and inverted faces distractor-types. This was to investigate if the presence of a distractor had an influence on the primary task performance regardless of type. A 2 (serial order 1 vs. 2) x 2 (target absent vs present) x 2 (distractor absent vs present) repeated measures ANOVA was conducted for the measure of response accuracy. This analysis showed significant main effects for serial-order (F(1,34)=6.41, p=0.016), target presence (F(1,34)=59.87, p<0.001) and finally, distractor presence (F(1,34)=4.9, p=0.034). No significant interactions were revealed. The same analysis conducted for the measure of RTs only showed main effects for serial-order (F(1,34)=43.36, p<0.001) and target presence (F(1,34)=97.97, p<0.001). There was a significant interaction between serial-order and target-presence (F(1,34)=4.31, p=0.046). A summary of the mean accuracy scores and RTs is presented in table 2.2. Altogether, primary task performance is faster and more accurate in the first 3 blocks compared to the final 3. This is also true for when the target is present in the search array, compared to when it is absent. In addition, the interaction of serial-order and target-presence for RT reveals practise effects, that participants find it easier to cope with target-absent trials with practise. The presence of a distractor, regardless of whether it is an upright or inverted face image, significantly reduces response accuracy but does not slow down performance on the primary task. Serial-order Mean acc (%) std. error Mean RT (msec) std. error Target Distractor-type First 65.2 1.7 Second 68.9 1.9 Present 77.6 1.6 Absent 56.5 2.5 Absent 68.3 1.6 Present 65.8 1.8 824.4 27.2 731.5 21.8 685.9 18.9 870.1 30.5 776.1 23.2 779.8 24.4 Table 2.2: Summary table of mean response accuracy and RTs, and respective standard errors, for the main independent variables of i)target-presence and ii)distractor-type. Discussion: The experimental design of Experiment 1 contained several confounds that were addressed in this second experiment. Firstly, the responses made in this experiment could only be accurately derived from conducting a visual search of the letter array and not by inference. This eliminates the use of an internal strategy to compensate for difficulties caused by the experimental limitations imposed in visual search; such as, limited visual exposure or distractor presence. In addition, the whole trial display is reduced in size, specifically to allow it to fall within the foveal region. Results from experiment 1 failed to confirm the hypothesis that upright faces involuntarily captures selective attention, hence severely limiting its successful application to the primary task; that is, a purposeful visual search of a letter array. The purpose of Experiment 2 was to resolve the aforesaid confounds and thus, clarify the results of Experiment 1. The results obtained here are similar to those of Experiment 1. Like Experiment 1, performance on the visual search task was not influenced by the type of distractor used. Upright faces were not more effective than inverted faces in distracting spatial attention away from the primary task. Nonetheless, it can be seen from this experiment that both distractors can exert a detrimental effect on visual search accuracy when they fall in the same foveal region. Thus, the noneffectiveness of distractors in the previous experiment might be attributed to the greater distance between themselves and the search array. The pattern of performance on the visual search task – independent of distractor-type - is in accordance with our expectations. With letter arrays that do not contain the target, responses are less accurate and slower compared to those that do contain a target. The analogy of a “attentional spotlight” is useful in explaining this. To solve the visual search task, each element of the array has to be scanned in a serial fashion, especially when the target is amongst like elements {Duncan, 1989 #112}. Search is expected to be faster for arrays that contain the target compared to those that do not, because the only way to return an accurate response for the latter is by assessing every element in the array. Searches on the former are terminated upon target detection and there is no imperative to analyse the whole array in such instances. It was originally thought that if upright faces captured spatial attention and hence, skew the path of this attentional spotlight, performance on a serial search task would be severely impaired on trials that had a simultaneous presentation of upright faces. This hypothesis cannot be confirmed by the results of Experiment 2. Thus, the results of this experiment confirm that a serial search is taking place. In addition, the interaction between serial-order and target-presence further suggests that practice might have improve search efficiency, such that serial search performance in the first 3 blocks assumes the efficiency of parallel searches with practice, in the last 3 blocks. Hence, our results strongly suggests that upright faces do not capture attention used in serial searches any more than inverted face controls; and that it is unlikely to capture attention used in parallel searches either. Therefore, the face-detection effect cannot be explained by a visual-spatial attentional system that is selective for upright faces. For further confirmation of this finding, two pilot experiments were subsequently conducted that allowed for slight modifications to the experimental design of Experiment 2. Namely: Experiment 3 introduces a backward visual mask that immediately follows the critical stimuli and Experiment 4 reduces the distance between distractor and letter array. The original investigations of the face-detection effect used a backward visual mask to disrupt any visual-processing of the stimuli after it was removed from the visual display {Purcell, 1986 #27;Purcell, 1988 #24;Rolls, 1994 #7}. The experiments reported here do not do so. Experiment 3 tests the possibility of face-detection effect as a construct of this particular paradigm. Unlike the previous experiment, distractors in Experiment 2 are significantly effective in impairing target search performance. Experiment 4 is conducted to assess the measure of an upright-face’s ability to capture attention, as a function of its proximity to the critical search array. In a similar task, Jenkins and Burton (2001) demonstrated covert face-learning effects when the upright face occupied the same visual space as the letter array. Attentional capture properties of an upright face could be bounded by foveal proximity to the critical array and only exhibit such properties within a certain radius of the relevant stimuli. Experiment 3 and 4 Two experiments are reported here, with only slight modifications on the design of Experiment 2. These two experiments served the purpose of replicating the results obtained in Experiment 2 The slight modifications are to the display stimuli and were introduced to increase the visual salience of the task-irrelevant distractor stimuli. The condition of serial-order has been removed from further analyses because the main focus is to replicate the finding that the orientation of face-distractors do not influence serial search performance and by inference, do not capture spatial attention. In Experiment 3, backward visual masking was introduced (cf. Purcell & Stewart, 1986;1988) and Experiment 4 brought the distractors even closer to the critical search array. Experiment 3 followed the same design as Experiment 2, except that a visual mask follows the critical search array instead of the standard central fixation cross used in both Experiment 1 and 2 (see Fig. 2). This visual-pattern of random noise replaces the test display and stays on until a keypress response is made. This rectangular pattern measures 12.77° x 13.3°. Fig. 2: Time-line of experimental trial in Expt 3 with backward visual masking In Experiment 4 the linear distance between distractors and fixation cross was reduced from 5.3° to 3°. This is the only difference between Experiment 3 and 4. All the experimental trials and test conditions were maintained. Participants There were 8 undergraduates with normal or corrected-to-normal vision for each experiment. The age-range was 19-22 years. Participants were awarded course credits for their participation. Materials and Apparatus All the materials and apparatus used for these 2 experiments were the same as Experiment 2. The pattern of random noise used as a backward-visual mask was created by applying an effects filter to a blank template, using Adobe Photoshop v. 6.5. Procedures Participants were provided with the same task instructions as Experiment 2. The experimental script was identical with the exceptions of the aforementioned differences. Experiment 3: Backward-visual Masking Results A 2 (target-presence: present vs. absent) x 3 (distractor-type: absent vs. inverted face vs. upright face) repeated-measures ANOVA was conducted on the measures of response accuracy and RTs. Again, the analysis for response accuracy reflected a significant main effect of target-presence (F(1,7)=34.57, p=0.001) but not for distractor-type (F(2,14)=0.38, p=0.691). The same was shown for reaction-time measures, with a significant main effect of target-presence (F(1,7)=57.6, p<0.001), though not for distractor-type (F(2,14)=0.44, p=0.66). Finally, there were no significant 2-way interactions to report. Table 2.3 summarises the mean accuracy and RTs of the participants responses. No distractor Present 73.7 Mean acc (%) 3.6 std. errors 595.3 Mean RT (msec) 55.1 std. errors Absent 44.5 4.5 812.8 96.5 Inverted Upright face-distractor face-distractor Present Absent Present Absent 73.4 44.3 74.5 45.8 4.7 6.4 4.2 5.5 643.4 735.8 613.4 831.9 75.6 106.6 66.5 133.8 Table 2.3: Summary table of mean response accuracy and RTs as well as corresponding standard errors, for the main independent variables of i) target-presence and ii) distractortype. Performance was faster and more accurate on search trials that contained a target. Despite the introduced modifications, the orientation of face distractor-type continued to effect no influence on primary task performance. The only statistically significant interaction between target-presence and distractor-type was for RTs (F(2,14)=4.39, p=0.03). This interaction is illustrated in the line-graph 2.2. However, this interaction did not address the issues presented in our hypothesis. Furthermore, this interaction was not replicated in Experiment 4 and thus, was not be subject to further analysis. target-present 850 target-absent 800 750 700 650 600 550 500 absent inverted-face upright-face Graph 2.2: Comparison of mean RTs across distractor-types for search performance on arrays with target-present and target-absent. Experiment 4: Reduced distance between distractors and the critical array Results: A 2 (target-presence: present vs. absent) x 3 (distractor-type: absent vs. inverted face vs. upright face) ANOVA was conducted on measures of response accuracy and RTs. On the measure of response accuracy, the independent variable of targetpresence was the only significant main effect (F(1,7)=31.57, p=0.001). The main effect of distractor-type was not statistically significant (F(2,14)=0.059, p=0.943). The same analysis of RTs also showed a significant main effect for target-presence (F(1,7)=10.3, p=0.015) but not for distractor-type (F(2,14)=0.68, p=0.52). Table 2.4 presents a summary of the mean response accuracy and reaction-times. No distractor Present 72.9 Mean acc (%) 2.8 std. errors Mean RT (msec) 487.5 39.7 std. errors Absent 47.7 4.7 636.6 46.5 Inverted Upright face-distractor face-distractor Present Absent Present Absent 77.1 46.9 73.4 49.5 3.5 5.7 2.9 5.2 513.1 637.3 484.3 630.9 38.4 37.7 34.8 55.8 Fig. 2.4: Summary table of mean response accuracy and RTs, and respective standard errors, for the main independent variables of i) target-presence and ii) distractor-type. Therefore, primary task performance was faster and more accurate on search arrays that contained a target. Search times and accuracy were independent of distractor-type and did not vary across this condition. Discussion: The results obtained in these two reported pilots were similar to those already obtained in Experiment 2. As with Experiment 2, there was a significant main effect of target-presence. Despite the modifications introduced, it cannot be shown that the orientation of the face-distractors was an influential factor on the attentional mechanisms that underpinned serial visual search. Even by increasing the prominence of these distractors, by introducing a backward visual mask and drawing the distractors closer to the critical search array failed to induce the expected results. We conclude that upright faces were not more effective than inverted faces in distracting spatial attention away from a letter-search task. Experiments 3 and 4 demonstrated that the effect of target-presence is extremely robust, even with small samples of participants. This supports the assumption that spatial attention is a limited resource as well as the analogy of an attentional spotlight that scans each individual element of an array until the critical target is found {Wolfe, 1998 #124}. Nonetheles, the presence of task-irrelevant upright faces in a proximal area did not appear to selectively attract this attentional “spotlight” away from the critical stimuli and hamper with this conscious search process. Overall Discussion The main motivation behind the experiments reported here has been to understand the face-detection effect in terms of early visual attention. In their dualroute model of face-processing, de Gelder and Rouw (2001) suggested that the facedetection effect could be better described as an epiphenomenon of a primitive ability to automatically orient visual attention towards upright face-configurations. In this chapter, visual attention has been defined teleologically; that is, in terms of how it might be utilised in visual searches. Based on de Gelder and Rouw’s (2001) model, the general prediction has been that - depending on the upright face-configuration’s spatial proximity to the critical search target – the search process could be facilitated or impeded by the upright face’s tendency to draw visual attention towards its location in the visual scene. Although past studies have generally been unsuccessful in using the visual search paradigm to support this hypothesis, it has been argued earlier in the chapter that this failure could have resulted from their use of the upright face-configuration itself as the search target {Nothdurft, 1993 #136;Brown, 1997 #132;Kuehn, 1994 #135}. In doing so, their participants were inevitably recruiting face-identification processes in their visual searches, that could have overshadowed the more primitive and involuntary processes of face-detection. So far, the only experiment based on the visual search paradigm that has provided support for this hypothesis, required participants to search for a non-face target i.e. line-orientation {Gorea, 1990 #21}. The aims in running the experiments here has been three-fold. Firstly, there is a strong motivation to establish a relevance between the face-detection effect and goal-directed behaviour. Given the claims about face-detection as an automatic and involuntary process, these experiments investigate whether the presence of upright faces could have an influence on goal-directed behaviour that has been designed to be as irrelevant to faces as possible; namely, visual search for a particular letter of the alphabet. Next, face-detection is theorised to be a primitive process that is distinctive from face-identification. Thus, efforts have been taken to remove any conscious involvement of face-identification processes in the experimental tasks and to see whether the face-detection effect might continue to exhibit itself nonetheless, by capturing visual attention. Finally, these experiments seek to define the face-detection process in terms of visual attention, which is a better-understood and well-defined cognitive process. This will allow better integration of the face-detection effect into our current understanding of the visual system and face-specific processes. Under certain conditions, the search process can be metaphorically likened to the use of an attentional ‘spotlight’ that sweeps through a visual scene, inspecting each item in turn, it reaches the spatial location of the critical target. Such a search can be described as serial and self-terminating {Wolfe, 1994 #134;Wolfe, 1998 #124}. As already mentioned, certain feature singletons e.g. colour are known to capture spatial attention (e.g.{Theeuwes, 1991 #118} and it is on this basis that the same claim has been put forward for upright face-configurations. If indeed, the upright faceconfiguration captures visual attention and if attention is like a ‘spotlight’ that covers a finite spatial area, we ought have expected the accompanying presence of an upright face-configuration to impair search performance on the critical display of letterstimuli. In any case, the presence of a distractor, whether upright or inverted, was expected to detract from optimum visual search performance, simply by being an extra non-target element in the visual scene. However, the general findings of these experiments did not support this prediction. Whilst the presence of a task-irrelevant distractor did reduce search accuracy on the primary task (Experiment 2,3,4), upright face-configuration did not impair performance any more than an inverted faceconfiguration. Despite this, the pattern of visual search performance in all the reported experiments here have been consistent with that found other visual-search studies. In Experiment 1, our a priori expectations of task-difficulty was validated. In replication of previous findings, target-distractor similarity was a consistent determinant of search performance. Thus, the target-letter ‘K’ was more easily found when it was embedded in an array of curved letter-distractors e.g. ‘C’, than when amongst similar, angular letter-distractors e.g. ‘X' (cf. Neisser, 1964; Duncan & Humphreys, 1989). Judging by the findings of experiments 2,3 and 4, the search process for the target-letter ‘K’ when amongst visually-similar distractors is also comparable to that of a serial and self-terminating , judging by the findings of experiment 2,3 and 4 (cf. Wolfe, 1994; 1998). Participants of experiments 2, 3 and 4 had a different task demand from those of the first experiment. Instead of reporting the spatial location of the target, the only requirement was to indicate their awareness of target-presence. Therefore, experimental trials were either target-absent or target-present. The serial-search model predicted worse performance for target-absent trials and this was a highly consistent finding across experiment 2, 3 and 4 {Sternberg, 1966 #114;Wolfe, 1994 #134;Wolfe, 1998 #124}. This prediction is based on the supposition that if serial-search (as opposed to parallel search) is used, one can be absolutely certain that a target is absent only by scanning the entire display, item-by-item. In contrast, serial searches on target-present trials self-terminate the instant the target is found, resulting in shorter response latency; and more accurate performance for limited presentation times. In other words, we could be certain that visual spatial attention was deployed in these experiments; the same form of attention that is sometimes described as an attentional “spotlight” {Treisman, 1988 #122}. The pattern of these visual search results provide the assurance that the experimental design has fulfilled its purpose of invoking the use of well-defined visual search processes on the primary task. Experiment 1 allowed a comparison of the influence of an upright face-configuration on both parallel and serial search processes. With experiments 2, 3 and 4, an assessment was made on how the presence of an upright face-configuration could guide serial search. The results obtained indicate that the serial search process is not influenced by an upright faceconfiguration at all. Contrary to our predictions, the presence of an upright face-distractor did not reliably detract from normal visual search performance; measured in reaction-time and accuracy. In fact, the presence of face-type distractors did not influence visual search in Experiment 1 at all. This prompted several changes in the original experimental design. The improvements suggested included: changing the task to prevent alternative task-solving strategies e.g. inference. only testing the influence of upright faces on search performance for arrays with target-similar elements. drawing the face-type distractors closer to the visual search array so that it fell in the foveal region. The most significant change introduced has been that of the primary taskresponse. In Experiment 1, participants were required to indicate if the target letter was to be found in the letter-string above or below the fixation cross. This inadvertently introduced a confound in that participants were plausibly able to return an accurate response by only monitoring one of the two letter-strings. Since all the trials were target-present, participants were able to accurately infer that the target was to be found in the complementary letter-string, if it was absent in the letter-string actively monitored. By changing the task to a target-presence (yes/no) task in Experiment 2, participants were now compelled to search the whole array in order to come up with an accurate answer. The results in Experiment 1 also indicated strong practise effects for searches for the critical letter-target amongst target-dissimilar distractors. To reduce performance variability across trials, only the difficult search trials were used in all subsequent experiments. Finally, the task-irrelevant face distractors were drawn closer to the critical search array for Experiments 2,3 and 4, so that it fell within the foveal region and difficult to ignore. The final recommendation was made because it was noted that the task-irrelevant distractors failed to influence search performances at all, regardless of their type. This suggested that the taskirrelevant distractors were completely ignored even if they were images of upright faces. In Expt 2, the presence of task-irrelevant distractors impaired detection accuracy on the primary task. This implied that the modifications worked and that at the very least, the presence of distractors was exerting a perceptual load on the primary task, if not an attentional one. Nonetheless, the severity of impairment on the primary visual-search performance was completely independent of the type of taskirrelevant distractors. Upright faces did not reduce search accuracy and response latency on the letter-detection task any more than inverted faces did. Finally, further modifications were systematically introduced in Experiment 3 and 4 to render the task-irrelevant distractors comparable to those used in the facedetection experiments {Purcell, 1986 #27;Purcell, 1988 #24} as well as increase their visual salience. In Experiment 3, a backward visual mask was introduced after the critical display was presented just like with the face-detection experimental paradigm (cf. {Purcell, 1986 #27;Purcell, 1988 #24}. Results did not differ from those obtained in Experiment 2. Subsequently, the upright/inverted face-distractors were drawn even closer to the primary search display for Experiment 4. Again, this failed to bring about findings that supported the hypothesis. Namely, that an upright face-configuration leads to the involuntary capture of visual attention. Given the current findings, it is appropriate to conclude that an upright faceconfiguration is not capable of attracting visual attention away from the spatial locus of a critical search array. Still, it is important to note that in requiring participants to conduct a visual search for the letter ‘K’, these experiments have explicitly encouraged the establishment of an endogenous search strategy. Therefore, it could still be that the face-detection effect is based on an automatic process that orients exogenous visual attention towards upright face-configurations; but, only in the complete absence of an endogenous attentional strategy. The purpose of using nonface stimuli in the critical search display was to completely remove any possible involvement of the face-identification system in the task-solving process. The findings of the studies reported here indicate that simply in having a well-specified target to search for eliminates any influence that an upright face-configuration might be able to exert on visual orienting. In the dual-route model by de Gelder and Rouw (2001) (see Fig.1.3), the general object-identification system can be tuned to a continuum of whole-based or part-based perceptual encoding strategy; the face-identification system is considered to be a specialised sub-system of this, and is preferentially biased to whole-based encoding. By having a specific visual item to search for, one is pre-disposed to employing the object-identification system, be it a face or letter of the alphabet. Farah and colleagues {Farah, 1992 #191;Farah, 1998 #192} might even go so far as to argue that just as face-identification is biased to whole-based perceptual encoding, letteridentification is ideally placed to benefit from part-based encoding. This is because letters are the building blocks of whole words, but are of significant importance by themselves. Essentially, the findings in this study concur with those conducted by Nothdurft (1993) and Kuehn and Jolicuer (1994). That is, upright faces are not capable of capturing visual attention when one is searching for a well-defined target; whether the target should be an upright face or a known letter of the alphabet. It is not entirely clear how this account might explain the results of Gorea and Julesz (1991) whereby, they did find a benefit for target search performances if the target was a constituent of the upright face pattern. In this particular experiment, the target was merely any non-oblique line in an array of oblique lines of varying angular difference from the targets. Thus, we could argue that since orientation is, arguably, not a member of any well-defined object category (cf. faces, letters of the alphabet), participants were able to circumvent the object-identification system in their searches. In doing so, it allowed the the search process to be completely sensitized to the influence of the face-detection system that effectively oriented observers to the upright face-configuration in the display. When introducing this chapter, abrupt visual onset was raised as a stimuli that lays strong claim to the ability to capture attention {Yantis, 1990 #115;Jonides, 1988 #116;Yantis, 1984 #117}. However, there have been failed attempts in inducing attentional capture, even with abrupt visual onsets. Reconsidering the supporting literature on attentional capture, it is noted that a priori knowledge of target position can allow the observer to demonstrate resilience to performance decrements induced by attentional capture {Yantis, 1990 #115}. Behavioural evidence has revealed that regions of attentional inhibition can be found, surrounding a region of attentional facilitation {Mounts, 2000 #79;Mounts, 2000 #78}. It could have been that always presenting the critical search array in the same region resulted in creating a circumscribed region of attentional facilitation where the critical array was expected to be, and a surrounding region of attentional inhibition that completely suppressed the attentional-capture properties of an upright face could have exerted. However, this is not to say that an upright face cannot capture attention under conditions when the observer has no a priori expectations. In the next chapter, a study will be presented in which participants were not predisposed towards either the identification of any category of visual stimuli or any particular spatial region in the visual field. Having established that upright faceconfigurations do not capture attention when an endogenous search strategy is in place, the next chapter sets out to investigate whether upright faces can orient visual attention towards itself in the complete absence of any endogenous search strategies.