1 Differential Effects of Emotion Focused Therapy and Psycho-education in Facilitating Forgiveness and Letting Go of Emotional Injuries Leslie S. Greenberg, Serine H. Warwar, Wanda M. Malcolm York University, Toronto Correspondence should be sent to the first author Leslie S. Greenberg, Dept. of Psychology York University 4700 Keele St. Toronto, ON, Canada M3J 1P3. Email: lgrnberg@yorku.ca This study was supported by a grant ID# CRF 5202 from the Campaign for forgiveness research to the first author. 2 Abstract This study compared the effectiveness of Emotion-focused Therapy (EFT) involving gestalt empty-chair dialogue in the treatment of individuals who were emotionally injured by a significant other with a psycho-education group designed to deal with these injuries. In addition, this study examined aspects of the emotional process of forgiveness in resolving interpersonal injuries and investigated the relationship between letting go of distressing feelings and forgiveness. A total of forty-six clients assessed as having unresolved, interpersonal, emotional injuries were randomly assigned to an individual therapy treatment of EFT or a psycho-education group. Clients were assessed pre-treatment, post-treatment, and at 3-month follow-up on measures of forgiveness, letting go, depression, global symptoms, and key target complaints. Results indicated that clients in EFT using empty chair dialogue showed significantly more improvement than the psycho-education treatment on all measures of forgiveness and letting go, as well as global symptoms and key target complaints. Key words Interpersonal injuries, Forgiveness, Letting go, Emotion-focused therapy, Psycho-education 3 Differential Effects of Emotion Focused Therapy and Psycho-education in Facilitating Forgiveness and Letting Go of Emotional Injuries Forgiveness has recently been proposed as an important aspect of emotional recovery following an interpersonal injury (Enright & Fitzgibbons, 2000; Worthington, 1998, 2001). Although the debate continues as to whether forgiveness should be granted to injurers when they will not take responsibility for their actions, or when they continue to perpetuate harmful acts (Worthington, 2005), forgiveness has been shown to have a positive impact on physical, relational, mental and spiritual health, whereas unforgiveness can be distressing and may leave people ruminating about their injuries and feeling hostile towards those who injured them (Witvliet, Ludwig, & Vander Laan, 2001). The majority of studies on the facilitation of forgiveness (Al-Mabuk, Enright & Cardis, 1995; Hebl & Enright, 1993; McCullough & Worthington, 1995; Ripley & Worthington, 2002; Worthington & Drinkard, 2000; Wade, Worthington, & Meyer, 2005) have involved psycho-educational group programs designed to promote the benefits of forgiveness to self and others, and these provide the knowledge and skills associated with a particular model of forgiveness. To date only two studies have been published that report on an investigation of the effectiveness of individual therapy in facilitating unilateral forgiveness (Coyle and Enright, 1997; Freedman and Enright, 1996). The present study examined the effectiveness of Emotion-focused Therapy (EFT) compared to a psychoeducational group in facilitating emotional resolution and forgiveness. Numerous investigators have proposed that both emotion work and empathy play important roles in forgiveness (Davenport, 1991; Enright & Fitzgibbons, 2000; Fitzgibbons, 1986; Hope, 1987; Karen, 2001; Malcolm, Warwar, & Greenberg, 2005). One of the assumptions of EFT is that the blocking of primary biologically adaptive emotions subverts 4 healthy boundary setting, self-respectful anger and necessary grieving, and that adequate processing of unresolved emotion leads to its transformation (Greenberg, 2002). Many clinicians (Akhtar, 2002; Baures, 1996; Enright & Eastin, 1992) emphasize the value of facilitating insession expressions of adaptive anger at violation, and suggest that facilitating forgiveness requires an acknowledgement of the legitimacy of emotions such as resentment and hatred towards the offender. Baures (1996), Boss (1997), and Fincham (2000) consider resentment and desires for revenge to be closely linked with self-respect, and Greenberg and Paivio (1997) suggest that there may be times when it is therapeutic to encourage clients to talk about their revenge fantasies. From these perspectives, the desire to retaliate is normalized as a sign of how damaged the injured person feels. Encouraging such expressions in therapy is not the same thing as promoting outer-directed blaming or hurling of insults. In encouraging clients to speak from their inner experiences of violation, the therapist is promoting ownership of a client’s emotional experience and is empowering clients to appropriately assign responsibility for harm done. Ownership of emotion also helps clients focus on their own needs and concerns rather than getting stuck in blaming the other or feeling victimized. The danger in short-circuiting expressions of anger as might occur in some treatments is that the client may end up condoning or excusing the injurer’s hurtful behavior, or inappropriately take too much responsibility for the unfolding of events that surrounded the injury. Therapists also need to facilitate the process of grieving the loss of, or damage to, a significant relationship, as well as the shattering of the client’s view of self and the world that may have been caused by the injury. Akhtar (2002) addresses the relationship between mourning and forgiveness, and Greenberg and Paivio (1997) emphasize that work with betrayal and abandonment often involves a process of facilitating normal grieving in which anger and sadness 5 play central roles. Facilitating an imaginary dialogue with the injurer can help the client grieve and say goodbye to what has been lost or irreparably damaged as a consequence of the injury (Greenberg Rice & Elliott 1993; Elliott, Watson, Goldman & Greenberg 2004). Emotion-focused Therapy (Greenberg 2002), which employs empty-chair dialogue for the resolution of unfinished business as one of its major methods has been found to be an effective intervention in the treatment of depression, interpersonal problems and trauma (Greenberg & Watson, 1998, 2005, Paivio & Greenberg, 1995; Paivio & Niewenhaus, 2001). Paivio and Greenberg’s (1995) comparative study of a psycho-educational group intervention versus individual EFT using gestalt empty-chair dialogue in the resolution of unfinished business, supported the efficacy of the empty-chair intervention. The results demonstrated a significant reduction in symptomotology, target complaints, and interpersonal distress and more resolution of unfinished business. The empty-chair method as it is employed in EFT is also a particularly effective tool in promoting empathy felt toward the offender (Paivio, Hall, Holowaty, Jellis, & Tran, 2001;Paivio & Nieuwenhuis 2001; Paivio & Greenberg, 1995). In imaginatively bringing the injurer and injury alive, the client moves from a cognitive discussion with the therapist, to an imaginal confrontation and dialogue with the injurer. In so doing, the client is helped to move reified inner representations of self and other (injurer) into a transitional space in conscious awareness where the representations can be re-examined, reworked, and resolved. Empirical evidence also is mounting in support of the importance of transforming emotions by changing one emotion with another emotion (Frederickson, 1998; Greenberg 2002, 2004), and this suggests that a maladaptive emotion state can be effectively transformed by undoing it with the presence of another more adaptive emotion. More specifically Frederickson, 6 Marcuso, Branigun & Tugade (2002) have shown that positive emotion undoes the cardiovascular after-effects of negative emotion. Greenberg (2002) has suggested that the key to transforming maladaptive emotions is to access alternate healthy adaptive emotions to act as resources in the self. Thus, in an emotion-focused treatment, feelings related to unforgiveness such as anger, contempt and pain are eventually changed by accessing feelings of sadness, compassion, empathy and concern. McCullough and his colleagues have shown that empathy for the perpetrator mediates successful forgiveness (McCullough, Worthington, & Rachal, 1997; McCullough, Rachal, Sandage, & Worthington, 1997). This proposition is consistent with clinical observation, theory, and empirical evidence concerning forgiveness (Macaskill, Maltby, & Day, 2002; McCullough, Worthington, & Rachal, 1997; McCullough, Rachal, Sandage, & Worthington, 1997; Worthington & Wade, 1999). When accessed, empathy involves understanding another’s feelings and is a complex cognitive/affective state that facilitates forgiveness of an interpersonal injury. As Rowe et al. (1989) have pointed out, empathy towards the injurer involves being able to see the other person as acting in a quintessentially human manner, which flows out of the context of his or her own self-focused needs and perceptions. This includes (but does not require) the possibility of recognizing that what the injurer did was similar to something one has done, or could do under the same circumstances. In addition to assisting in the revision of how one sees the injurer, cognitive perspective taking sometimes allows the injury itself to be recast within a broader understanding of the context of the unfolding of events. However, cognitive perspective taking of this nature does not have to involve warm benevolent feelings associated with forgiveness. Something more is required for forgiveness and this appears to be compassion for the injurer, or affective empathy. 7 Affective empathy is best understood as a means of imaginative entry into the world of the other, which generates a bodily felt sense of understanding what the other person may have been feeling, without actually sharing the same experience (Greenberg & Rosenberg, 2003). Berecz (2001) suggests that the task for the injured person is to imaginatively transpose himself or herself into the other person’s place in an attempt to understand the unfolding of events from the injurer’s perspective. Unforgiveness has been defined as the combination of a complex set of negative feelings towards an injurer, and it has been shown that people can decrease unforgiveness without increasing forgiveness (Worthington, Sandage & Berry, 2000; Worthington & Wade, 1999). Unforgiveness is regarded as being stuck in negative emotions and a hyperaroused stress response through rumination (Harris, & Thoresen, 2005). It is noteworthy that reducing unforgiveness is not the same as promoting forgiveness. Forgiveness seems to include the reduction of unforgiveness, or letting go, through decreasing negative feelings and thoughts in relation to the injurer. In addition to, and in contrast with letting go or reducing unforgiveness, forgiveness is also comprised of the increase of positive emotions such as compassion, empathy or understanding felt towards the injurer. In our view, forgiveness thus appears to involve two important emotional processes: resolving the hurt and anger involved in the injury; and the possible generation of positive feelings of compassion, loving, kindness and empathic concern for the injurer. People thus may be able to resolve emotional injuries, by reducing or letting go of their bad feelings, or by letting go of bad feelings and increasing positive feelings ie: by forgiving. The main purpose of the present study was to evaluate the effectiveness of Emotionfocused Therapy (EFT) (Greenberg et al., 1993) involving empty-chair dialogue in the treatment 8 of individuals who had been emotionally injured by a significant other. Empty-chair work was used to facilitate emotional transformation by: both expressing and processing the anger and sadness to the offender; facilitating empathy; and asking clients to play the role of the injurer, thereby having them imagine what the injurer might feel if he or she were capable of comprehending the consequences and impact of his or her actions on the client. The primary hypothesis was that EFT, which used empty-chair dialogue to process unresolved emotion, would produce better outcomes than a psycho-education group (PG) in the treatment of interpersonal, emotional injuries on measures of forgiveness and letting go, and other indices of outcome. It was assumed that an experiential treatment such as EFT that works by evoking, processing and transforming emotion would address the emotional causes of the injury more directly than a psycho-education group that was less emotion-activating. This study also examined the emotional process of forgiveness in resolving interpersonal injury and evaluated whether forgiveness was necessary to resolution of the injury. In the present study, recovery from an emotional injury therefore was conceptualized as occurring in one of the following two ways: 1) forgiving the injurer, which was defined as letting go of unresolved bad feelings or reducing unforgiveness plus the development of empathy and compassion for the other; or 2) letting go of the bad feelings which involves letting go of unmet needs and negative feelings in relation to the injury or injurer and changing negative perceptions of self in relation to the injury or injurer, without the development of empathy and compassion for the other. The second hypothesis was that all people who forgave would let go of bad feelings but that not all who let go would forgive. The third hypothesis was that reported emotional arousal would be higher in the EFT, than in PG 9 Treatment focused on facilitating the resolution of specific unresolved interpersonal, emotional injuries that had occurred at least two years prior to the start of therapy and continued to be distressing. The injuries that clients brought to treatment were both emotional and interpersonal; they were emotional in that they involved intense lingering unresolved feelings of hurt or anger and betrayal, and they were interpersonal in that the injurer was a significant other in the injured person’s life. Injuries involved abandonment, betrayals or violations by significant others, such a friends, bosses, family members, or intimate partners. Method Participants The sample for the present study consisted of 46 clients who had an unresolved interpersonal, emotional injury with a significant other that had occurred at least two years prior to commencing treatment. The requirement that the injury not be more recent was to ensure that the natural process of recovering from hurts had been given time to work and that the injured person was not in the midst of coping with the immediate aftermath of the injury. Participants were required to be 18 years of age or older. Exclusion criteria for the study, based on the assumption a brief treatment program would be unsuitable for some people, were as follows: victims of incest; individuals who had attempted suicide or had lost a significant other in the past year; those currently in physically violent relationships; individuals currently abusing drugs or alcohol; individuals diagnosed with antisocial, borderline or narcissistic personality disorder, post-traumatic stress disorder, or a psychotic disorder. Individuals who were already in psychotherapy elsewhere were also excluded. The average age for clients in the EFT treatment was 43 years. The average age for the PG treatment was 46. The overall population thus had a mean age of 44.5 and S.D = 8.3 (range 10 22 - 67). There were seven males and 16 females in the EFT treatment and 13 males and 10 females in the PG treatment. In the EFT treatment, eight individuals had never been married, 13 were married, and two were separated or divorced. In the PG treatment, five had never been married, nine were married, and nine were separated or divorced. In terms of their level of education, in the EFT treatment one individual had completed high school, three had some college or university training, 12 had graduated from college or university, and seven had postgraduate experience. In the PG treatment, five individuals had completed high school, three had some college or university training, six had graduated from college or university, and nine had postgraduate experience. Ethnicity in both groups was predominantly Caucasian with 1 client of South Asian and one of East Asian origins in each treatment group. In terms of pre-treatment diagnosis on DSM Axis I and II, there were nine clients (39%) with a least one Axis I diagnosis in the EFT treatment, and seven clients (30%) with at least one Axis I diagnosis in the PG treatment. In the EFT treatment there were six clients with at least one Axis II diagnosis, and in the PG treatment there were at least four clients with an Axis II diagnosis. The mean Global Assessment of Functioning was 77 for each treatment, with the range being 65-95 for the EFT treatment and 60-90 for the PG treatment. There were no statistically significant differences between treatment conditions on any of these variables. Emotional Injuries The types of emotional injuries clients presented in this study, and the nature of the relationship with the significant other are summarized in Table 1. Each participant targetd one injury. Parents were the main perpetrators of the injuries. In the EFT treatment, 18 (78%) of the clients were dealing with an interpersonal injury in relation to at least one parent and two (8%) with an ex-partner, while in the PG treatment, 13 (57%) of the participants were dealing with an 11 injury regarding at least one parent and five (21%) with an ex-partner. The people in the “other” category included a boss, a neighbor, and people in non-parental positions of power relative to children. Therapists There were eight therapists in the EFT condition, two of whom were male and six female. Two of the therapists were registered psychologists, one had a doctorate and five were advanced doctoral students in clinical psychology. Prior to training for the treatment study, all therapists were required to have at least one year of EFT therapy training including prior empathy training, and one year of experience as a therapist. Therapists in the EFT treatment received an additional 30 hours of specialized training based on a treatment manual for resolving emotional injuries developed for this project (Greenberg, Malcolm & Warwar, 2002). The group had two leaders. One of the group leaders was a registered psychologist who had devised the PG treatment and conducted it on a number of prior occasions. The co-leader was a doctoral student and was trained by the first leader. The therapists in both treatment conditions received weekly supervision throughout the study to promote adherence to treatment manuals. Treatments Both interventions involved 12 hours of treatment distributed over approximately 12 weeks. Emotion Focused Therapy The treatment manual for this study is based on the principles outlined for Emotionfocused Therapy (EFT) (Greenberg et al., 1993; Greenberg, 2002), also known as Processexperiential therapy. This therapy includes the implementation of the person-centered relational attitudes of empathy, positive regard, and congruence, as well as marker-guided, process 12 directive, experiential interventions. In EFT, the therapist uses the following interventions: gestalt two-chair dialogues when clients present in-session self-evaluative conflicts; clientcentered systematic evocative unfolding for problematic reactions over which clients are puzzling; and gestalt empty-chair dialogue for resolving currently felt unfinished business with a significant other. Focusing (Gendlin, 1996) is also utilized in this approach to assist clients in attending to their internal experience and to obtain a bodily felt sense of the issues they are exploring and struggling with (Gendlin, 1996, Greenberg et al., 1993). The emphasis in EFT is on accessing primary adaptive feelings and maladaptive emotion schemes in order to make them amenable to change (Greenberg, 1993, Greenberg & Paivio, 1997). A specialized EFT treatment manual (Greenberg, Malcolm, & Warwar, 2002) was developed for this project to focus on facilitating the resolution of emotional interpersonal injuries. The treatment protocol is summarized in the following four phases which overlap rather than being purely sequentially. Phase I: Creating an Alliance. The first phase of treatment involves creating a therapeutic alliance with the client by empathically responding to and validating the client’s pain and emotional experience of the interpersonal injury. This stage also entails helping clients to identity the impact of the injury and articulate and clarify the most problematic aspects of the injury for them. Phase II: Evocation and Exploration. The second phase of treatment involves acknowledging, experiencing, and expressing the anger, sadness, pain, and other distressing feelings associated with the emotional injury. Empty chair work is utilized to help clients process unresolved feelings towards the injurer. Therapists were advised to begin work on Evocation and Exploration as early as the second session if the injury was clear and safety and 13 the bond seemed to be sufficiently strong no later than the 3rd session if the client appeared ready, and to continue up until the pen ultimate session if needed. Empty chair work however was not to be done in every session but if suitable in at least half the sessions in the evocation phase. The next two phases integrate within and overlap with phase 2. Phase III: Self-Interruptive Work. The third phase of therapy involves interventions facilitated by therapists at client markers of interruption such as emotional constriction, resignation, or hopelessness. These interventions are aimed at turning the passive, automatic process of interruption into an active one. This phase aims to heighten clients’ awareness of how they interrupt themselves and to promote change in these interruptive processes so that emotions preventing resolution can be accessed and processed. Phase IV: Empowerment and Letting Go or Forgiving. This final phase entails accessing previously unexpressed emotions, and mobilizing and promoting the entitlement of unmet needs. The therapist promotes a change in the way the client views the injurer, facilitated by emotional arousal and accessing of past unmet needs. This phase also involves helping clients grieve and let go of unmet needs. Elaborating the world-view of the other aids empathy towards the injurer and the therapist helps the client understand or hold the other accountable. Homework. Clients were asked to complete homework throughout the course of treatment. At the start, they were asked to keep a diary of their feelings and thoughts in relation to their injuries and to note how the therapy sessions played a role in their change processes. At session six, clients were given a handout which instructed them to write an unmailed letter to the injurer accusing the injurer of knowing the impact of his or her hurtful behavior. The second part of this homework exercise instructed clients to write a letter in response to themselves denying the accusation from the perspective of the person who injured them. This was done to 14 highlight that change was to come from the client, not the perpetrator, as the perpetrator may never change. At session seven, clients were given some definitions of forgiveness and asked to think about their understanding of forgiveness and to consider whether forgiveness was important to them personally with respect to their emotional injuries. At session eight, clients were given a handout which asked them to reflect on the bond that still held them to the injurer, and to write down their difficulties in letting go of the emotional injury and what was sustaining the painful feelings. These were discussed in the session Psychoeducation Group The psycho-education group manual (PG) was devised for this study (Malcolm, 2001) and drew on various sources for content (Bolger, 1999; Klassen, 2001; Paivio & Greenberg, 1995; Smedes, 1984; Worthington and Drinkard, 2000). The six workshops were facilitated by the two workshop leaders. The introductory session provided an overview of all the sessions, along with a rationale for participating in the study and an explanation of the differences between PG and group therapy. Sessions two through five included a discussion of the previous session’s homework, a topic presentation by the facilitators, a coffee break with a personal reflection task and then group discussion of the presentation and personal reflection. Each session ended with the assignment of a homework task to be done between sessions and completion of session measures. The content presentations covered the following topics: the nature and structure of an emotional injury; understanding unfinished business and how it disrupts adaptive functioning; aspects of forgiveness, including what it is and is not, and why one would be motivated to forgive in the face of being hurt by another person; the role of pain and other strong emotions in experiencing and recovering from interpersonal emotional injuries; the process of reconciliation with the hurtful other and how it differs from forgiveness; and finally how to resolve an injury. 15 The content presentation of the first session was short and group discussion intentionally structured to create safety and assure group members that their participation in the group discussions was voluntary and at their discretion. The final session reviewed the material presented in the first five sessions and instead of a homework assignment, a bibliography was provided for those interested in further reading on the topics covered in the workshops. The PG group members received the same homework as those in individual therapy, in the same order and at approximately the same time intervals. They also received additional journaling and reflection exercises in order to provide homework assignments after each of the first five workshops. It was assumed that any change experienced in the PG group would be the result of discussion of and reflection on the information provided, which would produce change in attitude(s). It was further assumed that attitude change would in turn change the feelings participants had toward the injurer and injury. Measures A battery of self-report measures was administered before and after treatment to assess changes in specific domains. All clients were assessed approximately one week prior to treatment and one week following treatment. Clients were also assessed at a three-month followup on measures of letting go, forgiveness, level of depression, global symptoms, and target complaints. Forgiveness Measures The Enright Forgiveness Inventory (Enright, Rique & Coyle, 2000). The Enright Forgiveness Inventory is a measure of the degree to which one person forgives another who has hurt him or her deeply or unfairly. The first part of the inventory instructs individuals to visualize the emotional injury, focus on the offending person, and imagine what happened. The 16 participant is asked to write a description of the injury in his or her own words. The second part of the inventory is comprised of 60 items from three 20-item subscales measuring affect, behavior and cognition in relation to forgiveness. Participants are asked to rate each item on a 1 to six-point Likert scale from Strongly Disagree to Strongly Agree. The authors report a stability coefficient in a community sample for total EFI scores of .86 during a four week test-retest reliability study. The subscale test-retest coefficients ranged from .67 to .91. With respect to concurrent validity, the EFI has been positively associated with other measures of forgiveness. There is also support for the EFI’s divergent validity (Enright et al., 2000). The Cronbachs alpha for the scale as a whole in this sample was .81. Forgiveness Measure (Enright et al., 2000). This single item scale for assessing degree of forgiveness derives from the EFI and directly asks clients to what extent they have forgiven the person who injured them. Responses are indicated on a five-point Likert scale (1 = not at all, 3 = in progress, 5 = completely). The authors of the EFI did not use the term “forgiveness” in any other item of the EFI measure to avoid creating conceptual biases. For this reason the EFI is referred to as the Attitude Scale during its administration. The forgiveness measure is thus used to directly assess degree of forgiveness. In classifying peoples degree of forgiveness a score of 4 or above, i.e.: rating that they had forgiven, either “a lot” or “completely” was used to indicate that forgiveness had been attained Unfinished Business Empathy and Acceptance Scale (UFB EA). Singh (1994) developed the Unfinished Business Scale to measure resolution of unfinished business with a significant other. For the present study, items were extracted from this scale and adapted to create the empathy and acceptance subscale (UFB EA). The UFB EA Scale is comprised of six items and measures the extent to which clients feel acceptance and empathy towards the individual who 17 injured them. Clients were asked to indicate their agreement with the statements of empathy or acceptance on a five-point Likert scale (ranging from 1 = not at all, to 5 = very much). For example, empathy items consisted of statements such as ‘I feel compassionately understanding of the other person’, or ‘I have a real appreciation of this person’s own personal difficulties.’ Acceptance items included statements such as: ‘I feel accepting toward this person’ and reversescored items like ‘I see this person negatively.’ The items on this subscale have been found to inter-correlate highly in a sample of clients in treatment for unfinished business (Paivio & Greenberg 1995) and a sample of university students (Singh 1994) and the overall scale has been found to correlate with other outcome measures (Watson & Greenberg 1996; Paivio & Greenberg 1995). The Cronbachs alpha for the subscale in this sample was .87. Letting Go Measures Unfinished Business Feelings and Needs Scale (UFB FN). This scale was adapted from Singh’s (1994) Unfinished Business Scale. It measures the resolution of feelings and needs as they relate to the injurer, and positive changes in the perception of self. This measure is comprised of eight items on a five-point Likert scale (ranging from 1 = not at all, to 5 = very much). The UFB FN Scale contains three sets of items that refer to feelings, needs, and the self. The feelings subscale contains items such as ‘I feel unable to let go of my unresolved feelings in relation to this person.’ The needs subscale includes items like ‘I feel frustrated about not having my needs met by this person.’ Finally, self items included statements such as, ‘This person’s negative view or treatment of me has made me feel badly about my self ’, or ‘I feel worthwhile in relation to this person.’ This subscale of the UFB has shown inter-item reliability in a sample of clients in treatment and a sample of university students and has been found to 18 correlate with other outcome measures a sample of clients in treatment. The Cronbachs alpha for the subscale in this sample was .79. Letting Go Measure. This measure was constructed for this study to parallel the single item forgiveness measure. It is a single item self-report measure that asks clients to what extent they have let go of their hurt and angry feelings in relation to the injurer. Responses are indicated on a five-point Likert scale (1 = not at all, 3 = in progress, 5 = completely). Other Outcome Measures Target Complaints (TC) Discomfort and Change Scale (Battle, Inber, Hoehn-Saric, Stone, Nash, & Frank, 1968). The Target Complaints Discomfort Scale asks clients to specify three problems they would like to see change as a result of treatment. Clients were asked to rate each problem at three points in time (pre-treatment, post-treatment and three-month follow-up) in terms of how distressed they are by it. In addition, at two points in time (post-treatment and three-month follow-up) clients were asked to rate how much they felt it had changed since the beginning of treatment. Battle et al. (1968) reported high correlations with other outcome measures and test-retest reliability (r = .68) between pre- and post-psychiatric interviews. Global Symptom Index (GSI) of the Symptom Checklist 90 Revised (SCL-90-R). The SCL-90-R (Derogatis, 1983) is a well known instrument that measures general symptom distress, with high internal consistency (.77 to .90) and test-retest reliability (.80-.90) over a one-week interval with people with a variety of disorders (Derogatis, Rickels, & Roch, 1976). Calculations of change on the Global Symptom Index(GSI) was used as an outcome measure at three points in time (pre-treatment, post-treatment and three-month follow-up). Cronbachs alpha for GSI in this sample was .82 19 Beck Depression Inventory. This 21-item inventory is widely used to assess depression (Beck, Ward, Mendelson, Mock, Erbaugh, 1961). It has high internal consistency and correlates highly with other self-report measures of depression and with clinician’s ratings of depression (r = .60 to .90; Beck et al., 1988). Cronbachs alpha for BDI in this sample was .87 Process Measures Working Alliance Inventory (WAI; Horvath & Greenberg, 1989). The WAI is a 36-item scale rated on a seven-point Likert scale. The WAI is made up of three alliance subscales that assess the therapist-client bond and agreement on therapy tasks and goals. Internal consistency is high for the whole scale (.87 to .93) as well as the subscales (.89 to .92) (Horvath & Greenberg, 1989). Cronbachs alpha for WAI was .89 Emotional Arousal Session Report Measure (Warwar & Greenberg, 2002). The EA Session Report Measure was developed in order to evaluate the intensity of emotional arousal experienced during therapy. This measure consists of 18 emotions items, which the client rates on a seven-point Likert scale (1= not at all, 5 = moderately, 7 = very much). Clients are instructed to indicate the degree to which they felt each of the 18 emotions during their session. To account for the possibility that the 18 emotion categories omitted an emotion the client believed to be vital in describing their emotional experience, an additional item (question 19) gives the client an opportunity to rate the intensity of any other emotion they may have felt. The EA Session Report Measure was completed by clients after each session. This measure was constructed as a self-report form of the observer Emotional Arousal Scale (Warwar & Greenberg, 1999) which has been show to predict outcome. Procedure 20 Clients were recruited through advertisements in local community newspapers and flyers distributed to the university and general community. The advertisements announced that a treatment study was being conducted at the York University Psychotherapy Clinic and that individuals over the age of 18 who had been emotionally injured by a significant other in the past who were interested in participating in a treatment study should call for more information. The advertisements for the study also indicated that the injury should have occurred at least two years prior to responding to the advertisement, and that respondents should still be experiencing some lingering feelings of hurt or anger towards the other in relation to the injury. Eighty six participants who called were first briefly interviewed over the telephone to assess initial suitability regarding general inclusion and exclusion criteria, and to determine whether they were presenting with a specified unresolved target injury from at least 2 years prior and were willing to participate in a videotaped research treatment. Fifty nine suitable potential participants were invited to undergo a further assessment process to ensure that the proposed treatment program could meet their treatment needs. The initial two hour assessment interview was designed to obtain consent for assessment and treatment and assess clients in terms of Axis I and Axis II disorders using the Semi-structured Clinical Interview and Diagnosis protocol (SCID; Spitzer, Williams, Gibbon, & First, 1990). Clients also completed the Beck Depression Inventory and the SCL-90-R at the first assessment appointment. If this initial assessment supported the appropriateness of the proposed treatment for the client, he or she was invited to participate in the treatment study, and asked to come in to complete the rest of the pre-treatment measures. Forty six clients were successfully assigned to treatment. Participants were randomly assigned to either the PG or EFT treatment. Clients in the individual EFT treatment were seen for 12 one-hour weekly individual therapy sessions. Clients 21 in the PG group were seen for six two-hour sessions that were held bi-weekly for 12 weeks. Three PG groups (of n=9, n=8 and n=6) were run for a total of 23 participants. In the EFT treatment one therapist saw five clients, one saw four, two saw three and four saw two each. Sessions in the PG group were audiotaped only, whereas sessions in the EFT treatment were both audiotaped and videotaped. Clients in both groups completed questionnaires following each treatment session and were assigned homework at approximately the same time intervals throughout the course of treatment. Clients completed outcome measures at pre-treatment, termination of treatment, during a post-therapy interview, and at a three-month follow-up interview. A short form of the Working Alliance Inventory was given to the PG clients following session one, and to the individual EFT clients after session three. Therapists completed a post-session questionnaire after each session. Results Adherence to the therapy treatment manual in the EFT treatment was monitored by therapist and supervisor reports. Adherence to the PG group manual was monitored only by the therapists. Therapists reported on a 5 point scale ranging from “not at all” to “completely”, the degree to which they judged themselves to have adhered to the treatment protocol and in the individual therapy condition reported whether they had used chair dialogues during the session. The supervisor using the same five point scale, reported an adherence judgment from supervising a video tape of at least four of the individual therapy sessions. Means and standard deviations of therapist reports for the individual treatment over the sessions were 4.23 (1.11) and supervisor ratings were 4.14 (.52) indicating good adherence. The therapists reported a mean of 5.13 chair dialogues per treatment with a range of 4 – 7 per client. The PG treatment leader reported successful implementation of the group manual for each session. 22 Given that clients were grouped within treatment with 9, 8 and 6 respectively in the 3 PG groups, and a different number of clients were seen by each therapist, there was a nonindependence in the data and this could introduce a statistical bias in the analyses (Kenny, 1995). Treatment outcome scores thus were investigated for bias by looking at intra class correlation to measure homogeneity within groups in relation to variation between groups and the F statistics used in the analyses of variance were adjusted if the correlation was larger than 0.1. For the analyses that did not compare treatments, effects, the importance of ignoring statistical dependencies among observations, are likely to be relatively minor, and so in these cases dependence was ignored. Correlations were conducted between all pretreatment variables1. The pre-treatment BDI did not correlate with any of the other symptom measures at pretreatment. However, there were positive correlations between the Enright Forgiveness Inventory (EFI) and the Forgiveness Measure (r=.35, p<.05), and between the EFI and the Unfinished Business Empathy and Acceptance Scale (r=.56, p<.001). There were no significant differences at the .05 level between treatments on any of the pretreatment, demographic or other assessment variables assessed by means of one-way ANOVAs. A t-test conducted to evaluate whether therapeutic alliances were different in the two treatments showed that clients’ early working alliances were not significantly different at the .05 level (EFT individual therapy, M=5.89, SD=0.84; PG group, M=5.75, SD=0.78). Outcome In order to test the hypothesis that EFT would be more effective than PG repeated measures analyses of covariance were performed. Pre test scores of the dependent measure was used as the covariate with post treatment and follow up as the repeated measures and type of 23 therapy received (either PG or individual EFT) as the between groups factor. Target complaint change which does not have a measure at pre treatment was analyzed by a repeated measure analysis of variance with post and follow-up as the two occasions. The means for each of the measures for pre-therapy, post-therapy, and follow-up are provided in Table 2. There were no significant group by time interactions on any of the measures. The groups were significantly different on almost all measures such that the EFT groups exhibited the highest levels of forgiveness-related gains (i.e. Enright Forgiveness, Forgiveness, Empathy and Acceptance, Feelings and Needs, and Letting Go) as well as the greatest levels of symptom reduction (i.e. Target Complaints Discomfort, Target Complaints Change, and GSI) 2. However, the differences for the BDI were not statistically significant. Clinical Significance and Effect Size Cohen's d’s (1988), the standardized mean difference, which provide an index of the practical, as well as statistical significance of the differences between treatments and between pre and post treatment were calculated. The effect size for differences between treatments on the EFI was moderate d = .41, while pre-post effect sizes were large for both treatments, with d = 1 for EFT and .71 for PG treatment. This demonstrates that both treatments led practically to a large amount of change and that there was a meaningful difference between groups on the major measure of forgiveness. Pre- post effect on the GSI were d = .62, for the EFT treatment and was negligible for the PG treatment. The between treatment effect size was d = .66, again showing large effects for EFT over PG. The pre-post effects on empathy and acceptance were 1.73 and .74 for EFT and PG respectively and the between treatment effect size was .98. For feeling and needs the pre-post effects were 3.22 and 1.62 respectively with between treatment effects size of 1.62. The pre-post effects on TC discomfort were 3.84 and 2.27 for EFT and PG respectively 24 and the between treatment effect was 1.56. All these effects are very large. The between treatment effects on BDI was very small d =.06 and the pre- post effects of .47 and .69 although large were much smaller than on other measures. Forgiving and Letting Go Cut off Scores were used on the relevant outcome measures to classify clients according to whether they forgave their injurers and/or let go of their negative feelings towards the injurer (level 4 or above on each measure). The classification of clients on this dichotomous classification is shown in Table 4. Dichotimization on these single item measures however are suggestive rather than definitive because the dividing lines between forgiving and not forgiving and letting go and not letting go may not be that sharp. All of the nine clients in EFT who forgave the injurer also let go, as did the four people in the PG treatment who forgave the injurer. However, five people in the EFT treatment and three in the PG treatment let go but did not forgive. A Chi square analysis of the distribution comparing forgivers and those who let go was significant, X = 21.5 p<.001showing that 100% who forgave let go, while 38% who let go did not forgive. This suggests that letting go may be a necessary requirement of forgiveness (i.e., everyone who forgave also let go), but is not by itself sufficient for, nor the same thing as, forgiveness, since clients who met the criteria for letting go did not always consider themselves to have forgiven the injurer. It is important to note that in both groups a large proportion of those people who were classified as not forgiving or letting go rated themselves as in progress (level 3) on these tasks so it was not the case that they did not benefit from treatment. They simply had not yet reached as full a resolution according to our cutoff criteria as those who indicated they had more fully forgiven or let go. Test of emotional arousal in groups 25 Clients in each of the treatment modalities were compared on the intensity of selfreported emotional arousal (EA) experienced in the session to test for the hypothesized difference between treatment in reported emotional arousal. The EA measure was divided into positive and negative or unpleasant emotion clusters for the following periods: total duration of treatment and three phases of therapy (early, middle and late). The positive emotion cluster included happy and content, while the negative cluster included sad, angry, afraid, and pain. Changes in reported intensity of negative/unpleasant and pleasant in-session emotional arousal across three phases of each treatment were examined. For treatment comparisons the EFT sessions were grouped to form early (3 sessions), middle (5 sessions) and late phases (last 2 sessions). This structure was thought to best reflect the phase structure of the treatment. The six group sessions were broken into three sets of two sessions each. A 2x3x2 repeated measures ANOVA compared reported in-session intensity of positive and negative emotions in the two treatment conditions overall and by each phase of therapy. Means and standard deviations are shown in Table 4 and the graph over phases is given in figure1. There was a significant emotion by time interaction F (2, 43) =12.853, p<0.001, a significant emotion by group interaction F (1,44) = 24.6, p<0.001, and a significant three way emotion by time by group interaction F(2,43) = 3.29, p < .05. Post hoc analyses using a Bonferonni adjustment for multiple comparisons found the groups differed significantly (p < .001) on the amount of reported negative affect arousal overall in the treatment but that there were no significant differences at the .05 level between the treatment in self-reports of overall positive emotion experienced in-session over all. This acted as a form of implementation check showing that the individual EFT treatment was effective in arousing more unpleasant emotion. The clients in the EFT treatment reported significantly higher levels of negative/unpleasant 26 emotional arousal than the PG treatment, in the initial phase of treatment (p<.01) and in the middle phase of therapy, (p < .01). The clients in the PG treatment reported significantly higher levels of positive emotional arousal than the EFT treatment, in the middle phase of therapy (p < .01). Finally, during the late phase of therapy there was no significant difference at the .05 level between the groups in reported intensity of negative/unpleasant or positive emotional arousal. The EFT clients’ reports of in-session negative/unpleasant emotional arousal were found to be relatively stable over the first two phases, demonstrating no significant differences at the .05 level between early and middle phases. However, the EFT clients reported significantly less in-session negative/unpleasant emotional arousal in the final phase of therapy as compared to the middle phase (p< .05). Analysis of the PG clients’ reports followed a similar pattern. Differences between early and middle phases of treatment were not significant at the .05 level. However, a significant decline in reported negative emotional arousal from the middle to the late phase of therapy was present (p< .05). In an examination of the pattern of reported positive in-session emotional arousal, EFT clients reported a significant increase in the intensity of positive emotional arousal from the middle phase to the final phase of treatment (p<.001). In the PG group, a trend of increasing positive emotional arousal from the early to late phases of therapy was found. The PG clients’ ratings of positive emotions increased significantly from the early to middle treatment phase, p < .01 and from the middle to late phase, p< .05. Degree of reported in-session intensity of emotional arousal averaged over the whole treatment, or averaged of any of the phases of treatment, did not correlate significantly with change on any of the outcome measures for the combined sample or for either treatment. Correlational Analyses 27 Correlations calculated between residual gains in the forgiveness and letting go related variables, and symptom variables are displayed in Table 5. Pre-post changes were calculated as the standardized residuals by regressing initial scores onto final scores for all the symptom and outcome measures (Cronbach & Furby, 1970). As can be seen in the table, an increase in forgiveness and empathy and acceptance, and of feelings and needs all were related to a decrease in overall symptom distress Discussion The results of this study showed that clients in the individual EFT treatment showed significantly more improvement than those in the PG treatment on all measures of forgiveness, on measures assessing the degree to which clients had let go of distressing feelings and unmet needs in relation to the injurer and on target complaints at termination and follow-up. The two groups however were found to not differ significantly on the change in feelings and needs at termination, when possible dependence in the PG group was taken into account. Greater improvement also was reported in the EFT condition on the Global Symptom Index of the SCL90-R. There however was no difference between groups on the BDI, but given clients in this study were not depressed this is not surprising. The significant change in general psychological symptoms on the SCL 90-R in EFT, with no change in PG on symptoms, is notable, suggesting that an individual emotion-focused treatment in addition to enhancing forgiveness and letting go of the emotional injury, has positive effects on a person’s general level of well being that exceed that of a psycho-education group. Taken as a whole, this study provides support for the differential effectiveness of an EFT approach for promoting forgiveness, resolving emotional injuries and reducing general symptoms over a PG treatment intervention of the same duration. 28 In addition all those clients who reported that they forgave the injurer, regardless of group, also indicated that they had let go of the distressing feelings and unmet needs previously associated with the injury. In contrast, there were some individuals who indicated that they had let go of the distressing feelings and unmet needs associated with the injury, but had not forgiven the injurer. This suggests that letting go of persisting unresolved feelings such as anger, grief, sadness and/or hurt may be a necessary step in resolving past interpersonal hurtfulness, but may not be sufficient nor equivalent to forgiveness. Further research on this question is needed but seeing that the process of reducing un-forgiveness or letting go of bad feelings is distinct from the process of generating positive feelings, such as loving kindness, may be useful ones when examining the process of forgiveness and the possibility of the resolution of an emotional injury in the absence of forgiveness. Contrary to what the forgiveness literature suggests (McCullough & Witvliet, 2001, Orcutt 2006), forgiveness was not initially correlated with the pre-treatment variables measuring emotional health such as the BDI and particularly the Global Symptom Index (GSI) of the SCL90R. Thus clients at the start of treatment who were more forgiving were not psychologically healthier. This however may have been due to lack of range on both the forgiveness measure and the GSI and BDI and because participants were seeking help for presenting problems involving a lack of letting go and forgiveness. In our study pre-post increases in forgiveness and letting go variables however were related to a decrease in GSI. Thus, it seems that increases over treatment in forgiveness, in empathy and acceptance, toward the injurer, and being able to resolve feelings and needs, were related to improved health as measured by the GSI suggesting health benefits to resolving injuries both by forgiveness and letting go. Recently Orcutt (2006) found that offencespecific forgiveness at initial assessment correlated significantly with symptom distress 29 approximately 9 months later controlling for symptoms distress at initial assessment, and that time since offence mediated the relationship between forgiveness and symptom distress, suggesting a complex relationship between forgiveness and symptoms. Data on our treatment seeking population does not support that forgiveness correlates with distress at initial assessment but does show that increase in forgiveness certainly correlates with reduction in distress. In addition to showing better outcomes, the EFT group was found to involve more clientreported emotional arousal, especially of negative emotions in the mid-phase of treatment, confirming that the EFT treatment was more emotion- arousing than the PG treatment. The finding that there was more negative emotional arousal in the early phase of therapy for the EFT treatment than for the PG treatment was probably because EFT clients were more likely to access negative emotions right from the start as a function of therapy, rather than because they entered treatment with more negative emotions than clients in the PG treatment. This finding also suggests than the emotional change processes in therapy are not necessarily simply ones of replacing bad feelings with good feelings in a linear process but that at times feeling bad can in fact lead to feeling good. Thus working through bad feelings by facing them, allowing and accepting them, can lead to change (Greenberg 2002). It should be noted however that reported in-session emotional arousal did not relate to outcome in either group. This is likely because not all arousal of emotion is the same. For example some arousal may be a sign of distress rather than a sign of working through distress (Greenberg & Watson, 2006). Recently Greenberg, Auzra and Hermann (2007) showed that arousal alone is not necessarily a measure of productive emotional processing and that it is productive processing of arousal emotion that best discriminates good from poor outcome cases. In other studies the intensity of observed expressed emotional arousal however has been shown to predict outcome in the working phase of the EFT 30 treatment of depression (Missirilian, Toukmanian, Warwar & Greenberg, 2005; Warwar, 2003). The difference in the finding on the relationship between emotional arousal and outcome in this study to the finding in the studies on depression, if they are not because of differences in a depressed and an emotional injury population, may be explained by the differences between the nature of the experience of emotion, as measured by post session self-reports of emotional arousal in the session, and the expression of emotions, captured by observational measures of arousal. In the present study we observed that the session in which some clients reported high emotional intensity in the session (experienced emotion) showed few visible signs of the reported arousal (expression) and in fact often the clients were quite constricted in their expression of emotion in the session. They appeared to have felt a lot of anger or sadness but not necessarily expressed it, so although the self reports were indicative of emotion experienced, they were not indices of how much emotion was allowed and expressed in the session and made accessible for further processing. As Greenberg (2007) has suggested emotion awareness and expression are different emotion change processes and the latter, by revealing the self to the other, overcoming constriction and altering physiology and neurochemistry may be what is most therapeutic when dealing with unresolved painful emotions. Limitations The clients in this study were volunteers who responded to advertisements, and therefore may not be representative of the general population seeking help for emotional injuries. Furthermore, equivalence of type and intensity of injuries was not taken into consideration in assigning clients to groups, based on the assumption that this would be handled by randomization, but in small samples such as this, this may not be the case. In addition, resolution or forgiveness was not assessed in relation to whether the injured person still had an 31 ongoing relationship with the injurer, (as in the case with a living parent versus a deceased parent) or not. It also would have been preferable to insure a balance of men and women in both groups. To the extent that there were more women in EFT, this may have introduced a confound. Taking all these factors into consideration will require further research with a larger sample of clients. Because therapist and researcher allegiance to treatment model (and their possible ability to persuade clients of the relevance of the treatment process to their presenting problems) is a common factor known to improve outcomes, regardless of specific treatment models employed, one might attribute observed differences in this study to therapist training/allegiance, or researcher allegiance, rather than differences in specific treatment techniques. The EFT therapists had at least 1 year of experience with EFT, and received an additional 30 hr of training in this specific intervention prior to participation in the study. Similar training did not occur for the PG treatment leaders although the one leader was the developer of the group Psychoeducation treatment. Investigators were proponents of EFT and this too could influence results. In addition differences between factors operating in group and individual therapy modalities could account for the differences in effectiveness of treatment. Clients receiving individual therapy receive a treatment tailored to their needs, whereas in groups there is less flexibility and individualization. Spacing of sessions also was different in the two treatments. Individual therapy met weekly whereas the group sessions were twice the length of individual sessions, but met only once every two weeks. This could provide more continuity of treatment and support in the individual therapy. On the other hand group participants benefit from other factors in groups that promote change that are not available to the individual participants, such as group support and a sense of the universality of their problems and having two therapists. 32 References Akhtar, S. (2002). Forgiveness: Origins, dynamics, psychopathology, and technical relevance. Psychoanalytic Quarterly, 71, 175-212. Al-Mabuk, R. H., Enright, R. D. & Cardis, P. A. (1995). Forgiveness education with parentally love-deprived late adolescents. Journal of Moral Education, 24 (4), 427-444. Battle, C., Imber, S., Hoehn-Saric, R., Stone, A., Nash, C., & Frank, J. (1968). Target complaints as criteria of improvements. American Journal of Psychotherapy, 20, 184-192. Baures, M. M. (1996). Letting go of bitterness and hate. Journal of Humanistic Psychology, 36(1), 75-90. Berecz, J. M. (2001). All that glitters is not gold: Bad forgiveness in counseling and preaching. Pastoral Psychology, 49, 253-275. Beck, A.T., Ward, C.H., Mendelson, M., Mock, J., & Rebaugh, J. (1961). An inventory for measuring depression. Archives of General Psychiatry, 4, 561-571. Beck, A.T., Steer, R.A. & Garbin, M.G. (1988). Psychometric properties of the Beck Depression Inventory: Twenty-five years of evaluation, Clinical Psychology Review, 8, 77-100. Bolger, E. (1999). Grounded theory analysis of emotional pain. Psychotherapy Research, 9(3), 342-362. Boss, J. (1997). Throwing pearls to the swine: Women, forgiveness, and the unrepentant abuser. In L.D. Kaplan & L.F. Bove (Eds.) Philosophical perspectives on power and domination: Theories and practices (pp. 235-247). Amsterdam-Atlanta, GA: Rodopi. Coyle, C. T., & Enright, R. D. (1997). Forgiveness intervention with postabortion men. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 65, 1042-1046. 33 Cronbach, L. J., & Furby, L. (1970). How we should measure “change” - Or should we? Psychological Bulletin, 74, 68-80. Derogatis, L. R. (1983). SCL-90-R administration, scoring, and procedures manual for the revised version. Towson, MD: Clinical Psychiatric Research. Derogatis, L.R., Rickels, K., & Roch, A.F. (1976). The SCL-90 and the MMPI: A step in the validation of a new self-report Scale. British Journal of Psychiatry, 128, 280-289 Davenport, D.S. (1991). The functions of anger and forgiveness: Guidelines for psychotherapy with victims. Psychotherapy, 28, 140-144. Enright, R. D., & Eastin, D. L. (1992). Interpersonal forgiveness within the helping professions: An attempt to resolve differences of opinion. Counseling and Values, 36, 84-103. Enright, R. D., & Fitzgibbons, R. P. (2000). Helping clients forgive: An empirical guide for resolving anger and restoring hope. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association Books. Enright, R. D., Rique, J., & Coyle, C. T. (2000). The Enright Forgiveness Inventory (EFI) user's manual. Madison: The International Forgiveness Institute. Fincham, F. D. (2000). The kiss of the porcupines: From attributing responsibility to forgiving. Personal Relationships, 7, 1-23. Fitzgibbons, R. P. (1986). The cognitive and emotive uses of forgiveness in the treatment of anger. Psychotherapy, 23, 629-633. Fredrickson, B. L. (1998). What good are positive emotions? Review of General Psychology: Special issue: New Directions in Research on emotion, 2, 300-319. Fredericson, B., Mancuso, R., Branigan, C. & Tugade, M. (2000). The undoing effect of positive emotions. Motivation and Emotion, 23, 237-258. 34 Freedman, S. R., & Enright, R. D. (1996). Forgiveness as an intervention goal with incest survivors. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 64, 983-992. Gendlin, E. T. (1996). Focusing-oriented psychotherapy: A manual of the experiential method. New York: Guilford Press. Greenberg, L. (2004). Emotion-focused therapy. Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy (special issue), 11, 3-16. Greenberg, L. S. (2002). Emotion-focused therapy: Coaching clients to work through feelings. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association Press. Greenberg, L. S. Auzra, & Hermann, Greenberg, L. S., & Paivio, S. C. (1997). Working with emotions in psychotherapy. New York, NY: Guilford Press. Greenberg L.S, & Rosenberg R. (2002). Therapist experience of empathy: In J. C. Watson, R.N. Goldman & M. Warner (Eds.). Client-centred and experiential psychotherapy in the 21st century: Advances in theory, research and practice. PCCS Books. Ross-on Wye. Herefordshire Greenberg, L. S., & Safran, J. D. (1987). Emotion in psychotherapy: Affect, cognition and the process of change. New York: Guilford. Greenberg, L.S., Malcolm, W. M., & Warwar, S.H., (2002). Emotion Focused Therapy Manual for Resolving Interpersonal Emotional Injuries. Unpublished Manual. Toronto, Ontario: York University. Greenberg, L. S., Rice, L. N. & Elliot, R. (1993). Facilitating emotional change: The moment by moment process. NY: Guilford Press. 35 Harris, A.H.S., & Thoresen, C.E. (2005). Forgiveness, Unforgiveness, Health, and Disease, In Everett L. Worthington, Jr. (Ed.), Handbook of forgiveness. (pp.321-333). New York, NY: Routledge. Hebl, J. H., & Enright, R. D. (1993). Forgiveness as a psychotherapeutic goal with elderly females. Psychotherapy, 30 (4), 658-667. Hope, D. (1987). The healing paradox of forgiveness. Psychotherapy, 24, 240-244. Horvath, A., & Greenberg, L. S. (1989). Development and Validation of the Working Alliance Inventory, Journal of Counseling Psychology, 36 (2), 223-233. Karen, R. (2001). The forgiving self: The road from resentment to connection. New York: Double Day. Kenny,D.(1995). The effect of nonindependence on significance testing in dyadic research Personal Relationships, 2 67-75 Klassen, D.(2001). The Forgiveness Workbook. Thunder Bay, ON: The Listening Centre. Macaskill, A., Maltby, J., & Day, L. (2002). Forgiveness of self and others and emotional empathy. Journal of Social Psychology, 142, 663-665. Malcolm, W. M. (1999). Relating process to outcome in the process-experiential resolution of unfinished business. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. Toronto, ON: York University. Malcolm, W. M., Warwar, S.H., & Greenberg, L.S. (2005). Facilitating forgiveness in individual therapy as an approach to resolving interpersonal injuries. In Everett L. Worthington, Jr. (Ed.), Handbook of forgiveness. (pp.379-391). New York, NY: Routledge. McCullough, M.E., & Witvliet, C.V.O. (2001). The psychology of forgiveness. In C.R. Snyder and S. Lopez (Eds.), Handbook of positive psychology (pp. 446-458). New York: Oxford. 36 McCullough, M. E., Fincham, F. D., & Tsang, J. (2003). Forgiveness, forbearance, and time: The temporal unfolding of transgression-related interpersonal motivations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 540-557. McCullough, M. E., Rachal, K. C., Sandage, S. J., Worthington, E. L. Jr. (1997,August). A sustainable future for the psychology of forgiveness. Paper presented at the meeting of the American Psychological Association, Chicago, IL. McCullough, M. E., Worthington, E. L., Jr., & Rachal, K. C. (1997). Interpersonal forgiving in close relationships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 321-336. Missirlian, T., Toukmanian, S., Warwar, S. &. Greenberg, L. (2005). Emotional Arousal, Client Perceptual Processing, and the Working Alliance in Experiential Psychotherapy for Depression. Journal of Consulting & Clinical Psychology. 73 (5), 861-871. Orcutt, H. K. (2006). The prospective relationship of interpersonal forgiveness and psychological distress symptoms among college women. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 53, 350-361. Paivio, S.C., & Greenberg, L.S. (1995). Resolving “unfinished business.” Efficacy of experiential therapy using empty-chair dialogue. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 63, 419-425. Paivio, S. C., Hall, I. E., Holowaty, K.A.M., Jellis, J. B., & Tran, N. (2001). Imaginal confrontation for resolving child abuse issues. Psychotherapy Research,11, 433-453. Paivio, S.C. & Nieuwenhuis, J. A. (2001). Efficacy of emotionally focused therapy for adult survivors of child abuse: A preliminary study. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 14, 115-134. 37 Raudenbush, S.W., & Bryk, A.S. (2002). Hierarchical linear models: Applications and data analysis methods (2nd ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Ripley, J. & Worthington, E. (2002). Hope-focused and forgiveness-focused group interventions to promote marital enrichment. Journal of Counseling and Development, 80, 452 – 463. Singh, M. (1994). Validation of a measure of session outcome in the resolution of unfinished business. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. Toronto, Ontario: Department of Psychology, York University. Shrout, P.E., & Fleiss, J.L. (1979). Intraclass correlations: Uses in assessing rater reliability. Psychological Bulletin, 86, 420-428. Skinner, C. J., Holt, D., & Smith, T. M. F. (1989). Analysis of complex surveys. West Sussex, England: Wiley. Smedes, L.B. (1984). Forgive and forget: Healing the hurts we don’t deserve. San Francisco: Harper San Francisco. Spitzer, R. L., Williams, J. B., Gibbon, M., & First, M. B. (1990). Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-III-R Personality Disorders (SCID-II). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press. Wade, N. G., & Worthington, E. L., Jr. (2003). Overcoming interpersonal offenses: Is forgiveness the only way to deal with unforgiveness? Journal of Counseling & Development, 81, 343-353. Wade, N. G., & Worthington, E. L., Jr. ,& Meyer, J.E. (2005). In Everett L. Worthington, Jr. (Ed.), Handbook of forgiveness. (pp.423-439). New York, NY: Routledge. Warwar, S. (2003). Relating emotional processes to outcome in experiential psychotherapy of depression. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, York University, Toronto, Ontario, Canada. 38 Warwar, S., & Greenberg, L. S. (1999). Client Emotional Arousal Scale-III. Unpublished manuscript, York Psychotherapy Research Clinic, York University, Toronto, Ontario. Warwar, S., & Greenberg, L. (2002). The Emotional Arousal Session Report Measure. Unpublished: York University, North York, Ontario, Canada. Germany. Watson, J. & Greenberg, L. (1996). Pathways to change in the Psychotherapy of Depression: Relating Process-to Session Change and Outcome. Psychotherapy, 33, (2), 262-274. Worthington, E.L., Jr. (1998). The pyramid model of forgiveness: Some interdisciplinary speculations about unforgiveness and the promotion of forgiveness. In Everett L. Worthington, Jr. (Ed.), Dimensions of forgiveness: Psychological research and theological perspectives (pp. 107-138). Philadelphia, PA: Templeton Foundation Press. Worthington, E. L., Jr. (2001). Five steps to forgiveness: The art and science of forgiving. New York: Crown Publishing. Worthington, E. L., Jr. (2005). Initial questions about the art and science of forgiving. In Everett L. Worthington, Jr. (Ed.), Handbook of forgiveness. (pp.1-13). New York, NY: Routledge. Worthington, E. L., Jr., & Drinkard, D. T. (2000). Promoting reconciliation through psychoeducational and therapeutic interventions. Journal of Marital and Family Therapy, 26 (1), 93-101. Worthington, E. L., Jr., & Wade, N. G. (1999). The psychology of unforgiveness and forgiveness and implications for clinical practice. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 18, 385418. 39 Worthington, E. L., Jr., Sandage, S.J., & Berry, J.W. (2000). Group interventions to promote forgiveness: What researchers and clinicians ought to know. In M.E. McCullough, K.I. Pargament, & C.E. Thoresen (Eds.), Forgiveness: Theory, research, and practice (pp.228-253). New York: Guilford Press. 40 Authors Footnotes 1. Readers can request a complete matrix of intercorrelations between all variables by writing to the first author at lgrnberg@yorku.ca. 2. In order to test for possible dependence in the data Intra Class correlations (ICC) were computed on the output from two separate one-way ANOVAs on the effect of the therapists in the EFT condition and of the 3 groups in the Psycho-Ed condition. ICC measures the relative homogeneity within groups in comparison to the between group variation. ICC is large and positive when there is no variation within the groups, but group means differ. It will be at its largest negative value when group means are the same but there is great variation within groups. A negative ICC occurs when between-group variation is less than within-group variation. In this situation, checking for independence, we would want the ICC to be close to 0 or negative, indicating that group mean differences are negligible relative to individual differences i.e., the grouping doesn’t matter. ICC recently is used in the context of hierarchical linear modeling (HLM) to measure the extent to which data clustering (i.e., non-independence of observations) is present. If the ICC is near 0, then HLM will give very similar results to a traditional ANOVA or regression that assumes independence (Raudenbush & Bryk, 2002, Shrout, & Fleiss1979). ICCs on post test were found to be low, below .08 or negative for both treatment conditions for all of the dependent variables except for Feelings and Needs (ICC=.15), in the PG group. This provides evidence that the grouping of clients within therapists and within groups was not a strong factor influencing the findings on the majority of measures in the study except possibly for the Feelings and need scale. Fs were adjusted according to the design effect, which is a function of ICC and sample size (Skinner, Holt, & Smith, 1989) for post test scores on the Feelings and need scale and no significant difference between groups was found at post 41 F(1,43)=2.65, p=.112 thereby altering the conclusion of difference on this measure. There was no difference in the between group comparisons on the other measures in which ICC had been lower than .08 42 Table 1 Interpersonal Emotional Injuries Reported by Client Sample ___________________________________________________________________ EFT PG ___________________________________________________________________ Issue Betrayal 5 8 Criticism 6 4 Neglect 4 3 Abandonment 5 4 Physical abuse 2 3 Sexual abuse 1 1 23 23 Total Significant other Both Parents 5 5 Mother 8 4 Father 5 4 Ex-partner 2 5 Sibling 2 2 Child 0 1 Other 1 2 23 23 Total ____________________________________________________________ Note. Each participant targeted 1 injury only 43 Table 2 Pre, Post and Follow-up Means and Standard Deviations of Outcome Measures Measure Enright Forgiveness Pre Post EFT PG EFT (n = 23) (n = 23) (n = 23) M SD M SD 199.22 197.09 60.58 58.14 Follow-up EFT PG (n = 23) (n = 23) (n = 23) M SD M SD M SD M SD 261.13 237.30 277.52 243.43 51.28 51.41 55.01 2.87 3.83 3.43 47.24 PG Ancova F(1,43) 4.98* Inventory Forgiveness Measure UFB Empathy and Acceptance UFB Feelings and Needs1 Letting Go 2.18 .72 14.04 4.43 14.87 5.06 2.00 .71 1 See footnote 2 2.39 .78 14.26 4.97 17.48 4.63 2.17 .94 3.74 .81 22.17 5.42 30.95 6.0 4.09** .79 1.06 17.78 5.48 25.58 8.35. 2.17 .98 .75 22.13 4.38 31.32 6.56 4.06 .71 7.91** 1.01 18.87 9.28** 4.78 28.05 4.32* 7.15 3.42 .76 437.69** 44 Table 2 (continued) Measure Pre Post Follow-up M SD M SD M SD M SD M SD M SD 10.48 10.07 4.40 6.48 4.61 6.22 Ancova TC Discomfort 1.53 1.64 -- -- -- -- .70 .58 2.25 3.03 2.41 7.03* 2.56 TC Change 7.16 1.64 5.39 1.99 7.70 6.3 1.29 1.91 .35 .45 12.67** GSI .43 .36 .46* .47 .60 .45 .31 6.73* .32 BDI 12.18 10.32 10.79 6.34 Note. Pretreatment scores used as covariates *p < .05, **p < .01 7.26 7.59 6.39 5.88 6.79 7.13 5.47 5.43 1.1 45 Table 3: Frequency of Forgiveness and Letting Go* Let Go EFT PG % of Total Yes No Yes No Yes 9 0 4 0 No 5 9 3 16 Yes 28.26% No 0 Forgive Total 30.43% 19.56% 15.21% 34.78% 17.39% 54.34% 45.65% 54.34% Note: A Chi square analysis of the distribution comparing forgivers and those who let go was significant, X = 21.5 p<.001. 46 Table 4 In Session Emotion Clusters by Phase of Therapy: PG and EFT ________________________________________________________________________ Phase of Therapy Emotion Early Middle Late PG EFT PG EFT PG EFT (N=23) (N=23) (N=23) (N=23) (N=23) (N=23) ________________________________________________________________________ Positive Emotions M SD 3.03 1.17 2.97 3.62** 2.73 4.17 3.82 1.16 1.26 1.23 1.53 1.44 3.58* 2.67 3.50** 2.11 2.60 Negative Emotions M 2.82 SD 1.40 0.91 1.12 0.98 1.04 1.09 ________________________________________________________________________ Asterisks indicate between group differences * p<.05 **p<.01 47 Table 5: Correlations between Pre-Post Residual Gain Scores On Forgiveness, Letting go and Symptom Variables(n=46): GSI BDI Enright Forgiveness Inventory -.41** -.08 Forgiveness Measure -.36* -.22 UFB Empathy & Acceptance -.37* .05 UFB Feelings & Needs -.38** -.29 Letting Go Measure -.23 -.17 Target Complaints (Discomfort) .27 .14 * p < .05, ** p <.01 48 Table 2 Pre, Post and Follow-up Means and Standard Deviations of Outcome Measures Measure Enright Forgiveness Pre Post EFT PG EFT (n = 23) (n = 23) (n = 23) M SD M SD 199.22 197.09 60.58 58.14 Follow-up PG Ancova EFT PG (n = 23) (n = 23) (n = 23) M SD M SD M SD M SD 261.13* 237.30 277.52 243.43 47.24 51.28 51.41 55.01 2.87 3.83 3.43 F(1,43) 4.98 Inventory Forgiveness Measure UFB Empathy and Acceptance UFB Feelings and Needs2 Letting Go 2.18 .72 14.04 4.43 14.87 5.06 2.00 .71 2 See footnote 2 2.39 .78 14.26 4.97 17.48 4.63 2.17 .94 3.74** .81 22.17** 5.42 30.95* 6.0 4.09** .79 1.06 17.78 5.48 25.58 8.35. 2.17 .98 .75 22.13 4.38 31.32 6.56 4.06 .71 7.91 1.01 18.87 9.28 4.78 28.05 4.32 7.15 3.42 .76 437.69 49 Table 2 (continued) Measure Pre Post Follow-up M SD M SD M SD M SD M SD M SD 10.48 10.07 4.40 6.48 4.61* 6.22 Ancova TC Discomfort 1.53 1.64 -- -- 2.25 3.03 2.41 7.03 2.56 TC Change 7.16 5.39 7.70** 6.3 12.67 -- -- .70 .58 1.64 1.99 1.29 1.91 .35 .45 GSI .43 .36 .46* .47 .60 .45 .31 6.73 .32 BDI 12.18 10.32 10.79 6.34 Note. Pretreatment scores used as covariates *p < .05, **p < .01 7.26 7.59 6.39 5.88 6.79 7.13 5.47 5.43 1.1 50 Figure 1. Emotional Arousal Across Phases 4.5 ◦ 4.0 □ Means ◦ □ □ 3.5 ○ 3.0 □ ○ □ ◦ 2.5 ◦ 2.0 Early Middle Phase of Therapy ◦ □ PG EFT Negative Emotion Positive Emotion Late