Language/culture socialization An anthropological perspective • Linguistics as a field is concerned primarily with the analytical study of a language to reveal its structure. • Psycholinguistics is the study of the psychological and neurobiological factors that enable humans to acquire, use, comprehend and produce language. • Linguistic anthropology is most concerned with the relationship between language, worldview, culture, and social relationships, etc. Linguists, psycholinguists, and linguistic anthropologists have all studied how children learn language and the patterns of language acquisition and competence. Different from linguists and psycholinguists, linguistic anthropologists are often concerned with how children in various cultures learn language and how they acquire their particular cultural attributes through the process of learning language. Linguistic anthropologists often conduct what are called language socialization studies (Don Kulick & Bambi Schieffelin: Language Socialization). 1 In the past, linguists conducted most of their language-learning studies among white middle class North American and northern European children. These children generally shared the linguistic and sociocultural backgrounds of the researchers who were studying them. Culture was, for the most part, invisible to these researchers, who erroneously concluded that children everywhere acquired language in the same ways. (Don Kulick & Bambi Schieffelin: Language Socialization) There are some basic patterns, but Anthropologists have studied children learning language all over the world. Anthropologists see that how children are socialized to learn language and through language varies from culture to culture. Language socialization paradigm • Insists that in becoming competent members of their social group, children are socialized through language • Children are socialized to use language • So language is not just one dimension of the socialization process • It is the most central and crucial dimension of that process 2 The language socialization paradigm claims that: Any study of socialization that does not document the role of language … in the acquisition of cultural practices … is incomplete and fundamentally flawed. As background, past linguistic and psycholinguistic studies assumed that cultural competence was complete after adolescence. In comparison: • Language socialization studies emphasize the socializing nature of all human interaction. We know now that we are socialized all of our lives to use language and to understand the world through language. Our various subcultures, professions, educational emphases and areas of expertise all influence the language categories we use to describe and perceive our relationships, experiences and beliefs. 3 Multiple agencies are present and should be accounted for in any social interaction Mother child interactions • It is not only the child who is being socialized • The child, through her/his actions and verbalizations, is also actively socializing the mother as a mother Co-workers socialize each other as co-workers Lovers socialize each other as lovers The goal in language socialization studies is to understand how people come to be culturally intelligible subjects (that is people who understand a cultural way of being). The main question here is ‘how’? 4 Below are a few classic examples of case studies to show what anthropologists mean by learning culture while learning language. Don Kulick studied infant caregivers in the Papua New Guinean village of Gapun. In this village, people assume that infants are naturally willful and stubborn. People are more likely to interpret infants’ behaviors as expressing dissatisfaction and anger. “A child cooing softly in its mother’s lap is likely to be shaken suddenly and asked ‘Ai! What are you mad about!’ The first words attributed to a baby are oki, mnda and ayata – words which mean ‘I’m leaving’, ‘I’m sick of this’ and ‘Stop it’, respectively. If a mother notices a baby reaching out towards a dog, she won’t tell the child to pet it. Instead, voicing what she takes to be the child’s inner state, the mother asserts, ‘Look, she’s mad now, she wants to hit the dog’, and she moves closer to the animal, thrusting out the child’s hand onto the dog’s fur, encouraging her. ‘That’s it, hit it! Hit it’! Imputed aggression in babies is frequently matched by anyone tending them, and the most common mode of face play with babies involves the caregiver biting her lower lip, widening her eyes, thrusting out her chin sharply, and raising the heel of her hand in a threatening manner, swinging it to within a few inches of the child’s face and then suddenly pulling it back again. After pulling several of these punches, the woman or man doing this laughs at the baby and nuzzles its face and body with her or his lips (Kulick 1992:99-104). - These kinds of routine caregiver-child interactions foreground anger as an emotion of important consequence. 5 - In Gapun, anger is a structuring principle of social life. - For example, anger is a crucial component of the village’s gender division. - Women are held to be selfish and always ready to publicly vent their anger. - Men are expected to suppress anger for the greater social good. - The conviction is held that to impose on another person and make them angry may cause them, their ancestors, or the supernatural entities associated with their land to act by causing someone to sicken or die (Kulick 1992, 1993, 1998). The Kaluli are another group in Papua New Guinea. Their beliefs about the nature of children differ from beliefs in Gapun. Instead adults assume that infants are ‘soft’ or helpless, vulnerable, and without understanding. The first sounds produced by children that are recognized by adults as Kaluli words are no: and bo, ‘mother’ and ‘breast’. “These words are significant in that they attest to a social view of language expressing the child’s primary relationship and the giving of food that is central to its constitution. Kaluli say that children know how to beg and whine to get what they want, but they must be taught to use language. Kaluli use no baby talk or simplified speech when communicating with their infants. Instead, they socialize young children by telling them precisely what to say and how to say it. - Kaluli children are understood to need to acquire assertive demeanors. 6 - Adults often pay little attention to what the young child might want to say or do. - Mothers make extensive use of the ‘say it’ routine to direct young children to request, command, tease and challenge. - They model what they want the child to say and how it should be said. - The child needs to learn how to go from ‘soft’ to ‘hard’ in his or her speech. - Hardness signifies verbal and social competence, which includes being able to verbally demand what one desires. - Once children are able to demand, they may be asked to give and share and enter into the system of reciprocity that defines Kaluli social life and relationships. - In this society it is important to know when to demand and when to appeal to others to feel sorry and give. The Gapun and Kaluli examples demonstrate how children learn to be appropriate members of their culture group. They learn how to be Kaluli or Gapun through the social frames that surround learning language. Another question might be how do children learn to be inappropriate members of their culture group? 7 How do some people become ‘bad’ and some ‘good’ relative to their culture groups? “Good” subjects are those members of a culture group who learn and adhere correctly to socialization. Don Kulick and Bambi Schieffelin (linguistic anthropologists) ask what happens when language socialization results in unexpected outcomes such as resistance. • To answer questions of how people become good or bad in relation to cultural expectations involves documenting how certain children or novices come to be what could be called ‘bad’ subjects. • (Not that they are inherently ‘bad’, necessarily, except that they do not respond to calls to behave in particular socially sanctioned ways.) Examples of ‘bad’ subjects • In societies that value sensitivity, ‘bad’ subjects might be people who don’t stop talking about themselves. 8 • In societies that stress generosity, ‘bad’ subjects might be people who are selfish. • In societies that value cooperation, they might be people who behave in criminal ways. • Traditionally, in psychology, people who didn’t conform were called ‘deviants’. • They were seen as the product of individual psychological processes. • They were thought to be socialized through negligence, abuse or idiosyncratic caregiving strategies. Some of these socialization issues are valid, but Kulick and Schieffelin say that to particularize (and individualize) even a case as serious as a serial killer is to ignore the fact that these are cultural products. First, serial-rapist murderers do not exist everywhere, as culturally imaginable subjectivity or as an actual occurring type of person. 9 Second, they are not exclusively children of negligent or abusive parents. Even when there are biological components to 'badness' the expresion of badness and manifestation of badness often differs from culture group to culture group. For example, scizhophrenia manifests itself differently from culture group to culture group, most likely because of contrasting socialization of language and culture, as well as values, beliefs and expectations of groups. To better understand how cultural patterns manifest into different expressions of badness or goodness, it is important to study the messages children learn about being good and bad in their various cultural settings. Basically when children are learning how to be good, they are also learning the inverse, or how to be bad, relative to their culture group. What is it about the ways that language socialization occurs that contributes to a type of badness that manifests itself in serial rapist murder in some cultures but not others? While we may not yet know the answer to that specific question, cultural analysis is crucial to better understanding types of ‘badness’ according to Kulick and Schiefflen. Again, remember that children receive socializing messages about how to behave and feel in particular ways. These messages also produce their own inversion. 10 Each time that an adult tells a child how to speak politely, the adult is also indicating how to speak rudely. • “You must say please” • “Don’t say that word.” • These commands tell the child what rudeness is. • The commands also point to the forbidden phrases. • In teaching politeness, adults provide a model of rudeness. • The socialization processes teaches what to repress and what to express. • What is repressed and expressed is relative to different culture groups. To understand how a culture’s version of badness is taught requires researchers to take a close look at how adults communicate with infants and children. 11 • Language socialization studies include exploration of the ways in which utterances manifest ‘dual indexicality’ …their surface content and the simultaneous inverse of that content Examples: A study in comparisons Patricia Clancey working with mothers and two-year-olds in Japan In her study of Japanese two-year-olds with their mothers, Clancey wanted to find out how children came to learn ideal Japanese communication styles that include being indirect and intuitive in conversations and mannerisms. She found that as children learn culture (through learning language) their mothers practice certain socialization routines. For example: - If a child is behaving inappropriately his mother might attribute feelings and speech to other people asking, “How is your grandfather feeling about what he sees you doing?” This tells the child how to read non-verbal behavior and to consider how other people might be feeling about his behavior. - The child’s mother might appeal to hito, ‘other people’, who are supposedly always watching and evaluating the child’s behavior. - The child’s mother might use strong affect-laden adjectives like ‘scary’ or ‘frightening’ to describe the child’s misbehavior. This makes it clear that the behavior is unacceptable and shameful. 12 “These kinds of communicative interactions sensitized children to subtle interactional expectations which in adult interactions are not expressed explicitly. They also encouraged children to acquire the specific anxieties and fears (such as the disapproval of hito) that undergird Japanese communicative style” (Kulick & Schieffelin, 10). In these types of socializing experiences, what do children learn are the reasons for being good? U.S. example Desirable leads to Desirable Undesirable leads to Undesirable In a study of common (but not universal) North American ways of communicating with children about good and bad behavior, adults use more conditional language, “If you clean your room, you can watch cartoons.” They also use more threats and warnings than do many Japanese mothers. A child who is afraid of a vacuum cleaner sound is told by her mother, “I won’t put the vacuum cleaner on if you drink all your juice.” • (Desirable for the child to desirable for the mother) 13 A father warns his child, “If I see you with matches, I’ll give you a spanking.” • (Undesirable behavior of child to undesirable behavior toward child) In these types of socializing experiences, what do children learn are the reasons for being good? English speaking adults: Desirability links: To control child’s behavior, adults link actions that are desirable for the adult to actions that the adult frames as desirable to the child. Undesirability links: • English speakers often use conditionals, promises, threats, and warnings. These are presented to children with an explicit reason for complying with the directive. • The reason is linked to consequences that the child would face (I’ll give you a spanking). • This way focuses on the child being good for the sake of avoiding punishment or to receive rewards. • (Is the inverse being bad to get away with it, expecting rewards for good behavior, etc.?) • 14 Japanese and Korean adults mostly do not present children with this kind of information. • They rely more on general statements (it won’t do … it’s scary) These do not assert what will happen if the child does or does not follow to the adult’s command to do, or stop doing, something. • This way focuses on the child learning self control for the sake of others’ welfare and opinions • • (Is the inverse fear of loss of self control and fear of other people’s opinions?) Questions for further study: • In learning how to be good through threats, warnings, and rewards, what is the inverse of such socialization? • In what ways do many North American people express ‘badness’ because they learned to be ‘bad’ through rewards, threats and warnings? 15