Investigating Conceptions of Math by the Use of Open-ended Mat Problems, presented as ICME-9 TSG11 N.Y. Wong Investigating Conceptions of Mathematics by the Use of Open-ended Mathematical Problems (*) Ngai-Ying Wong (nywong@cuhk.edu.hk) Department of Curriculum and Instruction, The Chinese University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong 1. Introduction The research team (#) started off the investigation as a curriculum issue. Students’ conception of mathematics was taken as part of the “attained curriculum” (learning outcome), a result of the “intended” and “implemented” curricula (Travers & Westbury, 1989). First of all, Asian students in general and Hong Kong students in particular outperformed their Western counterparts in international comparisons in mathematics, (Cai, 1995; Leung and Wong, 1996, 1997, Leung et al, 1999; Stevenson & Lee, 1990), but we suspected that they may lack conceptual understanding. As one facet of it, we suspected that students only possess a narrow conception of mathematics. Secondly, the mathematics curriculum was under reform in which the author was actively involved. As a first step, we need to make a full situational appraisal, to identify the strength and weaknesses of the current curriculum. There, students’ conception of mathematics was again one of the foci of investigation (Wong et al, 1999). Thirdly, we made some tried out the use of history mathematics in teaching (Lit & Siu, 1998). To evaluate the effectiveness of such a curriculum, we saw that conventional tests do not suffice. We incorporated assessments in the affective domain, including attitude and beliefs. All these led us to a more in depth investigation on the conceptions of mathematics. At the same time, we realised that a well-established methodology for such a series of studies may be lacking. Numerous studies have revealed that beliefs about mathematics influence students’ motivation to learn and their performance in the subject (Cobb, 1985; McLeod, 1992; Underhill, 1988). Mathematics is often regarded as an absolute truth (Fleener, 1996) and is seen as a set of rules (Carpernter, et al,1983; Clay & Kolb, 1983; Frank, 1988; Confrey, 1983; Dossey et al, 1988; Kolb, 1983; Kloosterman, 1991; Lester & Garofalo, 1982; McLeod, 1992; Peck, 1984; Wong, 1993, 1995). With such a conception, to the students, knowing mathematics is done through memorisation and algorithms and learning is a transmission process (Underhill, 1988). 2. Initial research Relationships between belief in mathematics and learning outcome or problem solving behaviour have been studied too (Kloosterman, 1991; Kloosterman & Stage, 1992, Stage & Kloosterman, 1995, Schoenfeld, 1989). However, most of these studies rely on one-way responses of the students and lack the chance of clarifying what is in the minds of the students by creating dialogues. The research team began to tackle the issue by confronting 29 students with ten hypothetical situation in which they were asked to judge whether “doing mathematics” was involved in each case. Most of the situations were taken from Kouba and McDonald (1991). Some examples are “Siu Ming said that half a candy bar is better than a third. Was he doing mathematics ?”, “An elder sister lifted her younger brother. She said that he must weigh about 30 pounds less than she. Did she do mathematics ?” and “Dai Keung and Siu Chun went to take a photo at the spiral staircase at the City Hall. When the photo was processed, We would like to thank Prof JinFa Cai for allowing us to use his open-ended questions in the study. The research in this paper was supported by the Direct Grant for Research 1997-98 of the Social Science of Education Panel, The Chinese University of Hong Kong. The opinions expressed are those of the authors and do not necessarily reflect views of the funding agency. (#) The team comprises Chi-Chimg Lam, Ka-Ming Wong and the author. (*) -1- Investigating Conceptions of Math by the Use of Open-ended Mat Problems, presented as ICME-9 TSG11 N.Y. Wong Dai Keung discovered that the staircase looked like a sine curve. Did he use mathematics when he looked at the photos ?”. Results revealed that students associated mathematics with its terminology and content, and that mathematics was often perceived as a set of rules. Wider aspects of mathematics such as visual sense and decision making were only seen as tangential to mathematics. In particular, they were not perceived as “calculable.” However, students did recognise mathematics as closely related to thinking. The details of which can be found in Wong, Lam & Wong (1998). 3. Use of open-ended problems 3.1. Research setting In 1998, we proceeded to make more in-depth investigations by the use of open-ended mathematics problems. Nine classes (around 35 students each) of each of Grades Three, Six, Seven and Nine were asked to tackle to a set of mathematical problems. Each set comprised 2 computational problems, 2-4 words problems and 4 open-ended questions. Two students from each class (2 9 4 = 72 students) were then asked how they approached these problems. The original hypothesis was, a narrow conception of mathematics (as an absolute truth, say) is associated with surface approaches to tackling mathematical problems and a broad conception is associated with deep approaches (Marton & Säljö, 1976; Wong, Lam, & Wong, 1998). Crawford et al (1998a, 1998b) already found that fragmented view is associated with surface approach and cohesive view is associated with deep approach, but they were investigating general approaches to learning rather than on-task approaches (Biggs, 1993). 3.2. Mathematical problems used The students were requested to attempt three sets of mathematical problems before the interview. They were computational problems, word problems and open-ended questions. In the third part, the students were asked to give explanations of their working procedures and the follow-up interview focused mainly on this part. The number of questions in each of the grade levels is listed in Table 1. Table 1. Mathematical problems used in the study Grade 3 Grade 6 Grade 7 Computational problems 2 2 2 Word problems 4 3 2 Open-ended questions 4 4 4 Grade 9 2 2 4 Cai (1999) defines an open-ended mathematics problem as one which allows openness in any of (a) given information, (b) goal to be achieve or (c) the process of solving it (Figure 1). process Given Open Goal Open Open Fig. 1 Definition of open-ended questions -2- Investigating Conceptions of Math by the Use of Open-ended Mat Problems, presented as ICME-9 TSG11 N.Y. Wong Becker (1998) further identified three types of open-ended questions, with ascending levels of difficulties, with (a) the process is open, (b) the end-products are open, and (c) the way of formulating problems are open. In the present research, open-ended questions from Cai (1995) and California State Department of Education (1989) were adapted for use in the present study. The final set of problems include (a) Problems with irrelevant information (Low & Over, 1989) (b) “Problematic word problems” (Verschaffel, et al, 1994, 1997, Yoshida et al, 1997) (c) Problems which allows more than one solution (d) Problems which allows multiple methods (e) Problems with different interpretation of question possible (f) Problems that ask for communication (g) Problems that need judgement (h) Problems that involve decision making 3.3. Results Consistent with what was found in previous research, students repeatedly showed in this study a conception of mathematics being an absolute truth where there is always a routine to solve problems in mathematics. The task of mathematics problem solving is thus the search of such routines. In order to search for these rules, they look for clues embedded in the questions including the given information, what is being asked, the context (which topic does it lie in) and the format of the question. Students held a segregated view of the subject too. Writing is not mathematics and mathematics is calculations with numbers and symbols. Many of them thought that by letting the answer to be found as an unknown x and by setting an appropriate solution, virtually all mathematical problems can be solved by such kinds of routines. Furthermore, one should only write down those things that are formal and that you are sure to be correct (otherwise, marks will be deducted for wrong statements). Therefore, leaving the solution blank is common found in their scripts which does not mean that the student did not tackled the problem nor that the student had no initial ideas of solving it. It is also clear from this study that students’ conception of mathematics is shaped by classroom experience. Most problems given to students lack variations, possess a unique answer and allows only one way of tackling them. It is thus not surprising that students see mathematics as a set of rules, the task of solving mathematical problems is to search for these rules and mathematics learning is to have these rules transmitted from the teacher (Wong, Marton, Wong, & Lam, in preparation). 4. An outlook to possible future research There are a number of areas that worth further research. First, we may need a more powerful and more established instrument to tap the conceptions of mathematics and mathematics learning. The interrelationship among students’ conceptions of mathematics and mathematics learning, their antecedents and consequences worth further exploration. By antecedents we mean the shaping of such conceptions by their exposure to mathematics, the mathematics problems they encounters and the conceptions of mathematics and mathematics learning of their own teachers. And by consequences, we refer to their approaches (surface vs deep) to tackling mathematics problems, their performance in solving routine and non-routine mathematics problems. Intervention programs to broaden students’ conception of mathematics could be another exciting area of research too. -3- Investigating Conceptions of Math by the Use of Open-ended Mat Problems, presented as ICME-9 TSG11 N.Y. Wong References Becker, J. (1998). A comprehensive review and synthesis of classroom related research at the elementary level. Plenary lecture. In H.S. Park, Y.H. Choe, H. Shin, & S.H. Kim (Ed.s). Proceedings of the ICMI-East Asia Regional Conference on Mathematical Education, 1, 243-286 Biggs, J.B. (1993). What do inventories of students’ learning processes really measure ? A theoretical review and clarification. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 63, 3-19. Cai, J. (1995). A Cognitive Analysis of U.S. and Chinese Students’ Mathematical Performance on Tasks Involving Computation, Simple Problem Solving, and Complex Problem Solving: Monograph 7 of Journal of Research in Mathematics Education. Reston, Virginia: National Council of Teachers of Mathematics. Cai, J. (1999). The use of open-ended questions in assessing student performance in problem solving. Lecture delivered at The Chinese University of Hong Kong, 23 March. California State Department of Education (1989). A Question of Thinking: A first look at students’ performance on open-ended questions in mathematics. Sacramento, CA: Author. Carpenter, T.P., Lindquist, M.M., Silver, E.A., & Matthews, W. (1983). Results of the third NAEP mathematics assessment: secondary school. The Mathematics Teacher, 76, 652-659. Clay, G.V., & Kolb, J.R. (1983). The acquisition of meanings for arithmetic symbols. In J.C. Bergeron, & N. Herscovic (Ed.s). Proceedings of the Fifth Annual Meeting of the North American Chapter of the International Group for the Psychology of Mathematics Education, Vol 1, 243-249. Montreal: University of Montreal. Cobb, P. (1985). Two children’s anticipations, beliefs, and motivations. Educational Studies of Mathematics, 16, 111-126. Confrey, J. (1983). Young women, contructivism, and the learning of mathematics. In J.C. Bergeron & N. Herscovics (Ed.s). Proceedings of the Fifth Annual Meeting of the North American Chapter of the International Group for the Psychology of Mathematics Education, Vol 1, 232-238. Montreal: University of Montreal. Crawford, K., Gordon, S., Nicholas, J., & Prosser, M. (1998a). Qualitative different experiences of learning mathematics at university. Learning and Instruction, 8, 455-468. Crawford, K., Gordon, S., Nicholas, J., & Prosser, M. (1998b). University mathematics students’ conceptions of mathematics. Studies in Higher Education, 23, 87-94. Dossey, J.A., Mullis, I.V.S., Lindquist, M.M., & Chambers, D.L. (1988). The Mathematics Report Card, Are we Measuring Up ? Trends and Achievement based on the 1986 National Assessment (NAEP Report No. 17-M-01). Princeton, N.J.: Educational Testing Service. Fleener, M.J. (1996). Scientific world building on the edge of chaos: high school students’ beliefs about mathematics and science. School Science and Mathematics, 96, 312-320. Frank, M.L. (1988). Problem solving and mathematical beliefs. Arithmetic Teacher 35, 32-34. Kloosterman, P. (1991). Beliefs and achievement in seventh-grade mathematics. Focus on Learning Problems in Mathematics, 13(3), 3-15. Kloosterman, P., & Stage, F. (1992). Measuring beliefs about mathematical problem solving. School Science and Mathematics, 92, 109-115. Kouba, V.L., & McDonald, J.L. (1991). What is mathematics to children ? Journal of Mathematical Behavior, 10, 105-113. Lester, F., & Garofalo, J. (1982). Metacognitive aspects of elementary school students’ performance on arithmetic tasks. Paper presented at the meeting of the American Educational Research Association, New York. Leung, F.K.S., Lam, C.C., Mok, I.A.C., Wong, K.M., & Wong, N.Y. (1999). Comparative study on the mathematics curricula of major Asian and western countries. Final report of a research commissioned by the Education Department, Hong Kong. -4- Investigating Conceptions of Math by the Use of Open-ended Mat Problems, presented as ICME-9 TSG11 N.Y. Wong Leung, F. & Wong, M. (1996). Hong Kong students’ mathematics achievement in the international comparison. In N. Law (Ed.), Science and Mathematics Achievements at the Junior Secondary Level in Hong Kong: A Summary Report for Hong Kong in the Third International Mathematics and Science Study, 24-36. Hong Kong: TIMSS Hong Kong Study Centre, The University of Hong Kong. Leung, F. & Wong, M. (1997). Hong Kong pupils’ mathematics achievement in the international comparison. In N. Law (Ed.), Science and Mathematics Achievements at the Mid-Primary Level in Hong Kong: A Summary Report for Hong Kong in the Third International Mathematics and Science Study, 25-39. Hong Kong: TIMSS Hong Kong Study Centre, The University of Hong Kong. Lit, C.K., & Siu, M.K. (1998). A research project on the effect of using history of mathematics in the school classroom. Paper presented at the ICMI Study Conference on the Role of the History of Mathematics in the Teaching and Learning of Mathematics, April 1998, Luminy. Low, R., & Over, R. (1989). Detection of missing and irrelevant information within algebraic story problems. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 59, 296-305. Marton, F., & Säljö, R. (1976). On qualitative differences in learning - I: Outcome and process. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 46, 4-11. McLeod, D.B. (1992). Research on affect in mathematics education: A reconceptualization. In D.A. Grouws (Ed.). Handbook of Research on Mathematics Teaching and Learning (A project of the National Council of Teachers of Mathematics). New York: Macmillan. Peck, D.M. (1984). Barriers to mathematical thinking and problem solving. In J. Moser (Ed.). Proceedings of the Sixth Annual Meeting of the North American Chapter of the International Group for the Psychology of Mathematics Education, 165-170. Madison, WI: University of Wisconsin. Schoenfeld, A.H. (1989). Explorations of students’ mathematical beliefs and behavior. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 20, 338-355. Stage, F.K., & Kloosterman, P. (1995). Gender, beliefs, and achievement in remedial college-level mathematics. Journal of Higher Education, 66, 294-311. Stevenson, H.W., & Lee, S. (1990). Contexts of Achievement: Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development Serial No. 221, Vol. 55. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press. Travers, K.J., & Westbury, I. (1989). The IEA Studies of Mathematics I: Analysis of Mathematics Curriculum. Oxford: Pergamon. Underhill, R. (1988). Mathematics learners’ beliefs: A review. Focus on Learning Problems in Mathematics, 20, 55-69. Verschaffel, L., De Corte, E., & Lasure, S. (1994). Realistic considerations in mathematical modeling of school arithmetic word problems. Learning and Instruction, 4, 273-294. Verschaffel, L., DeCorte, E., & Borghart, I. (1997). Pre-service teachers’ conceptions and beliefs about the role of real-world knowledge in mathematical modelling of school word problems. Learning and Instruction, 7, 339-359. Wong, N.Y. (1993). When do Hong Kong students consider themselves understand mathematics ? Paper presented at the Tenth Annual Conference of the Hong Kong Educational Research Association. Hong Kong: The Chinese University of Hong Kong. Wong, N.Y. (1995). The Relationship between Hong Kong Students’ Perception of their Mathematics Classroom Environment and their Approaches to Learning: A Longitudinal Study. Unpublished Ph.D. thesis, The University of Hong Kong. Wong, N.Y., Lam, C.C., Leung, F.K.S., Mok, I.A.C., & Wong, K.M. (1999). An Analysis of the Views of Various Sectors on the Mathematics Curriculum. Final report of a research commissioned by the Education Department, Hong Kong. -5- Investigating Conceptions of Math by the Use of Open-ended Mat Problems, presented as ICME-9 TSG11 N.Y. Wong Wong, N.Y., Lam, C.C., & Wong, K.M. (1998). Students’ and teachers’ conception of mathematics learning: A Hong Kong study. In H.S. Park, Y.H. Choe, H. Shin, & S.H. Kim (Ed.s). Proceedings of the ICMI-East Asia Regional Conference on Mathematical Education, 2, 375-404. Wong, N.Y., Marton, F., Wong, K.M., & Lam, C.C. (in preparation). The lived space of mathematics learning. Unpublished manuscript. Yoshida, H., Verschaffel, L., & DeCorte, E. (1997). Realistic considerations in solving problematics word problems: Do Japanese and Belgian children have the same difficulties ? Learning and Instruction, 7, 329-338. -6-