Cheek pouch’s use and function: testing the Intragroup Competition versus Predation Hypotheses Cheek pouches, as a primitive trait of the subfamily Cercopithecinae, are one of the few morphological specialisations linked with dietary adaptations that have been greatly overlooked. This paper tests two functional hypotheses: the Intragroup Competition Hypothesis (ICH), which states that cercopithecine’s cheek pouches are used to reduce within-group contest feeding competition, and the Predation Risk Hypothesis (PRH), which states that cheek pouches are used to increase the energy intake in potentially risky areas allowing food to be transported away and processed at a safe site, while minimising exposure time. This study establishes the context (i.e. spatial distribution) for feeding competition prior to the evaluation of the ICH hypothesis. The hypotheses were tested with data from a troop of yellow baboons, Papio cynocephalus, (N=24) observed during a period of 12 months in Mikumi National Park, Tanzania. Results showed that cheek pouch use was not associated with competitive contexts of the contest type. In addition, there were no individual differences (i.e. age, sex and rank) in cheek pouch use. Pouch use was associated with predator risk positioning and the likelihood of a predator encounter. The findings are supportive of the anti-predator function of cheek pouches used to assist the animal's food acquisition, minimising time spent in hazardous sites. The study concludes that there is no evidence indicating that cheek pouches provide a reduction of group living costs, as there is no evidence that the use of these minimises the effects of contest feeding competition on animals. Keywords: contest competition, intragroup competition, predation, cheek pouches 1 INTRODUCTION Whilst intraspecific competition is widely accepted as an important factor driving the evolution of primate social behaviour (Alexander 1974; Kummer 1979; Dunbar 1988; Sterck et al. 1997) and described as far more important than intergroup competition (Janson 1985), predation is considered one of the most important selective pressures on animals (Dunbar 1988; van Schaik 1989). It is held to possess the power to modify behaviour and morphology (Alexander 1974; van Schaik 1983, 1989; Stanford 2002). In coping with intragroup competition costs, primates have the option of employing different resource exploitation strategies. The ratio of costs to benefits for each strategy may differ depending on an individual's competitive ability (i.e. sex, dominance rank and age). As a consequence, different behavioural profiles may emerge if each individual chooses its 'optimal strategy'. Strategies that have been identified in cercopithecines include: (a) independent foraging at greater distances from the troop centre, which has been associated with adult males (Post 1981); (b) feeding on less valuable items to avoid food competition (Saito 1996); (c) reducing foraging efficiency of other females by supplants (e.g. Barton & Whiten 1993); (d) choosing a safe (central) position (van Noordwijk & van Schaik 1987; van Schaik 1989); (e) gaining priority of access to foods and thereby increasing tolerance by the dominant animals (Sigg & Falett 1985; Belisle & Chapais 2001) and (f) the use of cheek pouches to reduce intragroup feeding competition costs (Murray 1975; Lindburg 1977; Richard 1985; Deutsch and Lee 1991; Lambert and Whitham 2001; Lambert 2005). These studies have not directly focused on pouching behaviour (but see Murray 1975; Hayes et al. 1992; Lambert and Whitham 2001; Lambert 2005), have been descriptive (Murray 1975; Hayes et al. 1992) and mostly conducted in captivity (Murray 1975; Lambert and Whitham 2001). Field investigations are extremely rare (but see Lambert 2005) and consequently, very little data is available in the literature. On the other hand, in coping with predation, animal’s behavioural responses include: the forming of large groups (Hamilton 1971; van Schaik 1983), polyspecific association 2 (Cheney & Wrangham 1987; Struhsaker 1981), vigilance (Cowlishaw 1998; Steenbeek et al. 1999; Hill & Cowlishaw 2002), spatial positioning (Hall & DeVore 1965; Rhine & Westlund 1981; van Noordwijk & van Schaik 1987; Ron et al. 1996), alarm calls (Cheney & Seyfarth 1981) and/or the selection of raised positions (Cowlishaw 1993). Some anti-predator strategies identified are of the morphological type, and may include for example camouflage (e.g. olive colobus monkey, Oates 1994), large incisive structure in adult males (Plavcan & van Schaik 1992) and some authors suggest the presence of cheek pouches may allow the animal to reduce its exposure time in predator risk environments by transporting foods to safer positions (e.g. Richard, 1985; Caldecott 1986; Lambert 1998). Although the implications of such use are considerable, the contention remains largely unsubstantiated. Lambert (2005) is the only published work to date that directly evaluates cheek pouch use as both a strategy to reduce feeding competition and to mitigate vulnerability to predation in forest dwelling Cercopithecus ascanius and Lophocebus albigena. The study showed that animals irrespective of sex and age filled their cheek pouches and retreated to safer sites in order to minimise times spent in unsafe sites. Although patch size, used to estimate contestability of food, was not associated with cheek pouch use, the use of cheek pouches in both species increased with the number of neighbours in a patch and in C. ascanius took place in larger fruiting trees. These findings are said to suggest that animals use their pouches to mitigate the costs of intragroup competition. Unfortunately, the study wasn’t able to examine the use of cheek pouches in relation to the individual’s resource holding capacity (i.e. animals with less holding capacity are expected to use cheek pouches more than animals that can monopolise a food resource), which may help distinguish between the two hypotheses tested (e.g. all animals regardless of rank must minimise predation risk). Baboons’ feeding behaviour differs from C. ascanius and L. albigena in that their diet includes a wider range of foods that vary in relation to processing costs (Norton et al. 1987). The greater variability in the behavioural ecology of Papio cynocephalus provides a good opportunity to examine cheek pouch use and evaluate the hypotheses. 3 This study sets out to test two hypotheses of the use and function of cheek pouches: (1) the Intragroup Competition Hypothesis (ICH) whereby cheek pouches are used to maximise food intake that could otherwise be lost in competition to other associates, reducing the effects of contest feeding competition on animals. We predicted that (a) individuals would use their cheek pouches more (i.e. fill, distend and empty) when the risk of contest competition is high. In an attempt to minimise inferences (i.e. test as many steps as possible in the argument), we first established the context for contest feeding competition. We did this by examining feeding interference rates in relation to spatial distribution of resources; and that (b) in a multimale social group with female dominance hierarchies, differences in pouch use will be expected with varying competitive abilities. We predicted an inverse relationship between cheek pouch distension, filling and emptying and the ability to monopolise a resource as measured by age, sex, and dominance rank. That is, younger individuals will use their pouches more than older individuals, females more than males, and low-ranking animals more than high-ranking individuals. (2) The Predation Risk Hypothesis (PRH) states that cheek pouches are used to increase the acquisition rate in potentially risky areas, minimising exposure time and therefore reducing the likelihood of capture. At times when animals perceive danger, food can be easily transported and processed at safe site. We predicted that: (c) animals would take advantage of their cheek pouches in high predator risk positions. Risk indicators such as vegetation cover, animal height and number of neighbours in proximity will be associated with cheek pouch filling and distension. In addition, exposed positions will be associated with other anti-predator behaviours such as vigilance as well as cheek pouch filling and distending when the animal is feeding. (d) Since evidence suggests that body size is not negatively associated with predation rates (Cowlishaw 1993, 1994; Zuberbühler & Jenny 2002), this hypothesis predicts no individual differences in anti-predator behaviours. This study assumes that all animals are equally vulnerable to predation and need to minimise risks. However, if animals are differentially exposed to predation risk sites (e.g. low ranking animals spending more time in unsafe positions), these differences should be reflected in the use made of anti-predator behaviours. Therefore, when there are no differences in unsafe 4 positioning between animals: no sex, age or rank differences will be observed in outward vigilance and cheek pouch behaviours. When differences in unsafe positioning occur: animals that spend more time in unsafe positions will also spend more time scanning the environment and more time filling and distending their cheek pouches. (E) As the likelihood of encountering a predator is not constant, it is assumed that animals, in order to survive, tune their behaviour according to their perception of danger. For example, animals are more likely to be vigilant on days that follow a predator encounter (citation). Therefore, we predict that pouch emptying will be associated with safe positions when the probability of a predator encounter is high. For example, animals will transport food stored in pouches away from the acquisition site when the probability of encountering a predator is high. Alternatively, when the probability of an encounter is low, pouch emptying will not be associated with safe positions with regard to predator risk, as it is energetically more costly to transport food away. Although cheek pouches may serve multiple functions and some of these capacities may be by-products of selective pressures for other abilities, the aim of this paper is to focus on two of the potential current functions of this anatomical trait (see Gutierrez 2003 unpublished findings that support other functional explanations). 5 METHODS Study Area and Subjects This study was conducted in Mikumi National Park, Tanzania, from January 1998 to January 1999. The data presented here come from all members of the long-term studied Viramba Troop of Papio cynocephalus except infants (N=24). Troop history can be found in e.g. Rhine & Westlund (1981) and Rhine (1986).. Brachystegia woodland characterises the study area, which receives an average of 842 mm of rainfall per year. Further details regarding habitat and ecology of the study site can be found in Norton et al. (1987). Baboon predators in Mikumi include leopards, lions, hyenas, birds of prey and wild dogs. With the exception of wild dogs all others are thought to be very common. Overall, based on the number of potential predators (as well as tracks encountered), reported attacks and alarm calls recorded, researchers are largely of the view that a high predation risk exists for the Viramba baboons (Anderson 1986; Norton, pers. comm). Sampling RegimeFocal animal follows were conducted from 7am to 5pm for periods of thirty minutes (with 2-minute sample intervals) recording a total of 1,126 hours of information. Both instantaneous and one-zero time sampling methods (Martin & Bateson 1986) were used. Contest Feeding Competition Despite inconsistency in how different studies have measured feeding competition of the contest type (e.g. Janson and van Schaik 1988), the recording of the frequency of spatial displacements in the feeding context seems to be the most frequently used and best available measure of the intensity of feeding competition (Post 1978; Shopland 1987; van Schaik and van Noordwijk 1988; Barton 1989; Barton and Whiten 1993; Saito 1996; Sterck and 6 Steenbeek 1997), providing valuable information about the variation in the levels of contest competition which animals experience. Another less commonly used estimate of feeding competition, the number of neighbours in proximity, was avoided as it is problematic (but see Barton 1989; Hayes et al.1992; Shopland 1987). Firstly, spacing may be in itself a strategy to reduce competition when predation risk is low (van Schaik and van Noordwijk 1988) and may therefore not be a reliable estimate when animals have provided for anti-competition behaviours. Secondly, when feeding on patches, animals that have contested their access to the food patch may not be able to monopolise resources further, particularly if food is of similar quality and abundant. These animals may then simply scramble rather than contest for food. Thirdly, the female’s reproductive status has been shown to relate to the number of neighbours (Barton 1989, Gutierrez 2003 unpublished data). Consorting and lactating females were more likely to attract a higher number of neighbours. Finally, a high number of neighbours in proximity may simply reflect the physical structure of the arboreal patch. For the same study population, individuals feeding on ground level were found to have a higher number of neighbours than those feeding on the crown (Gutierrez 2003 unpublished data). The two-dimensional ground space appeared in fact more extreme in terms of clumping of fallen foods and individuals were necessarily clumped in space. Hence, ‘feeding supplants’ were recorded to examine contest competition by food distribution. Supplants given and received were analysed using one-zero sampling, which was preferred because it is a more reliable estimate of rapid event-like behaviours (Hawkins 1999; Martin and Bateson 1986). Food distribution Food distribution was categorised into clumped and dispersed. 'Clumped distribution' was defined in line with other studies (e.g. Shopland 1987) as a concentrated food site (i.e. single-tree, multiple trees, shrubs or herbaceous clumps) where at least 40% of adult/sub-adult members fed. To exclude rapid visits to food patches, a minimum visit time of ten minutes was necessary in order to record a patch feeding event. Only one food distribution type per 7 follow contributed to the mean length of time spent per individual. Dispersed feeding was recorded when animals foraged on non-patchy foods. Cheek Pouch Use Cheek pouch use was defined both as a dynamic (i.e. filling and emptying) and as a static measure (i.e. distension), and recorded using instantaneous time sampling. Cheek pouch filling was defined as any increment in pouch distension; pouch emptying was defined as any chewing of foods stored in cheek pouches; and finally, pouch distension was defined as the degree of distension of cheek pouches. In order to measure distension, pouch capacity was divided into ‘less than one-third full’, ‘one-third to two-thirds full’ and ‘two to three-thirds full’ and recorded on each side. Predation Risk Three variables were used in the definition of predation risk. (1) Attacks are more successful when baboons are in high cover where visibility is poor (Cowlishaw 1993). Cover of the prey animal was recorded as the degree of visibility assessed at the baboon eye-level in a three-metre perimeter. Categories used were low cover (i.e. more than two thirds of the animal's body is visible) and high cover (i.e. one third or less of the animal's body is visible). (2) Height of the animal's position. Lower heights are related to higher predation risk (Cowlishaw 1993; Steenbeck et al. 1999). As tall tree heights reduce the possibility of attack by some important predators (e.g. lions), baboons are said to be safer at heights above two metres (Cowlishaw 1993). Height of the animal was categorised at ground level and above two-metres (e.g. on tree crown). In order to control for rank bias, categories were allocated with approximately the same number of low, medium and high-ranking individuals. (3) Number of neighbours. Individuals with more neighbours are said to be in less danger (van Noordwijk and van Schaik 1987; Steenbeek et al. 1999; Olupot and Waser 2001) because 8 more animals are vigilant and capable of detecting the predator. In addition, more animals in proximity reduce the probability of being selected for attack. And finally, more animals can deter predators in case of retaliation. The number of neighbours in proximity of the focal animal was defined as the number of troop members within a three-metre radius. This was chosen as a conservative estimate to control for the effects of within group competition (Robinson 1981). The spatial position of the animal was recorded on every instantaneous point sample. Cover, height and the number of neighbours in proximity were combined to define a safe versus unsafe position of the focal animal. A safe position was indicated by low cover, height above 2 metres and at least two neighbours within three metres of the focal animal. An unsafe position was indicated by high cover, ground level height and no neighbours within three metres. Probability of Predator Encounter The probability of a predator encounter was defined as the likelihood of meeting a predator based on an animal’s past predator encounters. It is the probability of coming across a predator assuming the outcome of the previous day's encounter. This assumes that animals will engage in anti-predator strategies all the more immediately following a predator encounter. The effect of interacting with a predator was examined only during the day following an encounter. This was considered a conservative approach. Two categories were used: (a) high probability: animals experienced predator encounter on previous day; and (b) low: animals experienced no predator encounter on previous day. 9 Vigilance Vigilance of the animal's surroundings was recorded as an estimate of the animal's anti-predator strategies. Although not free from confounding problems (Stanford 2002), there is some evidence for the anti-predator function of scanning behaviours (Cowlishaw 1998; Steenbeck et al. 1999). Outward vigilance (i.e. 'scanning the environment' where there is no other troop member) was recorded in the form of visual scan of the animal's surroundings and clearly distinguished from inward scanning for other troop members, which is not considered in this study. Small juveniles were excluded from analyses as they may still rely on other members for vigilance. Individual variables Animal's sex, age classes and rank categories were recorded. Age was categorised as small juvenile (i.e. 1-2,5 years), large juvenile (i.e. 2,5-1st consort or 5 years), sub-adult males (i.e. 5 years- transfer) and adults. Rank was categorised as high, medium and low dominance using tertiles of dominance cardinal rank computed using Zumpe and Michael (1986). Statistics All sets and subsets of data tested for normality using the Kolmogorov-Smirnov test (Sokal and Rohlf 1995) conformed to the normal distribution (P>0.05). Multivariate analysis was used where overlap between samples made it possible (most cases). Effect sizes are reported for significant p-values. All statistical tests are two tailed, with alpha set at 0.05. Bonferroni post hoc tests were employed where significant effects were observed in analysis of variance. 10 RESULTS Food distribution and Feeding supplants Food distribution categories differed significantly in the feeding supplant rates observed (Paired t-test: t23=-3.126, P=0.005, d= 0.732, see Figure 1). Animals encountered higher rates of supplants while foraging for dispersed foods than while feeding on clumped resources. These results revealed that animals experienced greater competition while foraging for dispersed food resources. Cheek pouch use and food distribution (Prediction A) Repeated measures analysis of variance examining the association of resource distribution with cheek pouch distension, filling and emptying revealed significant differences (F(1,23)=107.838, P<0.001, partial η2=0.82 ; F(1,23)=45.257, P<0.001, partial η2=0.66; F(1,23)=4.731, P=0.040, partial η2=0.17, for pouch distension, filling and emptying respectively). In particular, animals spent significantly more time distending (see Figure 2), filling and emptying their pouches while feeding on clumped resources than while foraging on dispersed foods. 11 Figure 1 Feeding supplant rates by food distribution: clumped feeding versus dispersed foraging (mean/+SE). Paired t-test: t23=-3.126, P=0.005; d= 0.732. 5 Feeding supplants (%) 4 3 2 1 0 Clumped Dispersed Figure 2 Cheek pouch distension by food distribution (mean/+SE), F(1,23)=107.838, P<0.001, partial η2=0.82. Pouch distension (% of instants) 70 60 50 40 30 20 10 0 Clumped Dispersed 12 Pouch use and competitive ability (Prediction B) Results from multivariate analysis revealed no association between the animal's competitive ability and cheek pouch use. Sex: cheek pouch distension in females was not significantly different from males (F(1,11)=0.617, P=0.449). Similarly, sexes did not differ in cheek pouch filling (F(1,11)=0.082, P=0.780 nor cheek pouch emptying (F(1,11)=0.470, P=0.507). Age: there was no significant main effect of age categories on baboons' cheek pouch distension (F(2,11)=1.858, P=0.202, cheek pouch filling (F(2,11)=1.715, P=0.225), or cheek pouch emptying (F(2,11)=0.582, P=0.575). Using a non-categorical measure (i.e. years of age) yielded similar results (p>0.05 for all Pearson correlations). Dominance Rank: No significant differences in cheek pouch distension (F(2,11)=0.823, P=0.464), cheek pouch filling (F(2,11)=0.126, P=0.883) or cheek pouch emptying (F(2,11)=0.600, P=0.566) were observed between rank categories. Results from correlation analysis using the animal's cardinal dominance rank (i.e. percentage score in rank) and the percentage of pouch distension, filling and emptying were consistent with multivariate analysis (all p>0.05). It is possible that animals put into place behavioural strategies other than cheek pouch use to deal with ‘everyday competition’, and that these being sufficient, do not require cheek pouch exploitation. However, during resource scarcity, animals with low competitive ability may need to invest in an additional strategy to cope with even greater levels of contest competition. If these are the underlying mechanisms of coping with competition, rank effects should become apparent under these severe periods. However, there is evidence that this not the case. No rank differences in cheek pouch use were observed during the period of greatest food scarcity1 (all p>0.05), which interestingly corresponds to the season of greatest use of cheek pouches. 1 Patch richness was defined as amount of edible food available in a patch in relation to the total patch capacity. Rich, medium and scarce patch richness categories were obtained combining both measures of food quantity and food availability (Gutierrez Diego, 2003 unpublished data). 13 Predator Risk and Pouch Use (Prediction C) Predator risk indicators were good predictors of the animal’s behaviour. Overall, animals avoided unsafe positions, spending very little time in these. Baboons spent more than twice the time in safe compared to unsafe locations. (Paired t test: t23=6.394, P<0.001, d=2.07; see Fig. 3). Most of the daylight time (i.e. 80%) was spent in conditions that fell between these two extreme safety categories. However, when animals took unsafe locations, they distended and filled their cheek pouches significantly more than when they were in safe positions (Paired t test: t23=12.076 P<0.001, d=3.27 and t23=6.470, P<0.001, d=1.92 for pouch use and filling respectively, see Fig. 4). In addition, compared to safe contexts, unsafe contexts elicited higher rates of outward vigilance (Paired t test: t23=2.217, P=0.037, d=0.55). In order to ensure that these results were not an artefact of differences in the availability of predator risk positions, differences in the relative availability of safe and unsafe positions were examined by testing for the differences in the proportions of follows in which any time was spent in either a safe or unsafe position. A test for the equality of the proportions (Arcus) revealed no significant differences between proportions (proportion unsafe=0.30, proportion difference=0.045, 95% CI for proportion safe=0.34, difference proportion =0.018 to 0.081, p=0.002). Thus, discountingdifferential availability of positions as a confounding variable. In addition, animals used their cheek pouches to compensate for lower food intake immediately after predator encounter. After a predator encounter, pouch use first decreased below normal levels and then increased steadily with time (see Figure 5). 14 Figure 3 Time spent in safe versus unsafe positions by all troop members (mean/+SE). Paired t test: t23=6.394, P<0.001, d=2.07. 16 Time spent (% of instants) 14 12 10 8 6 4 2 0 Safe Unsafe Figure 4. Time spent using and filling pouches by risk of place (mean/+SE). Paired t test: t23=12.076 P<0.001, d=3.27 and t23=6.470, P<0.001, d=1.92 for pouch use and filling respectively. 50 45 Pouch use Pouch filling 40 Time spent % 35 30 25 20 15 10 5 0 Safe Unsafe 15 Figure 5 Pouch distension and occurrence of predator encounters in minutes (mean/+SE). 70 Pouch distension (%) 60 50 40 30 20 10 0 No encounter Within 15min 15-60min 60-180min 180min-same day Individual Differences in Anti-predator Behaviours (Prediction D) Vigilance: there was no difference between sex and age groups on the time spent scanning the environment (Independent Samples t test: t18=0.653, P=0.522, for sex; One-way ANOVA: F3,20=0.591, P=0.628, for age groups). However, rank categories differed in the time animals spent scanning the environment (One-way ANOVA: F2,17=9.620, P=0.002, partial η2=0.53). Medium rank individuals spent significantly less time surveying the environment. No differences between low and high-ranking animals were observed (PB>0.05). This behavioural pattern was attuned to the animal's exposure to predator risk positions. Rank differences were found between the various amounts of time spent in safe positions (One-way ANOVA: F2,21=8.211, P=0.002, partial η2=0.44). Medium rank individuals spent high amounts of time in safe positions and consequently have less need to monitor the environment. High-ranking individuals spent less time in safe places compared to mid rank (PB=0.002). However, no significant differences were found in the amount of time spent in safety between high and low ranking individuals (PB=0.318). Low and high rank individuals did not differ in the amount of time they spent in safe places and consequently did 16 not differ in the amount of vigilance they invested. No significant differences were found between the rank categories in the amount of time spent in unsafe positions (One-way ANOVA: F2,21=0.819, P=0.455). Therefore, these results suggest no high-ranking advantages and no low-rank disadvantages in terms of safety from predators. Cheek pouch use: in line with the above results, no significant sex differences were found in cheek pouch distension or filling when the subset of data that examined predator risk was used (Independent Samples t test: t22=0.848, P=0.406 and t22=1.066, P=0.298 for pouch distension and filling respectively). Similarly, no significant differences in pouch distension or filling were observed between age-classes (One-way ANOVA: F3,20=1.505, P=0.244; F3,20=2.155, P=0.125, for distension and filling respectively). Finally, the rank groups did not differ in their observed cheek pouch distension or filling (One-way ANOVA: F2,21=0.710, P=0.503; F2,21=0.503, P=0.612 for pouch distension and filling respectively). Predator Risk and Cheek Pouch Emptying (Prediction E) Baboons emptied their cheek pouches significantly more while in unsafe places. Significant differences were found in pouch emptying by predator risk positions (Paired t test: t23=4.67, P<0.001, d=1.53). Since cheek pouches were filled more in unsafe positions (see Fig. 4), it is likely that this behavioural pattern reflects animals filling, emptying and refilling their pouches. However, more interestingly, time spent in unsafe positions varied significantly with the probability of encountering a predator (Paired t test: t12=4.595, P=0.001, d=1.93). Baboons spent more time in unsafe places when the probability of meeting a predator was low. In other words, animals avoided exposing themselves when the probability of a predator encounter is high. When the probability of encountering a predator was low, significant differences in pouch emptying were found with predator risk position (Paired t test: t23=6.68, P<0.001, d=1.82). Pouch emptying occurred more frequently in unsafe positions. However, the subset 17 of data to test pouch emptying by predator risk when the probability of a predator encounter is high was too small. A Mann-Whitney U test on this data revealed no significant differences (U=1.5, N1=2, N2=6, P=0.11, d=0.83). Assuming parametric data, available power to detect a difference in the behavioural pattern (at the 5% level) between the conditions was very low (14%). With the data here being non-parametric the true power is likely to be even less. Discussion Cheek Pouch Use in Relation to Food Distribution (and Contest Competition) Cheek pouche use in the Mikumi baboons was related to food distribution. In particular, cheek pouch use was more important for baboons when encountering clumped food resources (i.e. animals filled, distended and emptied their pockets in association with patchily distributed resources). However, for the Mikumi baboons during this study clumped food was associated with lower levels of contest competition than dispersed food. This result therefore does not provide support for the ICH hypothesis but is consistent with the PHR. These findings are consistent with the literature in relation to cheek pouch use being associated with clumped resources. Lindburg (1971) observed macaques (Macaca mulatta) employ their cheek pouches while feeding on small food items which occurred in a patchy fashion. Murray (1975) also refers to the frequent use of cheek pouches under clumped conditions across genera. A similar pattern of pouch use was reported for Cercopithecus ascanius and Lophocebus albigena (Lambert 1998; 2005) and finally, in captive Papio cynocephalus (Lambert and Whitham 2001). 18 Captive studies have found intensive pouch use under crowded conditions. This has led some authors to suggest that cheek pouches mitigate intragroup contest competition (Murray 1973; Deutsch and Lee 1991; Lambert and Whitham 2001). However, this assertion overlooks other important factors that arise from the captive nature of these studies. That is, that competition is not generated uniquely by the food per se, but by the predictability of food absence during a set period of time. In captivity, food is usually only available at particular time/s of the day, and therefore a food item if missed will only be restored during the following feeding session. On the contrary, food availability in the wild is non-predictive. Animals encounter resources under a continuous search fashion. Cheek Pouch Use and Individual Characteristics Cheek pouch use was not associated with individual characteristics of sex, age or rank. These results are in line with previous studies that have examined rank in relation to cheek pouch use. This again is consistent with the PRH and not the ICH. Lambert (1998; 2005) found no sex effect on cheek pouch use in two cercopithecine species: Cercopithecus ascanius and Lophocebus albigena. Hayes et al. reported some sex differences in cheek pouch use. However, it is very difficult to draw a clear behavioural pattern from the results. Analyses were computed for the various age groups as well as time of the day, without reporting interactions between these variables. In addition, no overall cheek pouch use comparison was presented for sex. There is no further support for sex differences in the study of wild populations. Sex differences in cheek pouch use have only been observed under captive conditions (Lambert and Whitham 2001). In this context, adult females pouched food more often and to a greater extent than males. Age differences in cheek pouch use in Papio ursinus were only apparent in males (Hayes et al. 1992). The frequency with which males used cheek pouches increased with age, peaking at the juvenile stage. However, no significant age differences were found in females. In Lambert’s (2005) study, L. albigena age groups did not differ in their pouching of foods. Although subadult C. ascanius males used their cheek pouches less than adult males, this 19 seemed likely to be the result of a difference in diet rather than a behavioural pattern per se. That is, differential use of pouches may be explained by other factors such as nutritional requirements. Hayes et al. (1992) found that lower ranking adults (Papio ursinus) took no advantage of their cheek pouches to reduce feeding competition costs. While female’s dominance rank was not associated with pouch use, male’s dominance rank was positively associated. That is, the higher rather than the lower ranking males were making greater use of their pouches. Also, cheek pouch use was not associated with rank in wild living Cercopithecus ascanius and Lophocebus albigena (Lambert 1998). Furthermore, no significant rank effects on cheek pouch use were observed in Lambert and Whitham (2001) despite the fact that this was a captive study. Lower ranking animals made more use of their cheek pouchesbut no statistical rank effect was reported. Overall then, the primate literature does not support the idea that cheek pouches provide rank related advantages. Studies looking at the association between dominance rank and feeding success have shown contradictory results. While some research has found a relationship between rank and feeding success/time (Dittus 1979; Wrangham and Waterman 1981; Whitten 1983; Janson 1985, 1992; van Noordwijk and van Schaik 1987; Altmann and Muruthi 1988; Johnson et al. 1990; Saito 1996; Koenig et al. 1998: Presbytis entellus), others have not (Altmann 1980; Barton 1989; Post et al. 1980; Ron et al. 1996). This may be a consequence of the differing interpretations of the concept of dominance, a subject that has provoked considerable debate in the literature (see Drews 1993 for a review). Nevertheless, those studies that do not find clear feeding advantages may indicate that animals make use of alternative strategies to deal with feeding competition. In this study, the lack of association between rank and cheek pouch use, even during periods of food shortage, therefore eliminates one of the potential alternative strategies that low-ranking animals may select to overcome contest competition costs. 20 Overall then, the results of this study very clearly do not support the intragroup competition hypothesis. There was no evidence indicating that cheek pouches were used to minimise the effects of contest feeding competition on animals. These anatomical features seemed to be used to maximise food intake in relation to distribution of resources. In particular, animals made use of their cheek pouches when resource items were distributed in patches, in which food items cannot be monopolised once access to the patch has been gained. The evidence in support of the intragroup competition function in the literature is based on the finding that cercopithecines pouching foods did so to a greater extent when the number of neighbours was higher (Lambert 2005). No other compelling evidence has been put forward to date. Baboons’ anti-predator behaviour was not fixed but individuals invested differentially in accordance with their subjective perception of risk. Baboons avoided spending time in unsafe places, but when this was inevitable, they were more vigilant. In addition, when animals fed in dangerous positions, they filled their cheek pouches more frequently and to a greater extent than when feeding in safer sites. The use of cheek pouches allowed the rate of food harvesting to exceed chewing and swallowing rates, particularly consequential in case of a sudden need to escape from a predator or to retreat to a safer spot to avoid unnecessary risk. These results support previous suggestions made in the literature (Lindburg 1971; Richard 1985; Caldecott 1986; Lambert 1998, 2005). Overall, no individual differences were observed in vigilance behaviours. Since the analyses were conducted on all animals it is unlikely that the behavioural pattern was a 21 function of male competition. Furthermore, animals scanned the environment more frequently when on their own (Gutiérrez Diego 2003 unpublished findings), when the individual risk is higher, suggesting that vigilance was not attributable to the detection of other troops. Although the exact target of the vigilant animal cannot be established (Treves 2003), the lack of individual differences in terms of sex and age suggests that outward vigilance was an important anti-predator behaviour for all individuals (Cowlishaw 1998; Steenbeck et al. 1999). In contrast with previous studies (van Schaik & van Noordwijk 1988; Ron et al. 1996), higher-ranking individuals did not spend more time in safer positions. Consequently, these animals engaged in vigilance as much as the lower ranks. Only medium ranking baboons that spent more time in safer conditions invested less time in vigilance. Therefore, no high-ranking advantages and no low-ranking disadvantages in terms of safety from predators were observed. In line with other studies, no sex differences were observed (Cowlishaw 1998). As with vigilance, no overall individual differences were observed in cheek pouch use. No differences in pouch distension and pouch filling were detected between males and females, age classes and rank categories. Since predation affects all individuals, the failure to find individual differences in cheek pouch use provides further support for the Predation Risk Hypothesis. Baboons emptied their pouches more in unsafe places that were selected when the probability of encountering a predator was low. Similarly, Lambert (2005) found that animals that pouched food often stayed at the same location. While in these sites, animals may fillrefill their "pockets", therefore maximising foraging efficiency and avoiding the loss of the resource in case of the need for urgent flight. Furthermore, foraging efficiency was not only increased by pocketing foods while in unsafe places but also by incrementing food intake via pouches after being faced with a predator. 22 There is no clear result in terms of what baboons do when they are exposed to a high risk of predation. Although animals did not empty their pouches more in situations of high risk, that they empty these in positions of safety cannot be ascertained due to the small statistical power involved. In examining movement patterns after pouch filling, Lambert (2005) found that irrespective of sex and age, animals either stayed in the same place of acquisition or retreated to safer positions. Thus suggesting that animals minimised predator risk by avoiding vulnerable positions. Since baboons, red tails and mangabeys adopt behavioural patterns that should make them harder to capture, these results are consistent with the hypothesis that cheek pouches serve an anti-predator function. Vigilance was used in order to detect potential predators, whereas pouching foods enabled minimisation of exposure and loss of resource in risky areas. Time investments in antipredator strategies were far greater for cheek pouch use. This however, is likely to be a consequence of cheek pouches serving other functions (i.e. to minimise time constraints, Gutiérrez Diego 2003 unpublished findings). Results also indicate that cheek pouches allowed animals to 'catch up' with lower intake rates during days in which predators were encountered. Murray (1973) suggested that low levels of predation lead to spacing mechanisms to avoid within-group competition and in this case low levels of cheek pouch use is expected. If on the contrary, predation pressures are high, spacing becomes less relevant than avoiding predation. In this case, pouch use is expected to be high due to within group competition. From this view, predation becomes a determinant factor of intragroup competition, which may be mitigated by the use of pouches. Results from this study do not support this view in that pouch use is not a function of within group competition, as it is independent of the effect of intra-group competition and the kind of food available in high predation risk areas (Gutiérrez Diego 2003 unpublished findings). Thisresearch suggest that the Mikumi baboons used their cheek pouches to help mitigate the foraging costs of anti-predation behaviourPocketing activities were more 23 important than vigilance from the point of view of time and energy investment. In this sense, these anatomical features allow animals to behave as ‘energy maximisers’ since they facilitate foraging efficiency, as well as to behave as ‘time minimisers’ since they permit food transport and reduce time exposure in potentially risky areas. The use of cheek pouches allowed animals to minimise the loss of food resources, otherwise in jeopardy, when entering especially dangerous positions. Optimisation of the individual' fitness is therefore achieved through the investment in adaptive responses to an unsafe environment. The radiation of cheek-pouched monkeys took place in Africa and Asia approximately five million years ago (Fleagle & Reed 1999), involving a great diversity of habitats. In an attempt to explain the potential evolution of this anatomical trait, Lambert (2005) notes that the use of cheek pouches by current cercopithecines suggests feeding competition as the primary selection for cheek pouch evolution. However, habitat diversity inevitably led to inter-regional variation in the diets and feeding ecologies of primates. This may indicate that habitat as a single factor seems unlikely to have been the only selective force in the evolution of cheek pouches (but see Maruhashi 1993). Yet, in the course of evolution, predation is a problem that has affected cercopithecines in a more uniform manner and may provide a more plausible explanation. Alternatively, the use of cheek pouches in living populations may simply reflect by-products of a more general feeding/foraging efficiency function. As Lambert (2005) states, cheek pouches are a taxonomic trait that have received surprisingly little attention in the primate literature. Despite the frequent use casually observed by researchers, there is only a handful of published studies that have focused on this anatomical structure (Hayes et al., 1992; Murray 1973, 1975; Lambert and Whitham 2001; Lambert 2005). There is now data that suggests that cheek pouches play a role in dealing with predation. 24 References Alexander, R.D. 1974. The evolution of social behaviour. Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics, 5, 325-383. Anderson, C.M. 1986. Predation and Primate Evolution. Primates, 27(1), 15-39. Barton, R. A. 1993. Sociospatial mechanisms of feeding competition in female olive baboons, Papio anubis. Animal Behaviour, 46, 791-802. Barton, R. and Whiten, A. 1993. Feeding competition among female olive baboons, Papio anubis. Animal Behaviour, 46, 777-789. Boinski, S. & Chapman, C. 1995. Predation on primates: where are we and what’s next? Evolutionary Anthropology, 4, 1-3. Boinski, S., Treves, A. & Chapman, C. 2000. A critical evaluation of the influences of predators on primates: effects on group travel. In: On the move: how and why animals travel in groups (Ed. by S. Boinski & P.A. Garber), pp. 43-72. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press. Caldecott, O. 1986. An ecological and behavioural study of the pig-tailed macaque. In: Contributions to Primatology, Vol. 21, pp. 262. New York: Karger. Cheney, D. & Wrangham, R. 1987. Predation. In: Primate Societies (Ed. by B. Smuts, D. Cheney, R. Seyfarth, R. Wrangham, & T. Struhsaker), pp. 227-239. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press. Cheney, D. L. & Seyfarth, R.M. 1981. Selective forces affecting the predator alarm calls of vervet monkeys. Behaviour, 76, 25-61. Cowlishaw, G. 1993. Trade-offs between feeding competition and predation risk in baboons. Ph.D. thesis, University College London. Cowlishaw, G. 1994. Vulnerability to predation in baboon populations. Behaviour, 131, 293304. Cowlishaw, G. 1998. The role of vigilance in the survival and reproductive strategies of desert baboons. Behaviour, 135, 431-452. Dunbar, R.I.M. 1988. Primate Social Systems. Croom Helm. London and Sydney. Fleagle, J.G. & Reed, K.E. 1999. Phylogenetic and temporal perspectives on primate ecology. In J.G. Fleagle, Janson, C.H. & K.E. Reed (Eds.) Primate Communities. Cambridge University Press. Cambridge Gutiérrez Diego, V. 2003. The Use and Function of Cheek pouches in yellow baboons (Papio cynocephalus) in Mikumi National Park, Tanzania. Ph.D thesis, Universidad Complutense de Madrid. Hamilton, W.D. 1971. Geometry for the selfish herd. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 7, 295311. Hill, R.A. & Dunbar, R. 1998. An evaluation of the roles of predation rate and predation risk as selective pressures on primate grouping behaviour. Behaviour, 135, 411-430. 25 Hill, R.A. & Cowlishaw, G. 2002. Foraging female baboons exhibit similar patterns of antipredator vigilance across two populations. In L.E. Miller (Ed.) Eat or be eaten. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Isbell, L. 1994. Predation on primates: ecological patterns and evolutionary consequences. Evolutionary Anthropology, 3, 61-71. Lambert, J. 1998. A field investigation into the behavioral and food processing function of the cercopithecine cheek pouch. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, Supplement 26, 145-146. Lambert, J. 2005. Competition, predation, and the evolutionary significance of the Cercopithecine cheek pouch: the case of Cercopithecus and Lophocebus. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 126:183-192. Lambert, J. and Whitham, J.C. (2001). Cheek pouch use in Papio cynocephalus. Folia Primatologica, 72:89-91. Lindburg, D. 1971. The rhesus monkey in north India: an ecological and behavioural study. In: Primate Behavior: developments in field and laboratory research (Ed. by L. Rosenblum) New York: Academic Press. Martin, P. & Bateson, P. 1986. Measuring Behaviour. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Maruhashi, T. 1993. Cheek pouch use and primate evolution. Primates Research. 9, 235244. Murray, P. 1973. The anatomy and adaptive significance of cheek pouches (Bursae Buccales) in the Cercopithecinae, Cercopithecoidea. PhD thesis, The University of Chicago. Norton, G.W., Rhine, R.J., Wynn, G.W. & Wynn, R.D. 1987. Baboon diet: a five-year study of stability and variability in the plant feeding and habitat of the yellow baboons (Papio cynocephalus) of Mikumi National Park, Tanzania. Folia Primatologica 48, 78120. Oates, J.F. 1994. The natural history of African colobines. In: Colobine monkeys: their ecology, behaviour and evolution (Ed. by A.G. Davies, & J.F. Oates), pp. 75-128. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Plavcan, J.M. & van Schaik, C.P. 1992. Intrasexual selection and canine dimorphism in anthropoid primates. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 87, 461-477 Rhine, R. 1986. Ten years of cooperative baboon research at Mikumi National Park. In J.G. Else and P.C. Lee (Eds.) Primate ontogeny, cognition and social behaviour. Cambridge University Press. Cambridge. Rhine, R. & Westlund, B. 1981. Adult male positioning in baboon progressions: order and chaos revisited. Folia Primatologica, 35, 77-116. Richard, A. F. 1985. Primates in nature. New York: W. H. Freeman & Company. Ron, T., Henzi, P. Motro, U. 1996. Do female chacma baboons compete for a safe spatial position in a southern wookland habitat? Behavior 133: 475-490. 26 Stanford, C.B. 2002. Avoiding predators: expectations and evidence in primate antipredator behavior. International Journal of Primatology, 23(4), 741-757. Steenbeek, R., Piek, R.C., van Buul, M. & van Hoof, J. 1999. Vigilance in wild Thomas's langurs (Presbytis thomasi): the importance of infanticide risk. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 45, 137-150. Struhsaker, T.T. 1981. Polyspecific association among tropical rainforest primates. Zeitschrift Tierpsychology, 57, 268-304. Treves, A. 2000. Theory and method in studies of vigilance and aggregation. Animal Behaviour, 60, 711-722. Treves, A. 2003. Modeling vigilance remains unrealistic. Behavioural Processes, 63 (3), 137138. van Noordwijk, M. & van Schaik, C. 1987. Competition among female long-tailed macaques, macaca fascicularis. Animal Behaviour, 35, 577-589. van Schaik, C. 1983. Why are diurnal primates living in groups? Behaviour, 87, 120-144. van Schaik, C. 1989. The ecology of social relationships amongst female primates. In: Comparative Socioecology (Ed. by V. Standen and R.A. Foley), pp. 195–218. Oxford: Blackwell Scientific Publications. van Schaik, C. & van Noordwijk, M. 1988. Scramble and contest in feeding competition among female long-tailed macaques (Macaca fascicularis). Behaviour, 105, 77-98 Whitten, P.L. 1983. Diet and dominance among female vervet monkeys (Cercopithecus aethiops). American Journal of Primatology, 5, 139-159. Zuberbühler, K. & Jenny, D. 2002. Leopard predation and primate evolution. Journal of Human Evolution, 43, 873-886. 27