Creativity : Children’s development and individual differences Todd I. Lubart University of Paris V, France The capacity for creative thinking—the capacity to generate ideas that both original and adaptive to situational constraints—develops during childhood. This presentation will examine how the nature of creativity evolves with age and the cognitive, conative and environmental factors that are most relevant. Drawing on results from a series of cross-sectional and longitudinal studies of 6- to 16-year olds, several key issues will be discussed. We will look, for example, at the extent to which creativity is a general ability at each age or a set of specific abilities; do children tend to be creative to the same extent in a set of diverse tasks? Our findings suggest that the generality of creative ability varies with development. Next, the role of certain cognitive abilities will be examined. For example, results on the relationship between children’s creative thought and their ability for logical thought suggests that at some moments the development of these two capacities may be linked. In terms of conative factors the development of creativity-relevant traits such as risk taking will be discussed. Next, the importance of the environment for creative development will be highlighted. In particular, the extent to which the way that rules govern family life influence creativity will be addressed, with analyses contrasting families that have rigid, flexible or loose systems of rules for children. Finally, implications of this work promoting creativity through educational programs will be suggested. The evolving nature of creativity during childhood According to some authors, creativity is a gift, predetermined for each individual according to his or her genetic endowment. For others, creativity may have some genetic bases but is in large part due to environmental circumstances, with family and school playing an important role of catalyst. In any case, it seems that young children are not ready from day 1 to be creative in the same way as adults. For example, very young children, such as 2 year olds who are just learning to speak, may produce unusual utterances without really understanding the sense of the words that they have put together. Adults may interpret these children’s utterances in creative ways; however, is this creativity in the same sense as an adult who purposively breaks with linguistic tradition to express a new idea through modern poetry? In fact, one of the key capacities that develops, I would argue around 4 years old, is an understanding!! of constraints that must be respected when solving a problem or completing a task. Children thus begin at a certain moment to be able to generate behaviors that fit with implicit or explicit rules. We may potentially discuss creativity only when novel thinking can be “controlled” to fit within some boundaries, some constraints. Another important development that seems to occur for children is a shift in the way that ideas, including novel ones, are sought. Up until approximately 10 years of age, although this varies across individuals, associative thinking seems to be the main mechanism for seeking ideas. Indeed, sometimes this associative mechanism makes it difficult to follow “logically” the sequence of thoughts that children produce when trying to solve problems. However, with cognitive development, certain logical thinking capacities are put in place, as has been shown in the extensive literature on cognitive development. Around 10- to 12-years old, a more logically!!-oriented strategy for ideas becomes available to most children and provides a powerful alternative way to seek new ideas. This is another key developmental step in the evolution of creative thinking. Some specific data concerning this idea will be presented a little later. In fact, all throughout its development, creative thinking is linked to cognitive and social development. Some authors postulate, for example, that many adolescents experience a decline in creativity (which is temporary for some and permanent for others). This decline is attributed to the emergence of strong peer pressures around 13 to 16 years old to fit with one’s peers. To illustrate the basic idea that creative thinking unfolds during childhood and slowly reaches its adult form, consider some studies that Christophe Mouchiroud and I conducted on the generality and specificity of creative capacity for children from 6 to 16 years old (Mouchiroud & Lubart, in press). We were interested in whether children tend to show a general creative capacity or rather a set of specific creative capacities. Studies with adults show that the correlations between creative thinking tasks, such as writing an original short story and making a creative drawing, tend to be positive but weak, often near r = .30 . This correlation, a measure of concordance between performance on two tasks for a sample of individuals indicates that task performance is not completely independent but is not highly linked either. It is interesting to examine whether children show the same partial task specificity or if they have a generalized creative capacity that, with increasing life experiences, becomes more and more specialized. We explored these ideas in a cross-sectional study in which 90 children aged 6- to 11-years old completed a set of creative divergent thinking tasks. These tasks included certain parts of the Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking, such as generating many unusual uses for a cardboard box, a storytelling task, and a set of tasks that we created concerning creative thinking in social situations. An example of these social creativity tasks is having children generate as many original ideas as possible in order to join a group of children who are already playing a game, or ideas to convince one’s parents to give their permission to let the child watch television later than usual. We found that the social creativity tasks correlated rather well between themselves for each age group but for 6- to 7-year olds these tasks showed very weak correlations with object-oriented creative tasks, such as the box task, and with the story task. This suggests that at an early age, there are specific kinds of creativity and children may show different levels of performance for each kind. Concerning the results for 7-8 year olds, the pattern of correlations distinguishing different kinds of tasks was less clear. Finally for the 11- to 12-year olds, there were relatively strong correlations between all the various creative-thinking tasks (social, object-oriented, short story). Thus for this age group we may refer to a general capacity for creative thinking. In a second study with children aged 11- to 16 years old, we replicated the initial results for the 11- to 12-year old group and observed that the correlations between tasks were lower and decreased for the older age groups. The 15-16 year old group was rather similar to adults, showing a high degree of task specificity in their creative performance. Thus, there seems to be developmental periods when creativity is more general and other periods when it is more domain or task specific. We have proposed, based on other findings, that these developmental trends are linked to concurrent development, of general verbal intelligence, and of domain specific expertise through life experience. In any case, this research illustrates that the nature of creativity seems to vary with age and develop in children. A multivariate approach to creativity and its development Recently, a number of authors have developed a multivariate approach to creativity in which an individual’s creative potential is viewed as the result of a set of cognitive abilities and task-relevant knowledge, personality traits and motivations, and environmental factors that may facilitate or inhibit creative thinking (Amabile, 1996; Lubart, 1999; Sternberg & Lubart, 1995). For example, important cognitive abilities include the insight relevant capacities of selectively encoding, comparing and combining information, divergent thinking, and evaluation of one’s emerging ideas. Relevant personality traits are risk taking, individuality, openness, perseverance and tolerance of ambiguity. The environment involves a person’s family, school or work situation, and the broader cultural context. Environments may be physically rich (full of stimuli that are sources of ideas), socially rich (full of interesting people for exchanging ideas and challenging one’s thoughts), or emotionally rich (e.g., providing support against the rejection of new ideas). When these components combine interactively it is possible to understand the wide range of individual differences observed in children and adults. It is, in particular, possible (but statistically rare) that all of the components are optimized for a given individual and exceptional levels of creativity result. Consider now some developmental work concerning these cognitive, conative and environmental factors. Cognition As mentioned, a range of intellectual capacities have been found to be important for creativity. In our recent work, we have devised specific measures of some of these capacities. For example, we constructed a test of sensitivity to cognitive change using visual stimuli, which relates to selective encoding capacity. In this test, a child sees a series of images that change slightly each time (e.g., the face of a lion that becomes a face of a monkey). The child must indicate what is portrayed and we score when the child notes that the initial object or animal displayed has become something different. This particular test correlates with creativity in a drawing task in children. In another line of work on the cognitive basis of creativity, we conducted semi-longitudinal research with 8- to 12-year-old children. We examined how performance changes with age on the verbal subtests of the Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking and storytelling tasks. We found that the mean developmental patterns vary according to the task, with a temporary slump in performance for certain tasks around 9- to 10-years old. These tasks concerned finding unusual uses for well-known objects (a box and a string). Close examination of children’s responses suggested that there was a reorganization around 9- to 10-years old concerning the way that children search their knowledge base for new ideas. Younger children engaged in a primarily free associative search, whereas from 10 year old many children employed more systematic search patterns based on logical category structures. Interestingly, certain modes of logical thinking become fully operational after 9 years o!!ld according to post-piagetian studies. Thus, it seems that creative development and cognitive development are closely connected. Conation In terms of the role of personality traits and motivation in children’s creativity and development, a number of studies have begun to look at how traits such as risk taking develop and play a role in creativity or how intrinsic and extrinsic motivators influence children’s creative performance. In particular, Amabile, Hennessey and their collaborators have worked on this latter topic. I would like to mention briefly a study that I find interesting for thinking about personality, creativity and the potential impact of schools on its development. Clifford (1988) examined children’s risk taking in academic situations. She asked children at various grade levels (10- to 12-year olds) to solve problems of their own choice in verbal, mathematics, and other academic domains. The problems were clearly labeled as being appropriate for average children of various ages (age 6 to 14 years old). The results were somewhat surprising and disturbing. Children aged 10-years old selected problems designed for their age group, 11-year olds tended to chose problems geared towards 10 year olds, and for the oldest children the difference was even larger between real age and the age-level of the problems selected. This shows that children were increasingly risk averse with age, which is smart for getting good grades in school. This result, which we replicated with somewhat different tasks in France, can be considered disturbing, however, in terms of creativity because creative thinking involves taking risks, going against standard ideas, exposing oneself to failure and negative comments from peers, teachers, or parents. In research with adults, we have found that people who respond to hypothetical domain-specific situation scenarios in a way that shows willingness to take risk tend to produce more creative work in tasks in the domain examined (Lubart & Sternberg, 1995). These considerations among others led to the development of what we call the investment approach to creativity (Sternberg & Lubart, 1995). In this view, many people are not creative because they are unwilling to pursue unknown or little valued ideas, they don’t “buy low.” Being creative is, in part, a philosophy of life, which is acquired through childhood experiences. Environment Several authors have proposed that one of the key influences on creative development is the physical and social environment of the child. The family environment, for example, may provide cognitive (e.g., intellectual stimulation) and affective (e.g., emotional security) support for creativity as well as providing the physical setting in which a child grows (Harrington, Block & Block, 1987). Research by Lautrey (1980) and others has explored a social-cognitive dimension of the family environment, the “structure of family rules” for daily life, which has been found to influence intellectual development. In particular, families that have some structured rules to guide children but modify occasionally these rules provide both the structure and the perturbations of this structure that foster cognitive development. The families with “flexible” rules can be contrasted with those having “rigid” rules and those that lack rules in general. In a series of studies with Parisian children (7 to 12 years old), we examined how these variations in family rule systems are related to creativity. Children completed creativity tasks from the Torrance battery (which focus on divergent thinking) and parents responded to a questionnaire on family life. This parental questionnaire contained 20 items that measured whether each family’s system of rules for children was rigid, flexible, or unstructured. The questionnaire also assessed demographic and socioeconomic status. A sample question is : When your child plays at home : a) Your child can play only in certain places that you specified. (rigid rule) b) You have specified places for playing games but your child can play outside of these areas under certain circumstances. (flexible rule) c) Your child plays wherever he/she wants. (lack of rule) To characterize the family environment, we use two types of scores : (a) The number of questionnaire responses indicating a flexible rule structure (a continuous variable). (b) A categorization of each family as either predominantly flexible in its rule structure or predominantly rigid (a categorical variable). We found that the number of “ flexible ” rule responses (by parents) correlated positively with creative performance (Fluency) of children (correlations ranging from .12 to .4- depending on the sample, and the creativity task). The analysis of contrasted groups confirmed the correlational trends and showed that children from families with mainly “rigid” parental rules were, on average, less creative than families characterized mainly by “flexible” rules. Analyses concerning socioeconomic status show three results. First, creative task performance is related to SES (high SES, greater fluency). Second, family rule structure is rel!!ated to SES (high SES associated with flexible rules and low SES associated with rigid rules). Third, and most importantly, the positive relationship between flexible family rules and creative task performance occurs at each level of SES. Thus, our research studies indicate that there is a positive relationship between the flexibility of family rules and creative task performance in children (measured by the number of ideas produced). This result seems to hold across socioeconomic levels and remains after intelligence-related variables (such as factor g) are controlled statistically. In summary, the results suggest that family rule structure shows a small but consistent relationship to creative task performance. Family rule structure may be one of the components of the environment that influences creativity in children Implications for Educational settings The different points highlighted in this presentation lead to several recommendations for developing children’s creativity through educational programs. These programs may be school-based or family-based. First, knowledge of how creativity develops and its links with general cognitive and social development will help educators to devise programs that fit with the capacities and zone of proximal development for children at each age. In other words, what we may propose to foster creativity for 6 year olds will not be the same as what we would propose to 12 year olds. Second, most existing creativity enhancement programs focus on training children to think divergently. This is a valuable but limited intervention because there are other relevant cognitive abilities that also can be trained. In addition, following the logic of the multivariate approach to creativity presented earlier, we need to provide exercises that foster relevant conative aspects of creativity, such as practicing taking risks, and seeing that failure is not to be avoided at all costs. Finally, in terms of the environment, we need to understand that schools and family settings provide role models for children. Research suggests that many teachers and parents do not really value creativity and consider an ideal child to be one who sits quietly, asks questions of clarification and does not rock the boat. Thus, if we want really to foster creativity we may need programs that try to educate parents and teachers as well as to promote creative thinking directly in children. References Amabile, T. M. (1996). Creativity in context. Boulder, CO: Westview. Clifford, M. M. (1988). Failure tolerance and academic risk taking in ten- to twelve-year old students. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 58(1), 15-27. Harrington, D. M., Block, J. H., & Block, J. (1987). Testing aspects of Carl Roger’s theory of creative environments: Child-rearing antecedents of creative potential in young adolescents. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52(4), 851-856. Lautrey, J. (1980). Classe Sociale, Milieu Familial, Intelligence. [Social Class, Family Environment, and Intelligence]. Paris : Presses Universitaires de France. Lubart, T. I. (1999). Componential models of creativity. In M. A. Runco & S. Pritzer (Eds.) Encyclopedia of creativity (pp. 295-300). New York : Academic Press. Lubart, T. I., & Sternberg, R. J. (1995). An investment approach to creativity: Theory and data. In S. Smith, T. Ward, & R. Finke (Eds.) The creative cognition approach (pp. 271-302). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Mouchiroud, C. & Lubart, T. I. (in press). Social creativity: A cross-sectional study of 6to 11-year old children. International Journal of Behavioral Development. Sternberg, R. J., & Lubart, T. I. (1995). Defying the crowd: Cultivating creativity in a culture of conformity. New York: Free Press.