Cognitive and Personality Running head: INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES IN SUSCEPTIBILITY TO FALSE MEMORY Cognitive and Personality Differences Leading to Differential Induction of False Memories Ari Silbermann Cornell University 1 Cognitive and Personality 2 Abstract The purpose of this study was to examine how individual differences in cognitive and personality factors contribute to commissions of false memory. Seventy-eight Cornell University students were tested using the Deese-Roediger-McDermott (DRM) paradigm to elicit false recall. Word lists varied in qualitative components such as concreteness and associative strength. Participants completed a task assessing field dependence and surveys measuring mental absorption and creativity. Results showed similar trends in false recall rates to previous studies. Findings also revealed significant associations between creation of false memories and field dependence, less detailed mental imagery, and lower levels of self-assessed creativity. Concreteness level was found to be an important list-based factor in explaining how individual differences trigger false memory creation. Cognitive and Personality 3 Cognitive and Personality Differences Leading to Differential Induction of False Memories False memories can consist of either remembering items or events that never happened, or remembering them differently from the way they actually occurred. Laboratory methods have been successful in eliciting false memories from participants in a variety of situations (see Roediger, 1996 for a review). Two questions dominate research on false memories: are there specific types of tests or situations that lead to higher levels of false memory creation? Are there individual differences that cause some people to be more susceptible to creating false memories than others? The purpose of this study is to examine the latter of these questions in order to reveal if there are particular differences in cognitive processing or personality traits that lead to the reporting of more false memories, and to determine how these characteristics are associated with qualitative errors in memory. The Deese-Roediger-McDermott Paradigm False memories in this study were elicited by the associative priming procedure created for free recall by Deese (1959) and expanded by Roediger and McDermott (1995), referred to as the Deese-Roediger-McDermott (DRM) paradigm. In this procedure, participants are given lists of words containing common associates of a non-presented critical word. The non-presented words associated with the other items in the list are called target words or critical lures. For example, the following list converges on the missing word sweet: sour, candy, sugar, bitter, good, taste, tooth, nice, honey, soda, chocolate, heart, cake, tart, pie. After a study period, participants are asked to recall the words they were given. They are told to include only words they are confident were in fact present in the lists. Despite the immediate recall, participants will often falsely recall ‘sweet’ and describe their experience while remembering it (Roediger & Cognitive and Personality 4 McDermott, 1995). These researchers were able to replicate false recall of target words in other lists as well. The DRM paradigm is unique in that most other measures assessing false memory include stories, visual stimuli, and persuasive information—in other words an accumulation of detail—whereas word lists do not, at least at first glance, elicit a complex scenario to remember. It has taken a relatively long period of time from the original introduction of this measure for it to become a popular assessment in the study of false memory. Deese (1959) was only interested in how associations between words affected both veridical and false recall, and not the actual intrusions of the target words. Therefore, the fact that many of his lists elicited only few intrusions did not affect his purposes, and he did not extensively address these intrusion findings (Deese, 1959; Roediger, Watson, McDermott, & Gallo, 2001). Later, Roediger and McDermott (1995) decided to target false memory in their research and expanded the paradigm. Since this expansion, the DRM paradigm has been used in a multitude of studies examining various aspects of false memory. Theories Explaining False Memory Reproductive vs. Reconstructive Memory Bartlett (1932) is recognized as one of the first researchers to investigate false memory. In his study, he instructed the participants to read and later recall a Native American folktale, “The War of the Ghosts.” Along with numerous errors of omission in the story, subjects also made errors of commission, in which they added material to the story to make it more coherent (Roediger, 1996). Although his specific results have not been successfully replicated, Bartlett made a lasting contribution by piquing people’s interest in false memory, and in making the distinction between reproductive and reconstructive memory. Reproductive memory includes the Cognitive and Personality 5 accurate production of content from memory. Conversely, reconstructive memory refers to the process of filling in absent components during the recall procedure. According to Bartlett (1932), the process of reconstructing memory is marked by frequent errors. Bartlett also claimed that memories of hearing stories and participating in events involve error-prone reconstruction, while simple materials such as word lists spawn accurate, reproductive memory. However, with the introduction of the DRM paradigm, Roediger and McDermott (1995) suggested that Bartlett’s (1932) distinction be abandoned, and that in some way all memory is reconstructive. Recoding The information processing approach to cognition began in the 1950s and with it came new ideas about false memory. Miller (1956) introduced the concept called recoding to describe the process that receives information from the outside world in one form and subsequently transforms and represents it in a different way internally. One of the simplest methods of recoding is the grouping or chunking of a sequence of incoming information. Miller demonstrated this feat by presenting the findings of Smith’s (1954) investigation regarding techniques of repeating long strings of binary digits by mentally combining them in different ratios. Referring to this general process Miller (1956) states, “There are many ways to do this recoding, but probably the simplest is to group the input events, apply a new name to the group, and then remember the new name rather than the original input events” (p. 93). Since recoding is an active process performed by the individual, it is possible to commit errors either during recoding or during later decoding of the information. Activation Monitoring False memories in the DRM paradigm may be developed by an automatic spread of activation from presented words to critical lures (Watson, Bunting, Poole, & Conway, 2005). Cognitive and Personality 6 Balota et al. (1999) suggested that avoiding a false memory requires the person to differentiate between studied words and highly activated critical lures. Therefore, false memories in this paradigm may also be due to a failure of monitoring systems to differentiate between actual presentation and critical word activation. Source Monitoring Source monitoring (Johnson, Hashtroudi, & Lindsay, 1993) refers to the set of processes used to determine or attribute the origin of remembered information. The Source Monitoring Framework (SMF) is an extension of the reality monitoring framework originally proposed by Johnson and Raye (1981). According to the SMF, there are three important types of source monitoring: external source monitoring, internal source monitoring, and internal-external reality monitoring. External source monitoring refers to the discrimination among externally-derived sources, for example, discriminating between memories of statements made by one person and those of statements made by another person. Internal source monitoring includes distinguishing among internally-generated sources, for example, differentiating memories of what one thought from memories of what one said. Lastly, internal-external reality monitoring involves discriminating memories of externally-generated information (e.g. perceived items and events) from memories of internally-generated information (e.g. thoughts and imaginings). Memories for perceived events tend to include more spatial, temporal, and perceptual information than do memories for imagined events (Johnson, Hashtroudi, & Lindsay, 1993). Therefore, a memory containing more visual and spatial detail is likely to be judged as externally derived. Because of this relationship, those individuals who have the tendency to have more detailed mental imagery may more often commit a source monitoring error. In addition, conditions that promote the Cognitive and Personality 7 perceptual similarity of internally and externally derived events will increase the probability of confusing the two types of sources. Fuzzy-Trace Theory The fuzzy-trace theory (FTT; Reyna & Brainerd, 1995) is a framework for the interaction between memory and higher reasoning processes. According to the theory, events are simultaneously stored in verbatim and gist traces. Verbatim traces are episode or item specific and are usually accompanied by feelings of specific recollection, whereas gist or semantic traces elicit a sense of familiarity. Memory’s performance is based on the retrieval of both verbatim and gist traces. Falsely remembered objects or events usually fit within the gist of experience, whether they are associated with autobiographical memory, eyewitness testimony, or recall of word lists (Brainerd & Reyna, 2002). According to this theory, verbatim retrieval is more likely to occur when verbatim traces are dominant compared to gist traces, and vice versa. Verbatim and gust traces can be expected to have separate effects on false memory, because verbatim traces are associated with the reexperiencing of items or events in context, called recollection, while gist trace retrieval occurs when items or events that were not originally experienced are perceived to resemble actual experienced items. As the level of familiarity increases, “phantom recollections” can develop (Brainerd & Reyna, 2002). Phantom recollections are defined by the researchers as “illusory vivid mental reinstatements of events’ occurrence” (Brainerd & Reyna, 2002, p. 167). Therefore, when multiple words are associated with a non-presented word, such as in the DRM paradigm, gist retrieval would support false memory because of familiarity, while verbatim retrieval would stifle false memory. Cognitive and Personality 8 Distraction Prior studies have included stimuli to act as distractors during the memorization and recall process. The impact that these distractors have on memory ability can be explained by the Working Memory (WM) model. Baddeley and Hitch (1974) introduced the WM model, a tripartite system that is comprised of the articulatory loop (AL), the visuo-spatial sketchpad (VSSP), and an attentional component called the central executive (CE). The AL is responsible for the verbal component of short-term memory and is thought to be constructed in two parts – an active rehearsal process and a passive phonological store (Baddeley, 1990). While the AL has been the focus of many studies, including those on IQ performance (Ellis & Hennelly, 1980) and language acquisition (Gathercole & Baddeley, 1989), the VSSP is less understood as the visual memory component of the WM model. Logie (1986) tested the VSSP by presenting irrelevant pictures to subjects as they processed stimuli, either verbal or visual, in an attempt to later recall the items. In the verbal processing task, participants’ performance was unaffected by the irrelevant visual material, while this same material produced diminished results when accompanying visual processing. Learning from this effective methodology, Quinn and McConnell (1996) divided their participants into a rote instruction condition and a visual mnemonic condition. Those in the rote instruction group were told to commit words to memory by mentally adding each word to the one heard previously. Those in the visual mnemonic group were told to learn a list of pegwords (one is bun, two is shoe, etc.) and to use them to create a visual image of each word heard (for example, if a participant’s pegword for one is bun and the first word presented is lion, they would imagine a lion eating a bun as a strategy for remembering the word). During word presentation, Quinn and McConnell (1996) matched the timing of the item being said with the Cognitive and Personality 9 presentation of a visual image (similar to Logie, 1986). However, there were two distinct types of images displayed. The researchers called one type dynamic visual noise because the change in the display was constant and evenly distributed. The other type, displays consisting of line drawings that changed both inconsistently and randomly, was designed to engage attentional mechanisms. They found that the latter, irrelevant line drawings disrupted memory performance in participants in both conditions (rote and visual mnemonic instruction). The more intriguing finding was that dynamic visual noise only disrupted recall in the visual mnemonic group. These results suggest that the attentional disruption of the line drawings affects the overall CE, and the effect therefore trickles down to both slave systems, the AL and the VSSP. Nevertheless, it seems that the VSSP is specifically susceptible to irrelevant dynamic visual noise. This idea is supported by the finding that rote learning ability, handled in the AL, is undisrupted by dynamic visual noise. Hence, in this condition, unlike the visual mnemonic condition, the AL was able to make up for any interference in the VSSP. Individual Differences in Susceptibility to False Memory Despite the great amount of research being conducted with the DRM paradigm, relatively few studies have looked into whether individual differences in young adults affect the likelihood of false memories using this measure (Marmurek & Hamilton, 2000; Winograd, Peluso, & Glover, 1998). Nevertheless, the studies that have been conducted have revealed that certain cognitive and personality characteristics may be involved in the process of committing errors in memory. Absorption & Mental Imagery In their work on memory and consciousness, Jacoby, Kelley, and Dywan (1989) hypothesized that the ease with which a person is able to bring vivid events to mind increases the Cognitive and Personality 10 probability that the person will attribute it to being a memory. This theory is similar to that of Horselenberg, Merckelbach, van Breukelen, and Wessel, (2004), which states that individuals with high fantasy proneness, dissociation, and/or absorption could be susceptible to false memories because they misinterpret their detailed imagery as true memories. Tellegen and Atkinson (1974) define absorption as the tendency for an individual to have periods of complete attention that fully utilize one’s representational resources, including perception and imagination. The authors claim that absorption results in both a heightened and an altered sense of reality. In order to assess individual differences in absorption, the researchers created the Tellegen Absorption Scale (TAS). Participants are asked to assess their agreement with each of the 34 items on the scale (e.g. “When I listen to music I can get so caught up in it that I don’t notice anything else”, “Sometimes images come to me without the slightest effort on my part”). The higher an individual scores on the TAS, the greater the person’s capacity for mental absorption. The TAS and other related measures of mental imagery have been important in assessing individual differences in susceptibility to false memory. For example, Platt, Lacey, Iobst, and Finkelman (1998) tested participants’ memories at 6, 12, and 18 months following the O.J. Simpson criminal trial verdict, coupled with administration of the TAS. The investigators found that higher scores on the TAS (higher mental absorption) were associated with lower autobiographical memory accuracy. They concluded that subjects were not inaccurate because they did not remember the event, but rather because their memory of the event was distorted. Additionally, because the test for memory accuracy was performed as much as a year prior to the TAS testing, the authors argued that the relationship found between to two variables seemed to be consistent over time, under the assumption that the TAS measures a trait rather than a state. In Cognitive and Personality 11 a similar study, Hyman and Pentland (1996) asked participants to attempt to recall childhood events. Based on information from the parents of the participants, subjects were interviewed about several true events and one false event. During a separate session, the TAS and a similar measure of vividness of mental imagery, the Creative Imagination Scale (CIS; Wilson & Barber, 1978), were administered to groups of participants. Their results showed that higher imagery levels increased the probability of creating false memories. In addition, scores on the TAS and CIS were positively associated with each other. Findings by Dobson and Markham (1993) also support the hypothesis that more detailed imagers are more susceptible to false memories. They presented subjects of high and low visual imagery, measured by Marks’ (1973) Vividness of Visual Imagery Questionnaire (VVIQ), with a short film followed by text passages describing the events of the film. Only some of the passages were true. Participants were then asked whether certain information came from the film only, from the text only, presented in both, or not presented at all. The researchers found that lowimagery participants made more correct discriminations than high-imagery participants. They suggest that this result was due to the high-imagery group’s tendency to attribute pictorial images sparked by the recognition test statements to the film because their images included a lot of visual detail. This error represents a mistake in source monitoring (Johnson, Hashtroudi, & Lindsay, 1993). Despite the research demonstrating a link between higher levels of imagery and instances of false memory, some investigators have claimed that imagery can be advantageous to the remembering process. Jacoby, Levi, and Steinbach (1992) present the distinction between datadriven processing and conceptually-driven processing. In the memory context, data-driven processing refers to the mechanism that analyzes a stimulus input, while conceptually-driven Cognitive and Personality 12 processing includes the processing of contextual information and situational knowledge. These latter areas are where false memories are more likely to occur. Stimuli involving lower levels of analysis, such as written letters and words utilize more data-driven processing and usually do not require a high degree of conceptually-driven processing. Therefore, the probability of internallyderiving a false memory, such as producing a critical lure not presented in a word list in the DRM paradigm, may be reduced when encoding processes are limited to data-driven processing. In this case, more detailed imagers may commit less errors of false memory, because stimuli usually associated with concept-driven processing are limited. In order to test the hypothesis that detailed imagery has both positive and negative effects on false memory, Marmurek and Hamilton (2000) utilized mental imagery as both an individual difference and an experimental variable using DRM paradigm word lists presented in an auditory modality. The investigators divided the participants into three groups: a referential imaging (concept-driven) group, a font imaging (data-driven encoding) group, and a control group. Each group was instructed to use different study techniques to remember the word list items. Those in the referential imaging group were instructed to form a mental image that they felt was closely associated to each word on the list. This instruction is similar to that told to the visual mnemonic group in the study by Quinn and McConnell (1996). For the font imaging group, a picture displaying three sets of the alphabet, each in a different font, was presented in the front of the room. For each word, the participant was asked to picture the item written in one of the three fonts being shown, designated randomly by the instructor. The control group received no instructions regarding how to study the words. Following the word lists, a picture test was given to assess mental imagery ability. Their results showed that referential imaging led to higher rates of both correct and false recall when compared to font imaging techniques. Moreover, this Cognitive and Personality 13 pattern was only significant for good imagers. In addition, font imaging led to fewer false recalls for more detailed imagers compared to less detailed imagers. In fact, the overall proportion of recalling non-presented target words was lower for more detailed imagers than for less detailed imagers, a borderline significant finding. Therefore, these results support the theory of Jacoby, Levi, and Steinbach (1992) claiming that when encoding processes are data-driven, in this case font imaging of words, more detailed imagers are more successful. Roediger and McDermott (1995) suggest that associative processes may lead to the production of false memories. Instruction to imagine referents may elicit false recall if the mental images contain perceptual details that are consistent with the non-presented word. Susceptibility to creating false memories may then be a function of the ability to generate imaginative details. According to this formulation, less detailed imagers would be less likely to produce perceptual information that would eventually lead to associative-based source monitoring failures (Johnson, Hashtroudi, & Lindsay, 1993). On the other hand, more detailed imagers may be superior than less detailed imagers at imagining words presented in a visual modality because these stimuli would not lead to the formation of perceptual details, and are limited to data-driven processing (Marmurek & Hamilton, 2000). The focus for these detailed imagers remains solely on remembering the actual presented words. Prior false memory research has been conducted under the assumption that imagery is an undifferentiated construct, classifying individuals as either good or bad imagers. Blajenkova, Kozhevnikov, and Motes (2006) offer two distinct subsystems that process and encode visual information in different ways: object imagery and spatial imagery. Object imagery includes the representations of literal appearances of objects in terms of their precise form, size, and color. Spatial imagery refers to relatively abstract representations of the spatial relations among objects, Cognitive and Personality 14 locations of objects in space, and movements of objects. A study by Farah, Hammond, Levine, and Calvanio (1988) defends these classifications by demonstrating that these two imagery systems are anatomically and neurologically distinct. Kozhevnikov, Kosslyn, and Shephard (2005) found that those who prefer to use verbal-analytical information over imagery, called verbalizers, typically performed at an ‘intermediate’ level on imagery tasks, while visualizers (those who prefer to process information visually) could be divided into two groups: those who excelled on spatial imagery tasks (such as mental rotation) and those that performed well on object imagery tasks (such as the degraded picture test). From these results, the authors concluded that object imagers encoded and processed images holistically, as a single perceptual unit, whereas spatial imagers encoded and processed images analytically, part by part. Another issue concerns interpretation of the TAS in its entirety as representing capacity for imagery. It is clear from an examination of the items that some involve increased sensory and emotional responsiveness to stimuli, either external (“I can be deeply moved by a sunset”) or internal stimuli (“If I wish I can imagine (or daydream) some things so vividly that they hold my attention as a good movie or a story does”), while others involve something that can be best called extrasensory perception or perhaps an error in sensorial monitoring of the environment (“At times I somehow feel the presence of someone who is not physically there”). Though the differences between these two types of items are clear, there has been relatively little effort to analyze the factor structure of the scale, in part because the focus of research examining this scale has been on its relationship to hypnotizability; for the purpose of detecting degrees of hypnotizability, the Tellegen Absorption Scale in its entirety works very well, probably because hypnotizability involves both heightened sensory responsiveness coupled with a form of error monitoring to determine what is and what is not real. However, for the purposes of remembering Cognitive and Personality 15 lists, it may be that the ability to become completely engaged in an external stimulus is critical to short term memory as tested by word lists, and difficulty in distinguishing what is real from what is not may be detrimental. Field Dependence Another individual difference researchers have targeted as a potential contributor to susceptibility to creating false memories is field dependence. The concept of field dependence stemmed from the articulated versus global field approach, which emerged from research on individual differences in perceptual and intellectual functioning (Witkin, Goodenough, & Oltman, 1979; Witkin & Goodenough, 1981). Measures to test field dependence include the rodand-frame test (RFT), the body-adjustment test (BAT), and the rotating-room test (RRT). All of these measures assess differences in perception of orientation. For each of these tests, participants vary in the extent to which they refer to the external visual field or to the body itself to locate the true upright. Strategies for determining true upright are consistent across tests, supporting the idea that these individual reference points are related to cognition. From these contrasting strategies, the terms field dependence and field independence were established. The differentiation theory states that less differentiated individuals approach the world in a global, field dependent style, while more differentiated people approach the world in an analytical, field independent manner (Witkin, Goodenough, & Oltman, 1979). Later research revealed that locating the upright was found to be related to the separation of an organized field from the whole (Witkin, Goodenough, & Oltman, 1979). The embedded-figures test (EFT) was introduced in order to test the ability to separate organized fields from the whole. In this test participants are asked to find, or “disembed,” a simple figure within a complex design (Witkin, 1950). Disembedding ability in this context was related to structuring competence (Witkin, Cognitive and Personality 16 Goodenough, & Oltman, 1979). For example, a common aspect of each of these tests is that the individual is forced to restructure their perceptions or symbolic representations in order to meet the requirements of the task. Results revealed that field independent people are better at successfully completing these measures and are therefore seen as superior at cognitive restructuring ability when compared to field dependent individuals. Another measure of field dependence was created by Piaget and Inhelder (1956). The investigators initially developed the water-level task to test utilization of the horizontal coordinate system within a spatial network. The task includes a display of bottles each tilted at different angles. Participants are either told that the bottles are filled halfway with water, or that the level of water is indicated by a dot marked on each bottle. Then, participants are instructed to draw a line representing the water level in each bottle. Although the authors had originally proposed that this ability developed between ages 8 and 9, the Piagetian water-level task has frequently been used to measure, and has found conceptual deficits in, the spatial perceptions of college students (e.g. Thomas, Jamison, & Hummel, 1973; Kenyon, 1984; Rebelsky, 1964). Even with a horizontal line drawn below each bottle (Morris, 1971), or a superimposed grid on the task stimuli (Kelly & Kelly, 1977), large numbers of subjects continue to fail the task. Representation of the horizontal coordinate, measured by the Piagetian water-level task, depends on overcoming the influence of the direct visual field and involves restructuring, a cognitive ability that has been linked to the field dependence-independence variable (Witkin & Goodenough, 1981). Generating a mental representation is a key skill in the component-skills analysis model of water-level task performance (Kalichman, 1988). Furthermore, disembedding skills in this test mediate the generation of a mental image by determining the position of the container relative to its context. In fact, performance on the water-level task has been found to be Cognitive and Personality 17 associated with RFT performance (Willemsen, Buchholz, Budrow, & Geannacopulos, 1973), and the relationship among the water-level task and the RFT have caused some researchers to assume that the two measures are assessing the same construct (Abravanel & Gingold, 1977; DeLisi, 1983). Successful performance on the water-level task is associated with the field independence trait, while failure to indicate the true horizontal indicates field dependence. Field dependence-independence is an individual difference that affects cognition and structuring of mental images. With respect to false memory susceptibility, Witkin, Goodenough, and Oltman (1979) address self-nonself segregation in their differentiation theory by claiming that field independent (FI) individuals are better able to separate the self from the nonself than the field dependent (FD) individuals. This means that FI individuals usually rely more on internally generated information, while FD individuals rely more on externally derived information. This distinction has important implications in source monitoring effects. According to this theory, FD individuals should be expected to confuse the origins of memories more often than FI individuals. In order to assess participants’ memory, Durso, Reardon, and Jolly (1985) presented subjects with a series of sentences, some of which were complete and some of which had an obvious final word omitted. They were then asked to complete the latter sentences types by thinking of the missing word. Field dependence was then assessed using the Group Embedded Figures Tests (GEFT; Witkin, Oltman, Raskin, & Karp, 1971). Following this test, participants were presented with a recognition task in the form of a word list. They were instructed to delineate the source of each word as “thought,” “heard,” or “new”, depending on if they believed the words were omitted words, non-omitted words, or novel, irrelevant words. The researchers found that FI participants were more accurate at identifying the origin of their memories than were FD participants, indicating better source monitoring of memories. Cognitive and Personality 18 Characteristics of Lists as Contributors to False Memory The interaction between individual differences and measures used to elicit false memory may provide important answers to the field of false memory study. Initially, Roediger and McDermott (1995) examined instances of false recall by collapsing the results across all the lists used in the study, a method used by most researchers to follow. Unfortunately, this practice assumes that every list has the same effect on the production of false memory. In order to test this assumption, Stadler, Roediger, and McDermott (1999) then attempted to discover differences between lists in eliciting false recall by finding norms of 36 DRM paradigm word lists. Analyzing the question further, Roediger, Watson, McDermott, and Gallo (2001) examined qualitative characteristics of both individual word lists and their respective critical, nonpresented words to assess their relationship to false recall rates. Interestingly, but not surprisingly, the authors found that the lists varied considerably on a number of different variables. With respect to the word lists used in this study, Stadler, Roediger, and McDermott (1999) found that false recall ranged from 65% for the list associated with the non-presented word window, 54% for the list structured around sweet, down to 10% for the list around king. Starting with Deese (1959), a number of researchers have explored the characteristics of the word lists themselves (Deese, 1959; McEvoy, Nelson, & Komatsu, 1999; Roediger, Watson, McDermott, & Gallo, 2001). Deese (1959) defined interitem associative strength as the average relative frequency with which all items in a list tend to elicit all other items in the same list as free associates. Deese’s other contribution to the concepts involving word list characteristics was mean backward associative strength (BAS), the average tendency for words in the study list to elicit the critical item on a free association test. McEvoy, Nelson, and Komatsu (1999) were the Cognitive and Personality 19 first researchers after Deese to look at list-based factors underlying false memory creation. They reported that lists with high interitem associative strength yielded lower false recall of critical words relative to lists with lower interitem associative strength, whereas lists with high mean BAS yielded greater false recall than those with lower BAS. In other words, associative connections among presented words in each list decrease the probability of false recall, while associative connections from list words to the critical lure increase the probability of false memories. Their findings support those of Deese (1959). Nevertheless, not all lists follow these rules. For example, McEvoy, Nelson, and Komatsu (1999) reported that the king list has a relatively high BAS, which means that it should elicit high levels of false recall. Nevertheless, norms found by Stadler, Roediger, and McDermott (1999) convey the opposite, revealing that the king list elicited the lowest false recall rate. In order to try and discover other variables that may explain these relationships further, Roediger, Watson, McDermott, and Gallo (2001) examined critical word length, word frequency, concreteness, and forward associative strength, in addition to interitem associative strength and mean BAS. Word length was defined as the number of letters in each critical item and was representative of the relative distinctness of each word form. Word frequency was taken from the Kučera and Francis (1967) norms and was defined as the number of cases found in print per million words. Concreteness ratings, representing the tangibility of each critical item, were obtained from the word association norms of Nelson et al. (1999) and were scaled from 1-7 (1 = least concrete to 7 = most concrete). Interitem associative strength and mean BAS were defined in the same way as in previous studies (Deese, 1959; McEvoy, Nelson, & Komatsu, 1999). Forward association strength, a new list variable, was defined as the strength of associative connections from the critical word to the study words, a process that can be interpreted as the Cognitive and Personality 20 inverse of that explaining mean BAS. Veridical recall was measured using the average probability of correctly recalling of the studied words. The researchers found that critical word length was negatively correlated with false recall. The longer the critical word, and therefore the more distinct it is, the less likely it is to elicit false recall. The authors suggest that the higher level of distinctiveness may contribute by simplifying the monitoring process during retrieval. In addition, they found that mean BAS was the strongest correlate of false recall, consistent with previous findings (Deese, 1959; McEvoy, Nelson, & Komatsu, 1999). Testing Modality After considering individual differences and list-based factors as possible elements influencing false recall using word lists, the final issue to take into account is testing modality. The traditional DRM paradigm methodology outlined by Roediger and McDermott (1995) includes group testing by reading the words aloud at a rate of one word per 1.5 seconds. Many authors have reported a dramatic reduction of false memory rates when switching from an auditory study presentation to a visual study presentation (Smith & Hunt, 1998; Gallo, McDermott, Percer, & Roediger, 2001; Kellogg, 2001). For instance, Robinson and Roediger (1997) tested participants using a computer, which displayed each word on the screen for two seconds. They reported probabilities of overall false recall at .31 for 15-word lists, compared with a false recall rate of .55 for 15-word lists reported by Roediger and McDermott (1995). Smith and Hunt (1998) suggest that visual presentation of list words allows for more distinctive item-specific processing than does auditory processing. Participants are better able to discern between visual events (in this case, studied list words) and internally derived events (critical lures) than between auditory events (hearing list words) and internally derived events (critical lures). This relationship supports Johnson and Raye’s (1981) claim that visual presentation leads Cognitive and Personality 21 to better reality monitoring. It also relates to Marmurek and Hamilton’s (2000) study results showing that data-limited encoding leads to less false recall. Rationale for the Current Study The purpose of this study is to investigate how individual differences in cognition and personality cause some people to be more susceptible to creating false memories than others. In addition, another goal of the present research is to examine how these differences are related to qualitative dissimilarities of DRM paradigm word lists, using norms for individual lists found by previous researchers (Stadler, Roediger, & McDermott, 1999; Roediger, Watson, McDermott, and Gallo, 2001). False recall was assessed using three DRM paradigm word lists (list items associated with critical lures king, sweet, and window). Words were presented visually, but unlike the computer method (Robinson and Roediger, 1997), participants were able to study all 15 items at the same time. A distractor task was included between list presentation and word recall. Individual differences in absorption and mental imagery were tested using the TAS, and field dependence was measured using the water-level test. Lastly, no prior studies have examined the relationship between levels of creativity and false memory creation in this type of paradigm. Therefore, another aim of the current study is to uncover any possible relationship between these two variables. Creativity in this study was measured using the Creative Personality Scale (CPS; Goncy & Waehler, 2006). It is hypothesized that the overall false recall rates for each list will follow the same trend as found by Stadler, Roediger, and McDermott (1999) found, with the window list eliciting the highest level of false recall, followed by the sweet list, and then finally the king list. Nevertheless, because of this study’s administration in the visual modality, it is expected that these false recall rates be lower than those found by Stadler, Roediger, and McDermott (1999). Cognitive and Personality 22 Despite the previous research demonstrating how higher levels of imagery increase false memory, this research has been limited to recall of actual events (Hyman & Pentland, 1996; Platt, Lacey, Iobst, & Finkelman, 1998). Since the current study involves word lists, and therefore data-driven processing (Jacoby, Levi, & Steinbach, 1992), it is hypothesized that false recall will be associated with lower scores on the TAS, replicating previous results (Marmurek & Hamilton, 2000). Based on the theory of Witkin, Goodenough, and Oltman (1979) which states that FD individuals are more likely to confuse the origins of their memories, and on the similar results of Durso, Reardon, and Jolly (1985), it is hypothesized that FD participants will exhibit more false recall than FI participants. Method Participants The final sample for this study consisted of 78 Cornell University undergraduate and graduate students, including 24 males and 54 females. Participants ranged in age from 19-28 (M = 20.4 years, SD = 1.5 years). An additional 44 subjects took part in the study, but were not included in the final sample because of their previous experience with similar word lists and procedures. The procedure took place during a regular meeting of the class, Nutritional Science 341, Human Anatomy and Physiology on May 2, 2007 in Martha Van Rensselaer Hall, room 166. Students were informed of the voluntary study beforehand, and received extra credit for their participation. The University Committee on Human Subjects approved the methods for this study. Materials Word Lists Cognitive and Personality 23 Three word lists from the appendix of Roediger and McDermott’s (1995) article, used in their second experiment, were included in this study. Each list consisted of 15 words most commonly related to one critical but non-presented word (king, sweet, and window). These lists were based on materials outlined in Deese’s (1959) article and were originally created from Russell and Jenkin’s (1954) word association norms. Lists were presented visually with all 15 items displayed on one page. Words were double-spaced and printed in the center of the page. Blank pages were used for the list recall portion (See Appendix). In between list presentation and recall, an image from the Thematic Apperception Test (TAT; Morgan & Murray, 1935; Murray, 1943) was shown. Each list corresponded with the same TAT picture, but the lists were presented in different orders. Three separate versions of these materials were used for counterbalancing purposes. In order to decide which DRM paradigm word lists to use in this study, it was important to look at the norms of list-based factors outlined by Roediger, Watson, McDermott, and Gallo (2001) (See Table 2). The three lists varied greatly in false recall rates, with the window list having the highest rate of false recall. In addition, the veridical recall rate was similar across all three lists, allowing for the focus to of the variability between lists to be on false recall. Individual Differences Measures The test for field dependence-independence was modified from a version of the waterlevel task created by Piaget & Inhelder (1967). In addition, the questionnaire included the CPS (Goncy & Waehler, 2006), and the TAS (Tellegen & Atkinson, 1974). A comprehensive demographics survey was included at the conclusion of the other measures (See Appendix). The field dependence-independence assessment used in this study was designed to study participants’ capability of organizing space within a Euclidean reference system (Piaget & Cognitive and Personality 24 Inhelder, 1967). In this study, participants were shown a drawing of 7 bottles attached to a wall above a horizontal line, indicating a “counter.” The bottles were depicted as tilted at various angles with respect to the line representing the counter. A dot on each bottle represented the height of the water level. Participants had to draw a line showing the level of water in the bottles at the height of the dot. The CPS was designed by Goncy and Waehler (2006) in their study assessing the relationship between creative personality traits and musical experience. It consists of 30 statements measuring creativity (e.g. “I find many solutions to one problem,” “I enjoy novel challenges in my life”). For the current study, two items from the CPS were omitted and phrasing was slightly changed to others based on discussions of the clarity of the scale with other researchers in the laboratory. The TAS is a measure originally consisting of 34 items used to assess the extent to which individuals become immersed in everyday activities (Tellegen & Atkinson, 1974). Following feedback from other researchers, 4 items were eliminated because of their ambiguous construction. Procedure Participants were tested as a group during a regular class meeting. Materials were sealed and participants were instructed not to open them until a signal had been given. One of three different versions of the materials was assigned to each participant so that no participants seated next to each other received the same version. Word Lists Participants were told that they would be tested on their memory for word lists. When the signal was given, they opened the materials packet to the first list. Each word list was presented Cognitive and Personality 25 for 30 seconds, after which participants were instructed to turn to the next page which displayed an image from the TAT. They were then given instructions to write a story explaining what was happening in the image. They were given two minutes to complete this task. Participants then turned to the next page and were allowed one minute for free recall of the word list. They were told to record all the words they could remember from the list, but to only write down those that they felt confident were present in the lists (i.e. they were told not to guess). These instructions are in accordance with those of Roediger and McDermott (1995). A question asking if the participant had ever had prior experience with word lists similar to the ones used in the study was included on the final list’s recall page. Students not taking part in the study helped to monitor that all participants were obeying the timing instructions and not returning to previous pages in the materials packet. Individual Differences Measures After completing the first, timed portion of the study, participants were told that they could complete the rest of the materials at their own pace. From this point on all three versions of materials were identical. The first measure was the water-levels task assessing field dependenceindependence. Participants were asked to draw a line to illustrate the water level in each bottle. This test was followed by the questionnaire section which included the TAS (Tellegen & Atkinson, 1974), the CPS (Goncy & Waehler, 2006), and the demographics survey. Items on the CPS and TAS were intermixed and administered as a single scale (See Appendix). Participants’ responses for these measures were given on a four-point Likert scale (1 = Not at all like me to 4 = Exactly like me). When participants had completed the questionnaire, they were asked to turn in all materials and exit the room quietly. All participants finished the study within the class time period of 50 minutes. Cognitive and Personality 26 Data Analysis Data were analyzed using the statistical analysis software JMP® 5.0.1 created by the SAS institute located in Cary, NC. Results Overall rates of false recall False recall rates found for king, sweet, and window are included in Table 1. The total rate of false recall, or the probability of recalling at least one critical item, was 66%. Since the total number of participants recalling king was only 4, the corresponding list was omitted from further analyses, with the exception of veridical recall rates. It is interesting to note, however, that every participant who recalled king also recalled both sweet and window. There was no sex difference found in false recall rates. Additionally, there were no effects of the order of presentation of the lists on false recall. Overall rates of veridical recall The rate of veridical recall was similar across the king and sweet lists, but was found to be lower for the window list (See Table 1). Field dependence The overall rate of field dependence was 28%. Twenty-two percent of females were found to be field dependent, while 40% of males were found to be field dependent. Despite this discrepancy, there was no significant sex difference for this cognitive style. Relationship of field dependence to recall Falsely recalling the word window was significantly associated with field dependence, χ2(1, N = 78) = 4.65, p = 0.03. Furthermore, regardless of any other false memory commission, wrongly remembering the word window was related to being field dependent. Field dependence Cognitive and Personality 27 was also related to the veridical recall for each list. Differences in the number of correct items recalled for the king list (M = 1.3, SD = 0.7) revealed that those who were field dependent had much lower veridical recall, t(34) = 2.00, p = 0.05. This trend was replicated in the differences between field dependent and field independent participants for both the sweet list (M = 1.3, SD = 0.5), t(49) = 2.87, p < 0.01 and the window list (M = 2.2, SD = 0.5), t(40) = 4.60, p < 0.01. Relationship of Tellegen absorption score and factor scores to recall There were no significant associations found between overall TAS score and the tendency to falsely recall critical items. Entrancement Examination of the relationship of individual items of the Tellegen Absorption Scale to the false recall of sweet and window revealed that a number of items served to distinguish those who “recalled” sweet but no item differentiated people who did or did not “recall” window. The scores for these items were combined to form a scale that we have called Entrancement, based on the content of the items involved. These included: When I look at clouds I usually see figures, such as animals or people. If I wish I can imagine (or daydream) some things so vividly that they hold my attention as a good movie or story does. When I listen to music I can get so caught up in it that I don't notice anything else. It is sometimes possible for me to be completely immersed in nature or in art and to feel as if my whole state of consciousness has somehow been temporarily altered. I can sometimes recall past experiences in my life so vividly that it is almost like living them again. While acting in a play I think I could really feel the emotions of the character and 'become' her/him for the time-being, forgetting both myself and the audience. I often take delight in small things (like the five-pointed star shape that appears when you cut an apple across the core or the colors in soap bubbles). Cognitive and Personality 28 I can be deeply moved by a sunset. I like to watch cloud shapes change in the sky. Sometimes I can change noise into music by the way I listen to it. The value for Cronbach’s alpha for these items together was 0.825, and removal of any of the items diminished that value, as did addition of any other items from the TAS. We therefore considered these items to constitute a scale we have called entrancement. As can be noted from the item content, these items involve absorption into sensory input together with the ability to call up that same experience through memory and imagination. Relationship of creativity score to entrancement Self-assessed creativity and entrancement scores were highly correlated, r2(76) = .27, p < .0001. No sex difference was found for creativity scores. Relationship of creativity score to recall Participants who did not have any false recall had significantly higher scores for selfassessed creativity (16.0±0.5) than did those who had one or more false recalls (13.3±0.5) (p=0.023). Relationship of field dependence to self-assessed creativity Participants who were field dependent had significantly lower creativity scores than did people who were field independent (13±0.5 versus 15±0.3, t(78)=2.919, p=0.0046. Relationship of personality scores to recall of sweet and window Since the purpose of this study was to discover differences in individual susceptibility to creating false memories as they relate to different list-based factors, it was important to analyze the results after separating the sample into the following three groups (See Table 3): Cognitive and Personality 29 Those who falsely recalled both sweet and window versus those who did not commit any errors in memory Those who only falsely recalled sweet versus those with no false recall Those who only falsely recalled window versus those with no false recall Using this method, it was possible to uncover any individual differences in cognition and personality that may relate to list-specific origins of false memory. Recalling sweet & window vs. neither These analyses draw attention to the characteristics of those participants recording both critical, non-presented items (n = 19) as opposed to those recalling no false items (n = 26). Field dependence was significantly associated with falsely recalling both sweet and window compared with no false recall, χ2 (1, N = 59) = 4.11, p = 0.04. Those committing both errors in memory were therefore more likely to be field dependent. Creativity scores for these groups also yielded significant differences (M = 1.6, SD = 0.8). Those participants mentioning both critical items scored lower on the CPS than those showing no false recall, t(43) = 2.04, p = 0.05, displaying less creative characteristics. Recalling sweet vs. neither Analyses for these groups distinguish those committing errors only on the sweet list (n = 14) from those recalling no critical items (n = 26). Entrancement scores were significantly lower for the group falsely recalling the critical word sweet (mean ± SEM: 25±2 versus 31±2, t(38) = 3.143, p = 0.0032). Therefore, those only recalling sweet (and not window) scored significantly lower on the entrancement scale of the TAS compared with those who did not error in memory. Additionally, the difference in creativity scores (14±1 versus 16±0.5) revealed a significant association between recalling sweet and lower creativity, t(38) = 2.05, p = 0.0473). Cognitive and Personality 30 Recalling window vs. neither For those who only falsely recalled window but not sweet (n = 19), field dependence remained significant, χ2 (1, N = 78) = 4.07, p = 0.04. This false memory group also exhibited a significant difference in creativity scores (14±0.5 versus 16±0.5) based on the CPS, with the recording of the critical word associated with lower self-assessed levels of creativity, t(45) = 2.936, p = 0.0052, but no difference in entrancement. Discussion The purpose of this study was to investigate how individual differences in cognition and personality are related to list-based factors of the DRM paradigm in causing susceptibility to the creation of false memories. In order to achieve this goal, three word lists originally used by Roediger and McDermott (1995) were presented to participants to elicit false memory. Piaget and Inhelder’s (1956) water-level task tested field dependence and the TAS and CPS assessed absorption and creativity, respectively. Using the list-based factors taken from the norms of previous researchers (Stadler, Roediger, & McDermott, 1999; Roediger, Watson, McDermott, and Gallo, 2001), significant associations were found between individual differences in cognition and personality. False Memory Results supported the hypothesis that false recall rates would reflect the same trend as previous research. The word king was recalled falsely the least frequently, while the word window was falsely recalled the most frequently, with the rate of sweet inclusion falling in between (See Table 1). The hypothesis that false recall rates would be lower in the current study than previous studies (Stadler, Roediger, & McDermott, 1999) presenting DRM list words in the auditory Cognitive and Personality 31 modality was also supported by the results. These findings support the claim made by Smith and Hunt (1998) that visual presentation of items help participants distinguish between externally generated events (list items) and internally derived events (critical, non-presented items). Furthermore, the results support the source monitoring theory of Johnson, Hashtroudi, and Lindsay (1993) that states that visual presentation aids participants in distinguishing the source of their memories. False recall rates in the current study were greater than those found in previous studies using visual modalities (Robinson & Roediger, 1997). The major difference in the methodology between these two visual presentations is the number of items presented at one time. Robinson & Roediger (1997) displayed each word, one at a time, on a computer screen, whereas in the current study all 15 list items were presented at one time. Theories about the functions of the articulatory loop (AL) have important implications regarding this difference (Baddeley & Hitch, 1974). If participants are forced to study 15 items at once, there will be competition in the AL for the rehearsal and storage of each item. It is possible that by seeing all the items at once, participants felt that they needed to group words in order to remember them more effectively. Furthermore, by viewing them at one time, the common association may become more apparent, and subjects may group the items based around the association (Miller, 1956). In the case of the current study, this recoding process led to falsely recalling the target word at higher rates than those found in other studies using visual modalities. Imagery There were no overall associations found between false recall and Tellegen Absorption Scale scores (TAS). Former studies relating detailed imagery levels to false memory creation have used actual events and suggested information to test memory (Hyman & Pentland, 1996; Cognitive and Personality 32 Platt, Lacey, Iobst, & Finkelman, 1998) rather than word lists. These events are conceptually driven as opposed to the word lists in the current study, which are remembered (presumably) using data-driven processes associated with list learning. It is therefore unsurprising that no positive association was found between overall TAS scores and false recall. This conclusion supports the theory Jacoby, Levi, and Steinbach (1992) that states that when encoding processes are data-driven more detailed imagers are more successful. Marmurek and Hamilton (2000) found that when encoding processes are limited to datadriven processing, more detailed imagers exhibit less false recall. These findings led to our hypothesis that lower levels of imagery would be related to higher false recall rates. This claim was supported by results from the sweet list, if we consider that the variable entrancement involves sensory imagery, especially if we include auditory experiences as “images.” Dissociation is a highly discussed correlate of memory suggestibility (Wright & Livingston-Raper, 2002), but this research has not extended much into methodology using the DRM. A previous study by Winograd, Peluso, and Glover (1998) used DRM paradigm word lists and the Dissociative Experiences Scale (DES; Bernstein & Putnam, 1986), but found that results from these two measures were uncorrelated. Similarly, Wright, Startup, and Mathews (2005) found no significant correlation between DRM paradigm false memories and dissociation. To the extent that the entrancement scale reflects a degree of dissociative experience (as reflected in the items that indicate complete involvement in a sensory experience to the exclusion of all other experience), an association was found in the current study. It is useful to examine what differences exist between methodologies that would explain these contrasting findings. The DRM procedure requires the participant to err in two distinct processes. First, the subject must generate a memory for a critical item that is not even presented. Cognitive and Personality 33 Second, they must misattribute the source of this memory (source monitoring error; Johnson, Hashtroudi, & Lindsay, 1993). Procedures involving suggested events only require the misattribution of the source in order to elicit false memory. In order to explain the relationship, Wright, Startup, and Mathews (2005) claim that dissociation is positively related to misattribution, but not to the generation of the items, and therefore only affects tasks that require solely misattribution. In fact, the researchers state that it may be the case that the process of generating a memory is negatively correlated with dissociation. If this relationship is strong enough, the overall correlation between dissociation and false memory may be negative. The finding that lower entrancement scores are significantly associated with false recall of the word sweet supports this argument. But why is it that only the sweet list displays this relationship and not the window list? Sweet has the lowest measure of tangibility, assessed using a concreteness scale by Roediger, Watson, McDermott, and Gallo (2001), whereas window has one of the highest (See Table 2). In fact, window has one of the highest concreteness ratings of all of the word lists analyzed by these investigators. The combination of lower entrancement scores and a less tangible critical word elicits a positive correlation, while with the presentation of a more tangible lure, those participants with lower entrancement scores are more likely to realize that the actual critical item was not presented in the list. Field Dependence Prior research revealed that FD individuals are more likely to confuse the origin of their memories (Witkin, Goodenough, & Oltman, 1979; Durso, Reardon, & Jolly, 1985). The current findings partially support the hypothesis that FD participants would show greater false recall in the list learning paradigm. Field dependence was most positively associated with falsely recalling the word window, but less than significant correlations were found for recalling sweet. Cognitive and Personality 34 Therefore, it is important to consider what differentiates the window list from the other lists. An important distinction is in concreteness, or level of tangibility of the critical words. Window has the highest concreteness level of the three lists (Roediger, Watson, McDermott, & Gallo, 2001). While studying these words, it is more likely that participants are imagining a window based on the tangibility of the item, as noted above. This imagining is related to Kozhevnikov, Kosslyn, and Shephard’s (2005) definitions of spatial versus object imagery. Spatial imagers tend to perceive stimuli analytically, piecewise, whereas object imagers perceive stimuli holistically as a single object. Relating this to the field dependence-independence trait, Wilson, Goodenough, and Oltman’s (1979) differentiation theory states that FD individuals are more global in their approach, while FI individuals are more analytic. It is therefore possible that when looking at a list of highly concrete, or tangible, items, those that are FD and therefore more object imagers are more likely to imagine and remember the target word if it completes the image. For example, in the case of the window list, when presented with a list of its parts (i.e. ledge, sill, pane, glass, frame, sash, screen, etc.), those tending to imagine stimuli holistically will commonly recall window. On the other hand, those individuals analyzing objects part by part, FI individuals, will more often not recall window. Because the sweet list has a lower level of concreteness, the same relationship was not found. Creativity Prior to the current study, no research had been conducted to examine the relationship between self-assessed creativity and false recall in the DRM paradigm. In addition, most prior studies relating creativity and memory have looked to investigate the influence of working memory and cerebellum function on creativity (e.g. Mumford & Caughron, 2007). The current study, however, sought to examine the opposite – how creativity levels affect memory. A priori, Cognitive and Personality 35 one might guess that recalling a non-existent word is a creative act. However, creativity level, as measured by the CPS, was the only factor significantly associated with false recall in all three groups: those recalling sweet and window, those only recalling sweet, and those only recalling window. In all three instances, creativity scores were lower for those creating false memories. The creators of the CPS stated that a major construct of creativity that the questionnaire targeted was problem solving (Goncy & Waehler, 2006). It is possible that those individuals who assess themselves as better problem solvers use more analytic processes than their counterparts. This presumption is supported by the observation that people who were field dependent had significantly lower creativity scores than did people who were field independent. Furthermore, analysis and observation are both key elements of effective problem solving, and would also lead individuals to realize the absence of a critical lure in a word list. Limitations There were several limitations in the current study that are important to consider when analyzing the findings. These drawbacks are related to the methodology as well as to the sample. It is possible that the TAT image was acting as more than just a distractor. With the negative connotations associated with these pictures, it is possible that they instilled negative emotions or stress in the participants prior to the recall task. This negative affect may influence the way in which participants encode information. Encoding can be accomplished in either of two ways: item-specific processing or relational processing (Hunt & Einstein, 1981; Hunt & McDaniel, 1993). Item-specific processing involves encoding items by their specific features and distinctive qualities. Relational processing involves encoding items as they relate to other concepts. While both types of processing may have their advantages, item-specific processing in the current study would be expected to lead to lower levels of false recall. The affect-as- Cognitive and Personality 36 information hypothesis (Clore et al., 2001) predicts that affective cues experienced during tasks help determine whether or not an individual utilizes item-specific or relational processing. Positive affective cues are thought to encourage relational processing, while negative cues result mostly in item-specific processing. Storbeck and Clore (2005) used music to induce positive or negative moods, and found that individuals in negative moods were significantly less likely to recall critical lures than individuals in positive moods (and in the control group). These findings relate to Brainerd and Reyna’s (1995) Fuzzy Trace Theory, which suggests that true recall comes from verbatim retrieval while false recall stems from gist retrieval. Storbeck and Clore (2005) suggest that negative moods reduce relational processing and in turn impair gist processing. Therefore, if the TAT images were inducing negative affective cues, it is possible that false recall rates found in the current study were relatively lower than if a neutral or positive image was used. Nevertheless, false recall rates for the current study were lower than those in studies conducted in auditory modalities and higher than those in studies conducted with different visual presentations, supporting the original hypotheses. Another limitation to the current study is the homogeneity of the sample. The current sample included Cornell University students enrolled in a course about human anatomy and physiology. Therefore, the majority of the students in this class were pursuing a scientific major and 60% designated themselves as pre-med. Subjects with scientific training have been found to score similarly as a group in several cognitive styles, including field dependence and imagery. For example, scientists and those with scientific training have been found to be more field independent than subjects in nonscientific occupations or training programs, regardless of sex (Kalichman, 1986). Additionally, Kozhevnikov, Kosslyn, and Shephard (2005) found that scientists are more likely to be classified as spatial imagers as opposed to object imagers, tending Cognitive and Personality 37 to perceive and encode images analytically. Nevertheless, even if FI and spatial imagery rates are higher in this sample, within-group analyses should still uncover important associations between these measures and list-based factors contributing to false memory creations. Also, although males made up only 31% of the sample population, no significant sex differences were found for any measure. The findings of the current study are also limited because only three DRM paradigm word lists were used. In order to be able to further validate the results, more lists should have been included. This would have provided more data related to false recall in addition to further evidence of potential list-based and cognitive factors that contribute to the susceptibility to creating false memories. Unfortunately, the study was limited by the amount of time allotted for the class, and it was necessary to design the materials and procedures accordingly. Suggestions for Future Research Findings from the current study suggest many directions for future research in the field of false memory. The Object-Spatial Imagers Questionnaire (OSIQ; Blajenkova, Kozhevnikov, & Motes, 2006) was designed to classify individuals based on their visual preferences. In their study, the authors found that object imagers encoded and processed images holistically while spatial imagers were more analytic. The current study makes the argument that field dependent individuals may therefore be viewed as object imagers, and that this type of perception leads to greater false recall for lists with more tangible items. Therefore, further research would benefit from a study including measures for false memory, field dependence, and the OSIQ. In addition, in order to both replicate and expand upon the results of the current study, future research should include more DRM paradigm word lists. By adding more lists, investigators will be able to further examine the relationships between list-based factors and their Cognitive and Personality 38 associations with cognitive and personality characteristics. More specifically, lists with target words of varying degrees of concreteness will provide additional information about its role in helping to predict false memory creation. The relationship between creativity and false recall should be explored further. This was the first study to look at the association between self-assessed creativity and performance on the DRM paradigm. Lower creativity scores were significantly related to all three categories of false recall. Future research should consider measuring creativity using different scales and determining if its inverse relationship with the creation of false memories remains significant. Cognitive and Personality 39 References Abravanel, E., & Gingold, H. (1977). Perceiving and representing orientation: Effects of the spatial framework. Merrill Palmer Quarterly, 23(4), 265-278. Baddeley, A. D., & Hitch, G. J. (1974). Working memory. In G. Bower (Ed.), Recent advances in learning and motivation (Vol. 8 ed., pp. 47-90). New York: Academic Press. Baddeley, A. D. (1990). Human memory: Theory and practice. Needham Heights, MA, US: Allyn and Bacon. Balota, D. A., Cortese, M. J., Duchek, J. M., Adams, D., Roediger, H. L. I. I. I., McDermott, K. B., et al. (1999). Veridical and false memories in healthy older adults and in dementia of the Alzheimer’s type. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 16(3-5), 361-384. Bartlett, F. C. (1932). Remembering: A study in experimental and social psychology. Oxford, England: Macmillan. Bernstein, E. M., & Putnam, F. W. (1986). Development, reliability, and validity of a dissociation scale. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 174(12), 727-735. Blajenkova, O., Kozhevnikov, M., & Motes, M. A. (2006). Object-spatial imagery: A new selfreport imagery questionnaire. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 20(2), 239-263. Brainerd, C. J., & Reyna, V. F. (2002). Fuzzy-trace theory and false memory. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11(5), 164-169. Clore, G. L., & Martin, L. L. (Eds.). (2001). Theories of mood and cognition: A user's guidebook (viii ed.). Mahwah, NJ, US: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers. Deese, J. (1959). On the prediction of occurrence of particular verbal intrusions in immediate recall. Journal of experimental psychology, 58(1), 17-22. Cognitive and Personality 40 DeLisi, R. (1983). Developmental and individual differences in children’s representation of the horizontal coordinate. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 29, 179-196. Dobson, M., & Markham, R. (1993). Imagery ability and source monitoring: Implications for eyewitness memory. British Journal of Psychology, 84(1), 111-118. Durso, F. T., Reardon, R., & Jolly, E. J. (1985). Self-nonself-segregation and reality monitoring. Journal of personality and social psychology, 48(2), 447-455. Ellis, N. C., & Hennelly, R. A. (1980). A bilingual word-length effect: Implications for intelligence testing and the relative ease of mental calculation in Welsh and English. British Journal of Psychology, 71(1), 43-51. Farah, M. J., Hammond, K. M., Levine, D. N., & Calvanio, R. (1988). Visual and spatial mental imagery: Dissociable systems of representation. Cognitive psychology, 20(4), 439-462. Gallo, D. A., McDermott, K. B., Percer, J. M., & Roediger, H. L. I. I. I. (2001). Modality effects in false recall and false recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 27(2), 339-353. Gathercole, S. E., & Baddeley, A. D. (1989). Evaluation of the role of phonological STM in the development of vocabulary in children: A longitudinal study: Journal of Memory and Language, 28, 200-213. Goncy, E. A., & Waehler, C. A. (2006). An empirical investigation of creativity and musical experience. Psychology of Music, 34(3), 307-321. Horselenberg, R., Merckelbach, H., van Breukelen, G., & Wessel, I. (2004). Individual differences in the accuracy of autobiographical memory. Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, 11(3), 168-176. Cognitive and Personality 41 Hunt, R. R., & Einstein, G. O. (1981). Relational and item-specific information in memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 20(5), 497-514. Hunt, R. R., & McDaniel, M. A. (1993). The enigma of organization and distinctiveness. Journal of Memory and Language, 32(4), 421-445. Hyman, I. E. J., & Pentland, J. (1996). The role of mental imagery in the creation of false childhood memories. Journal of Memory and Language, 35(2), 101-117. Jacoby, L. L., Kelley, C. M., & Dywan, J. (1989). Memory attributions. In H. L. I. I. I. Roediger, & F. I. M. Craik (Eds.), Varieties of memory and consciousness: Essays in honour of Endel Tulving (pp. 391-422). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Jacoby, L. L., Levy, B. A., & Steinbach, K. (1992). Episodic transfer and automaticity: Integration of data-driven and conceptually-driven processing in rereading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 18(1), 15-24. Johnson, M. K., Hashtroudi, S., & Lindsay, D. S. (1993). Source monitoring. Psychological Bulletin, 114(1), 3-28. Johnson, M. K., & Raye, C. L. (1981). Reality monitoring. Psychological review, 88(1), 67-85. Kalichman, S. C. (1986). Horizontality as a function of sex and academic major. Perceptual and motor skills, 63(2, Pt 2), 903-906. Kalichman, S. C. (1988). Individual differences in water-level task performance: A componentskills analysis. Developmental Review, 8(3), 273-295. Kellogg, R. T. (2001). Presentation modality and mode of recall in verbal false memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 27(4), 913-919. Kelly, J. T., & Kelly, G. N. (1977). Perception of horizontality by male and female college students. Perceptual and motor skills, 44(3, Pt 1), 724-726. Cognitive and Personality 42 Kenyon, J. (1984). Paper-and-pencil tests of Piaget's water-level test: Sex differences and test modality. Perceptual and motor skills, 59(3), 739-742. Kozhevnikov, M., Kosslyn, S., & Shephard, J. (2005). Spatial versus object visualizers: A new characterization of visual cognitive style. Memory and Cognition, 33(4), 710-726. Kučera, H., & Francis, W. (1967). Computationalanalysis of present-day American English. Providence, RI: Brown University Press. Logie, R. H. (1986). Visuo-spatial processing in working memory. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology A: Human Experimental Psychology, 38A(2), 229-247. Marks, D. F. (1973). The vividness of visual imagery questionnaire. British Journal of Psychology, 64, 17-24. Marmurek, H. H. C., & Hamilton, M. E. (2000). Imagery effects in false recall and false recognition. Journal of Mental Imagery, 24(1-2), 83-96. McEvoy, C. L., Nelson, D. L., & Komatsu, T. (1999). What is the connection between true and false memories? The differential roles of interitem associations in recall and recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 25(5), 11771194. Miller, G. A. (1956). The magical number seven, plus or minus two: Some limits on our capacity for processing information. Psychological review, 63(2), 81-97. Morgan, C. D., & Murray, H. H. (1935). A method for investigating fantasies: The thematic apperception test. Archives of Neurology and Psychiatry (Chicago), (34), 289-306. Morris, B. B. (1971). Effects of angle, sex, and cue on adults' perception of the horizontal. Perceptual and motor skills, 32(3), 827-830. Cognitive and Personality 43 Mumford, M. D., & Caughron, J. J. (2007). Neurology and creative thought: Some thoughts about working memory, the cerebellum, and creativity. Creativity Research Journal, 19(1), 49-54. Murray, H. A. (1943). Thematic apperception test manual. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Nelson, D. L., McEvoy, C. L., & Schreiber, T. A. (1999). The University of South Florida word association, rhyme, and word fragment norms. Unpublished manuscript. Piaget, J., & Inhelder, B. (1967). The child's conception of space (F. J. Langdon, J. L. Lunzer Trans.). New York: W.W. Norton & Company, Inc. Platt, R. D., Lacey, S. C., Iobst, A. D., & Finkelman, D. (1998). Absorption, dissociation, fantasy-proneness as predictors of memory distortion in autobiographical and laboratory generated memories. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 12(Spec Issue), S77-S89. Quinn, J. G., & McConnell, J. (1996). Irrelevant pictures in visual working memory. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology A: Human Experimental Psychology, 49A(1), 200-215. Rebelsky, F. (1964). Adult perception of the horizontal. Perceptual and motor skills, 19(2), 371374. Reyna, V. F., & Brainerd, C. J. (1995). Fuzzy-trace theory: An interim synthesis. Learning and Individual Differences, 7(1), 1-75. Robinson, K. J., & Roediger, H. L. I. I. I. (1997). Associative processes in false recall and false recognition. Psychological Science, 8(3), 231-237. Cognitive and Personality 44 Roediger, H. L., & McDermott, K. B. (1995). Creating false memories: Remembering words not presented in lists. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21(4), 803-814. Roediger, H. L. I. I. I. (1996). Memory illusions. Journal of Memory and Language, 35(2), 76100. Roediger, H. L. I. I. I., Watson, J. M., McDermott, K. B., & Gallo, D. A. (2001). Factors that determine false recall: A multiple regression analysis. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 8(3), 385-407. Russell, W. A., & Jenkins, J. J. (1954). The complete Minnesota norms for responses to 100 words from the Kent-Rosanoff Word Association Test (Tech. Rep. No. 11, Contract N8 ONR 66216, Office of Naval Research). University of Minnesota, Minneapolis. Smith, R. E., & Hunt, R. R. (1998). Presentation modality affects false memory. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 5(4), 710-715. Stadler, M. A., Roediger, H. L. I. I. I., & McDermott, K. B. (1999). Norms for word lists that create false memories. Memory and Cognition, 27(3), 494-500. Storbeck, J., & Clore, G. L. (2005). With sadness comes accuracy; with happiness, false memory: Mood and the false memory effect. Psychological Science, 16(10), 785- 791. Tellegen, A., & Atkinson, G. (1974). Openness to absorbing and self-altering experiences ("absorption"), a trait related to hypnotic susceptibility. Journal of abnormal psychology, 83(3), 268-277. Thomas, H., Jamison, W., & Hummel, D. D. (1973). Observation is insufficient for discovering that the surface of still water is invariantly horizontal. Science, 181(4095), 173-174. Cognitive and Personality 45 Watson, J. M., Bunting, M. F., Poole, B. J., & Conway, A. R. A. (2005). Individual differences in susceptibility to false memory in the Deese-Roediger-McDermott paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 31(1), 76-85. Willemsen, E., Buchholz, A., Budrow, M. S., & Geannacopulos, N. (1973). Relationship between Witkin's rod-and-frame task and Piaget's water-line task for college women. Perceptual and motor skills, 36(3, Pt. 1), 958. Wilson, S. C., & Barber, T. X. (1978). The creative imagination scale as a measure of hypnotic responsiveness: Applications to experimental and clinical hypnosis. American Journal of Clinical Hypnosis, 20(4), 235-249. Winograd, E., Peluso, J. P., & Glover, T. A. (1998). Individual differences in susceptibility to memory illusions. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 12(Spec Issue), S5-S27. Witkin, H. A. (1950). Individual differences in ease of perception of embedded figures. Journal of personality, (19), 1-15. Witkin, H. A., & Goodenough, D. R. (1981). Cognitive styles: Essence and origins. New York: International Universities Press. Witkin, H. A., Goodenough, D. R., & Oltman, P. K. (1979). Psychological differentiation: Current status. Journal of personality and social psychology, 37(7), 1127-1145. Witkin, H. A., Oltman, P. K., Raskin, E., & Karp, S. A. (1971). A manual for the embedded figures test. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press. Wright, D. B., & Livingston Raper, D. (2002). Memory distortion and dissociation: Exploring the relationship in a non-clinical sample. Journal of Trauma and Dissociation, 3(3), 97109. Cognitive and Personality 46 Wright, D. B., Startup, H. M., & Mathews, S. A. (2005). Mood, dissociation and false memories using the Deese-Roediger-McDermott procedure. British Journal of Psychology, 96(3), 283-293. Cognitive and Personality 47 Appendix First, thank you for participating in this study! The study consists of three parts: 1. You will first be asked to complete a series of exercises about memory and imagination/storytelling. 2. Next, you will have your taste and smell sensitivity tested. 3. In the final part you will fill out a questionnaire concerning how you see the world, how you find your memory works, and in which you will give some demographic information. Again, thanks so much for your participation! Please wait for the signal to begin. Cognitive and Personality 48 Please memorize the words on the list below. You will be asked about them later. Do not go to the next page until you have been given the signal! Word list Queen England Crown Prince George Dictator Palace Throne Chess Rule Subjects Monarch Royal Leader Reign => Please wait for the signal to go on to the next page. <= Cognitive and Personality 49 In the space below, write a short story about what is happening in this picture. ____________________________________________________________ ____________________________________________________________ ____________________________________________________________ ____________________________________________________________ ____________________________________________________________ ____________________________________________________________ ____________________________________________________________ ____________________________________________________________ ____________________________________________________________ ____________________________________________________________ ____________________________________________________________ ____________________________________________________________ => Please wait for the signal to go on to the next page. <= Cognitive and Personality 50 In the space below, write down the words you memorized in the FIRST list: Cognitive and Personality 51 Please memorize the words on the list below. You will be asked about them later. Do not go to the next page until you have been given the signal! Word list Sour Candy Sugar Bitter Good Taste Tooth Nice Honey Soda Chocolate Heart Cake Tart Pie => Please wait for the signal to go on to the next page. <= Cognitive and Personality 52 In the space below, write a short story about what is happening in this picture. ____________________________________________________________ ____________________________________________________________ ____________________________________________________________ ____________________________________________________________ ____________________________________________________________ ____________________________________________________________ ____________________________________________________________ ____________________________________________________________ ____________________________________________________________ ____________________________________________________________ ____________________________________________________________ ____________________________________________________________ => Please wait for the signal to go on to the next page. <= Cognitive and Personality 53 In the space below, write down the words you memorized in the SECOND list: Cognitive and Personality 54 Please memorize the words on the list below. You will be asked about them later. Do not go to the next page until you have been given the signal! Door Glass Pane Shade Ledge Sill House Open Curtain Frame View Breeze Sash Screen Shutter => Please wait for the signal to go on to the next page. <= Cognitive and Personality 55 In the space below, tell a short story about what is happening in this picture. ____________________________________________________________ ____________________________________________________________ ____________________________________________________________ ____________________________________________________________ ____________________________________________________________ ____________________________________________________________ ____________________________________________________________ ____________________________________________________________ ____________________________________________________________ ____________________________________________________________ ____________________________________________________________ ____________________________________________________________ => Please wait for the signal to go on to the next page. <= Cognitive and Personality 56 In the space below, write down the words you memorized in the THIRD list: Have you had experience with word lists similar to the ones used in this study? Yes => Please wait for the signal to go on to the next page. <= No Cognitive and Personality 57 Part IV. Water levels: The image below diagrams a series of clear glass bottles. Imagine that they are attached to a wall, above a horizontal counter, indicated by the black bar. They are all partially filled with water. The level of water in each bottle is as high as the dot next to the bottle. Draw a line showing the level of water in each bottle, so that the line touches the dot. In which direction(s) did you draw the water levels (circle your answer)? Towards the dot (left to right) Away from the dot (right to left) Once you have completed this exercise, please go on to the next page. Both directions Cognitive and Personality 58 Questionnaire Part I. First, something about your attitudes and behaviors ~ Using the following scale, indicate to what extent each of the following is typical of you: 1. Not at all like me 2. Slightly like me 3. Moderately like me 4. Exactly like me _____ I can be greatly moved by eloquent or poetic language. _____ When reading a book, I can become so involved that I forget my surroundings and feel as if I were taking part in the story. _____ Sometimes I feel as if my mind could envelop the whole world. _____ When I look at clouds I usually see figures, such as animals or people. _____ If I wish I can imagine (or daydream) some things so vividly that they hold my attention as a good movie or story does. _____ Textures—such as wool, sand, or wood—sometimes remind me of colors or music. _____ When I listen to music I can get so caught up in it that I don't notice anything else. _____ I feel I do not have much freedom to choose the actions in my life. _____ If I wish I can imagine that my body is so heavy that I could not move it if I wanted to. _____ The crackle and flames of a wood fire stimulate my imagination. _____ It is sometimes possible for me to be completely immersed in nature or in art and to feel as if my whole state of consciousness has somehow been temporarily altered. _____ I am able to wander off into my own thoughts while doing a routine task and actually forget that I am doing the task, and then find a few minutes later that I have completed it. _____ I can sometimes recall past experiences in my life so vividly that it is almost like living them again. _____ While acting in a play I think I could really feel the emotions of the character and 'become' her/him for the time-being, forgetting both myself and the audience. _____ Some of my most vivid memories are called up by scents and smells. _____ I think that I am intelligent. Cognitive and Personality 59 Indicate to what extent each of the following is typical of you: 1. Not at all like me 2. Slightly like me 3. Moderately like me 4. Exactly like me _____ I have low confidence in myself. _____ I would not describe myself as an independent thinker. _____ I am a risk-taker. _____ I enjoy novel challenges in my life. _____ My thoughts often occur as visual images rather than words. _____ I do not consider myself a complex thinker. _____ I often take delight in small things (like the five-pointed star shape that appears when you cut an apple across the core or the colors in soap bubbles). _____ When listening to organ music or other powerful music, I sometimes feel as if I am being lifted into the air. _____ Some music reminds me of pictures or changing color patterns. _____ I find myself capable of handling a variety of situations. _____ My views are generally more extreme than those of my peers. _____ I often have "physical memories"; for example, after I've been swimming I may still feel as if I'm in the water. _____ People who know me would describe me as creative. _____ The sound of a voice can be so fascinating to me that I can just go on listening to it. _____ Most decisions I make in my life are objective and clear-cut. _____ I find that different odors have different colors. _____ I think I am a unique individual among my friends. _____ I can be deeply moved by a sunset. _____ I solve problems the same way other people do. Cognitive and Personality 60 Indicate to what extent each of the following is typical of you: 5. Not at all like me 6. Slightly like me 7. Moderately like me 8. Exactly like me _____ I am not involved in many activities. _____ My ideas are rarely different from those of my peers. _____ I am resourceful. _____ I can be critical of other people’s work. _____ When I have to make a decision, I tend to mull things over, weighing all the possibilities. _____ I try to find many solutions to one problem. _____ I am not a humorous person. _____ If I stare at a picture and then look away from it, I can sometimes "see" an image of the picture almost as if I were still looking at it. _____ I am a very cautious person. _____ I like to watch cloud shapes change in the sky. _____ I solve problems in creative ways. _____ I sometimes "step outside" my usual self and experience an entirely different state of being. _____ I put others before myself. _____ I am not particularly good at problem solving. _____ I can often somehow sense the presence of another person before I actually see or hear her/him. _____ Different colors have distinctive and special meanings for me. _____ I do well at connecting many different parts of my life. _____ Sometimes I can change noise into music by the way I listen to it. _____ I don’t have much spontaneity in my life. _____ I often know what someone is going to say before he or she says it. Cognitive and Personality 61 Indicate to what extent each of the following is typical of you: 9. Not at all like me 10. Slightly like me 11. Moderately like me 12. Exactly like me _____ At times I somehow feel the presence of someone who is not physically there. _____ I am generally creative in my work. _____ Sometimes images come to me without the slightest effort on my part. _____ I am a very conventional person. _____ I have a wide assortment of interests and activities. Cognitive and Personality 62 Part II. Next, a little bit about your day and your sleep: Overall, what percentage of time have you been feeling in the following moods today? In a bad mood _______% A little low or irritable _______% In a mildly pleasant mood _______% In a very good mood _______% SUM 100 % Last night, how many hours of actual sleep did you get? ______ What time did you go to sleep? ________ Was the amount of sleep you got last night typical? Yes If not, do you usually get: More sleep How tired do you feel right now? Very tired Somewhat tired No Less sleep Not tired, but not wide awake Do you consume caffeine (e.g. coffee, tea, soda, caffeine pills)? If yes: How long ago did you last have caffeine? Less than 1hour 1 hour 2 hours 3 hours 4 hours Yes Wide awake No 5 hours More than 5 hours ago When in the day do you usually consume caffeine (please circle all that apply)? Morning Midday Afternoon Evening What form of caffeine do you consume (please circle all that apply)? Coffee Tea Soda Pills Other (please specify) _____________ How satisfied are you with your health these days? Very satisfied Satisfied Not very satisfied Not at all satisfied How stressed do you feel about emotional issues in your life (i.e. due to relationship problems, family problems etc.)? Very stressed Stressed Not very stressed Not at all stressed How stressed do you feel about academic issues in your life (i.e. upcoming exams, papers etc.)? Very stressed Stressed Not very stressed Not at all stressed How stressed do you feel about physical issues in your life (i.e. health, amount of exercise)? Very stressed Stressed Not very stressed Not at all stressed Cognitive and Personality 63 Part III. Finally, some information about who you are… Month of birth: ____________ Year of birth: ___________ Sex: Male Female Major: __________________ College: __________________ Future occupation – please be specific:__________________________________________________ Are you pre-med? Yes No If you are pre-med, what specialty interests you the most? __________________________ Height (inches): ______feet______ inches Weight (pounds):_________ How physically active are you? Very Moderately Slightly Not at all If you participate in physical activity, for which of the following reasons do you participate? (circle all that apply): Health Recreation To lose weight Other: ______________ If you play a sport, please specify: Varsity Intramural Club How would you rate your taste perception? How would you rate your smell perception? Excellent Excellent Very good Very good Good Good Fair Fair Poor Poor What is your handedness? Right-Handed Mostly Right-Handed Ambidextrous Mostly Left-Handed Left-Handed Do you play a musical instrument? Yes No If yes, which instrument(s):____________________ How would you rate your proficiency at this instrument (or your best instrument if you play more than one? Expert Intermediate Beginner Do you do puzzles? Yes No If yes, what kinds of puzzles? ______________________________________________________ How often do you do them? __________________ Do you smoke? Yes No If yes, what do you smoke (e.g. cigarettes, cigars)? _____________________________ How often do you usually smoke? Daily 2-3 times/week Once a week < Once/week Did you smoke today? Yes No Did you drink alcohol (including alcohol in medications) yesterday? Did you drink alcohol (including alcohol in medications) today? Yes Yes No No Have you ever done binge drinking (defined as drinking over an extended period of time, usually two or more days, during which you repeatedly consume alcohol to the point of intoxication, and give up your usual activities and obligations)? Yes No Do have a cold or nasal congestion today? Yes No Are you currently taking any prescription medications? Yes No If yes, which ones? __________________________________________________ Cognitive and Personality 64 Are you currently taking over the counter medications (i.e. aspirin, Claritin, ibuprofen)? Yes If yes, which ones? __________________________________________________ Do you take nutritional supplements (for example vitamin pills, calcium, protein shakes)? Yes If yes, which ones? __________________________________________________ No No Do you find that these medications affect your sense of taste and/or smell? Yes No If yes, in what way do they affect your sense of taste or smell? ________________________________________________________________________ Do you often have the feeling of having a dry mouth? Yes No Do you have any kind of allergy? Yes No If yes, what kind(s) do you have? ___________________________________________________ Do you currently have nasal polyps? Yes No Have you had them in the past? Yes No Are you on a specific diet (i.e. vegetarian, Atkins, Kosher, South Beach, etc.)? Yes No If yes, please specify? __________________________________________________ Have you ever sustained a head injury (for example from a car accident or snowboarding)? Yes No If yes, please specify: __________________________________________________ Were you unconscious? Yes No Circle all the groups or areas of the world from which your ancestors came: Pacific Islands China Korea Japan Southeast Asia India/Pakistan/Afghanistan Central Asia Israel Ashkenazim Sephardim Middle East North Africa Sub-Saharan Africa Turkey Caucasus Greece Italy Spain Portugal France Belgium Ireland United Kingdom Netherlands Denmark Norway Sweden Iceland Finland Latvia/Lithuania/Estonia Russia Poland Germany Austria Switzerland Hungary Czech Republic Slovakia Bulgaria Romania Moldavia Ukraine Pre-Columbian America Was English the first language you learned? Yes No If not, at what age did you start learning English? ________ What language did you learn first? ______________ Can you read and write in this language? Yes No For women: What was the date of the start of your last menstrual period?________________________ Do you use oral contraceptives? Yes No How many periods have you had in the past 6 months?____________________________ Are your periods regular or irregular?__________________________________________ THANK YOU SO MUCH FOR COMPLETING THIS STUDY!!! Cognitive and Personality 65 Author Note Ari Silbermann, Undergraduate in the Department of Human Development, Cornell University. This study was conducted as fulfillment of an undergraduate honors program. I would like to thank Dr. Virginia Utermohlen for her guidance in overseeing this project. I would also like to thank Drs. Marienella Casasola and Stephen Ceci for their additional comments and support, and to all of the students participating in this study. Correspondence regarding this manuscript should be addressed to Ari Silbermann, Department of Human Development, Cornell University, Ithaca, NY 14853. Electronic mail can be sent to abs57@cornell.edu Cognitive and Personality 66 Table 1 Rates of False Recall and Veridical Recall for Three DRM Paradigm Word Lists ______________________________________________________________________________ Word List False Recall Veridical Recall ______________________________________________________________________________ King .05 .706 Sweet .42 .703 Window .49 .568 ______________________________________________________________________________ Cognitive and Personality 67 Table 2 Word List Norms from Roediger, Watson, McDermott, and Gallo (2001) ______________________________________________________________________________ Word List False Recall Concreteness BAS Connectivity Veridical Recall ______________________________________________________________________________ King .10 5.54 .230 2.07 .650 Sweet .54 4.53 .172 2.07 .630 Window .65 6.27 .184 0.67 .630 _____________________________________________________________________________ Cognitive and Personality 68 Table 3 Breakdown of Total Sample (N=78) by False Recall Responses of Sweet and Window ______________________________________________________________________________ Recalled Window Did Not Recall Window Total Recalled Sweet 19 14 33 Did Not Recall Sweet 19 26 45 Total 38 40 78 ______________________________________________________________________________