Anxiety-induced cognitive bias in non

advertisement
Physiology & Behavior (2009) Volume 98: 345-350
10.1016/j.physbeh.2009.06.012
Final Revision – NOT EDITED by the journal
Anxiety-induced cognitive bias in non-human animals
OLIVER H. P. BURMAN, RICHARD M. A. PARKER, ELIZABETH S. PAUL,
MICHAEL .T. MENDL
University of Bristol, Bristol, BS40 5DU, U.K.
Corresponding author: Oliver H. P. Burman
Word count: 5019
1
1
OLIVER H. P. BURMAN, RICHARD M. A. PARKER, ELIZABETH S. PAUL,
2
MICHAEL .T. MENDL. Anxiety-induced cognitive bias in non-human animals.
3
PHYSIOL BEHAV. - As in humans, ‘cognitive biases’ in the way in which
4
animals judge ambiguous stimuli may be influenced by emotional state and
5
hence a valuable new indicator of animal emotion. There is increasing evidence
6
that animals experiencing different emotional states following exposure to long-
7
term environmental manipulations show contrasting biases in their judgement of
8
ambiguous stimuli. However, the specific type of induced emotional state is
9
usually unknown. We investigated whether a short-term manipulation of
10
emotional state has a similar effect on cognitive bias, using changes in light
11
intensity; a treatment specifically related to anxiety induction. Twenty-four male
12
rats were trained to discriminate between two different locations, in either high
13
(‘H’) or low (‘L’) light levels. One location was rewarded with palatable food
14
and the other with aversive food. Once the rats had shown spatial discrimination,
15
by running significantly faster to the rewarded location, they were tested with
16
three ambiguous locations intermediate between the rewarded and aversive
17
locations, and their latency to approach each location recorded. Half the rats were
18
tested in the same light levels as during training, the remainder were switched.
19
Rats switched from high to low light levels (putatively the least negative
20
emotional manipulation) ran significantly faster to all three ambiguous probes
21
than those rats switched from low to high light levels (putatively the most
22
negative manipulation). This suggests that the judgement bias technique might be
23
useful as an indicator of short-term changes in anxiety for non-human animals.
24
2
25
Keywords: Emotion; Cognition; Anxiety; Cognitive Bias; Emotional state;
26
Behaviour; Animal Welfare
27
28
3
29
Introduction
30
Humans experiencing different background emotional states display contrasting
31
cognitive-affective biases (henceforth referred to as ‘cognitive bias’) in their
32
judgement of ambiguous stimuli. For example, people in a negative state tend to
33
make negative judgements about ambiguous situations or stimuli [e.g. 1,2,3].
34
Since Harding et al. [4] first demonstrated a novel technique to determine the
35
emotional state of non-human animals by measuring changes in judgement bias,
36
there has been considerable interest in the further development and validation of
37
this technique [e.g. 5,6-9]. Compared to many existing behavioural and
38
physiological measures of non-human emotional state, one potential advantage of
39
a cognitive bias approach is the ability to discriminate between similarly-
40
valenced emotional states such as depression and anxiety [3]. However, this
41
potential benefit has yet to be demonstrated.
42
43
Previous studies have manipulated emotional state by using unpredictable
44
housing events, each designed to be mildly stressful [rats: 4], or the
45
absence/removal of enrichment [starlings: 5,6, rats: 7,8]. However, these
46
affective (emotional) manipulations have been relatively long-term/chronic in
47
duration (e.g. enrichment absence: 21 days [8]; 7 days [5]; unpredictable
48
housing: 9 days [4]), and have induced negative emotional states that, whilst of
49
unknown specificity, are perhaps more likely to be related to states of depression
50
rather than anxiety (e.g. unpredictable housing: [10]; enrichment
51
absence/removal: depression and the experience of loss in humans, [11]). It is
52
therefore of considerable interest to investigate whether cognitive bias tasks in
53
non-human animals can be influenced by acute anxiety-related emotions as well
4
54
as the more chronic depression-like mood states studied previously, as has been
55
demonstrated in humans [e.g. 12].
56
57
The current study addresses this issue by employing a treatment designed to
58
induce an acute/short-term change in anxiety. This treatment involves varying
59
the level of ambient light, since higher light levels are considered increasingly
60
anxiety inducing for a nocturnal species such as the laboratory rat [e.g. 13,14,15];
61
presumably as an evolved response to increased threat of predation. For example,
62
the acoustic startle reflex in rats is potentiated by exposure to high light levels
63
[16] whereas in humans it is potentiated by exposure to darkness [17]. Light
64
levels can therefore be manipulated in order to generate different levels of
65
anxiety, as evidenced by the use of different light levels during anxiety testing
66
[e.g. 18] depending on whether anxiogenic or anxiolytic treatment effects are to
67
be studied. There is also evidence that a change/switch in light levels can itself
68
influence emotional state, for example, sudden darkness can result in clear
69
anxiolytic effects on rodent behaviour [e.g. 19,20,21]. For this reason, we
70
decided to investigate the effect of both constant and changing light levels on
71
cognitive bias in this study. Light intensity satisfies the requirement for an
72
appropriate affective (emotional) manipulation, namely that there is a previously
73
demonstrated effectiveness for inducing a change in emotional state (specifically
74
anxiety), therefore allowing the construction of clear predictions concerning the
75
potential impact of our treatment on cognitive bias.
76
77
The aim of this study was therefore to test the prediction that rats exposed to high
78
light levels will judge ambiguous stimuli negatively in comparison to those rats
5
79
exposed to low light levels, due to being in a putative negative emotional state
80
(anxiety). Furthermore, we predicted that rats tested in high light levels following
81
previous experience of the apparatus under low light conditions would be most
82
anxious and those tested under low light levels after experiencing high light
83
conditions would be least anxious, and hence these two groups of rats would
84
show the strongest contrast in their judgements of ambiguous stimuli.
85
86
In addition to choosing the appropriate treatment necessary to induce the desired
87
emotional state, it is also important to select an appropriate reward contingency
88
for the cognitive bias task that is being used as a proxy indicator of that
89
emotional state. It has been proposed that depression, being related to the
90
experience of loss [11], may be more commonly associated with a decreased
91
expectation of positive events, whereas anxiety, being threat-related [11], may be
92
more commonly associated with an increased expectation of negative events
93
[22]. In a previous study [8], we used both a treatment (loss of enrichment) most
94
likely to induce a depression-like state, and a reward contingency
95
(presence/absence of a reward) most likely to be sensitive to cognitive biases
96
associated with that state. In contrast, in this study we employ a treatment
97
designed to induce a change in anxiety (light levels), and a reward contingency
98
designed to be most sensitive to cognitive biases likely to be associated with that
99
state, namely the presence/absence of a negative event (the perceived ‘threat’ of
100
ingesting an aversive food).
101
102
103
Materials and Methods
6
104
105
Subjects
106
This work was carried out under UK Home Office licence (PPL 30/2249). We
107
used twenty four male Lister-hooded rats (Harlan, UK), approximately nine
108
months old at the start of testing. The rats had previously been used (at least three
109
months previously) in studies of incentive contrast and cognitive bias [7,8], and
110
so individuals were randomly allocated between the different treatments in the
111
current study in order to minimize any potential influence of previous
112
experience. The rats were housed in groups of three in standard cages (33cm X
113
50cm X 21cm) on a 12hr reversed light cycle, lights off 0800-2000, with food
114
(Harlan Teklad Laboratory Diet) and water available ad libitum. The housing
115
room was lit with a 60W (380 lumen) red light bulb allowing researcher
116
visibility. Rats could be individually identified by natural variation in their coat
117
markings. All the cages were provided with identical enrichment items (e.g.
118
nesting material, shelter).
119
120
Apparatus
121
In a different room to that in which the rats were housed, we positioned an eight
122
arm radial maze (arm length 70cm, arm width 10cm, hub diameter & height
123
30cm, Panlab) made of black Perspex with manually operated guillotine doors
124
leading from the decision area to each arm. The whole maze was raised 100cm
125
above the ground. Only one side of the maze was used (5 arms; see Figure One).
126
For the other side of the maze, the three unused arms had their doors
127
permanently closed, and a piece of white card (width 10cm, height 30cm) was
128
placed in front of the central unused arm (see Figure One) to provide a visual
7
129
landmark within the central maze hub. There were recessed goal pots (width
130
4cm, depth 3cm) located 6cm from the end of each arm; these could be removed
131
and cleaned between trials.
132
133
Figure One
134
135
In each trial, one goal pot was placed at the end of one of the five different maze
136
arms. The two ‘reference’ arms (rewarded or ‘aversive’) were positioned at 180˚
137
from each other. The three ambiguous ‘probe’ arms were positioned at
138
equidistant angles between the two reference locations, each separated by 45˚,
139
such that one probe arm was located midway between the two reference locations
140
(90˚), and the other two probe arms halfway between the central probe arm and
141
each reference arm (45˚ & 135˚; see Figure One). To let the rats into the arms
142
from the maze hub, guillotine doors located at the entrance to each arm were
143
opened/closed manually using a pulley system operated from behind a screen so
144
that the researcher was not visible to the subjects during training/testing. Also
145
behind the screen were a video and monitor linked to an overhead video camera
146
allowing the subjects to be recorded and their behaviour observed remotely.
147
148
The apparatus was based on that used in a previous judgement bias task [8] with
149
a few modifications. Firstly, the distance from the start box to the goal pot was
150
shorter (70cm vs. 80cm). Secondly, because the arms were enclosed, the subject
151
only had the option to move to the goal pot or not, and was not able to investigate
152
other locations. Thirdly, the angle between the locations was 45˚ rather than 15˚.
153
Finally, the location of the door to each arm acted as the cue for the subject to
8
154
approach the goal pot or not, whereas in the previous study it was the location of
155
the goal pot. All of these changes were made in an attempt to decrease the time
156
taken for the rats to learn the task, thus shortening the duration of the whole
157
cognitive bias test.
158
159
Treatments
160
For this study we manipulated the level of illumination of the test apparatus to
161
induce a change in emotional state (see Introduction). We illuminated the test
162
apparatus by suspending a light bulb 1m above the centre of the maze. A 60W
163
bulb was used to produce higher (white) light levels (700 lumen, 100lux (centre
164
of maze), 65lux (end of arms)), designed to induce a state of relatively moderate
165
anxiety, whilst a 10W bulb was used to produce lower (white) light levels (50
166
lumen, 15lux (centre of maze), 10lux (end of arms)), designed to induce a state
167
of relatively low anxiety [e.g. 14,16,23].
168
169
Half of the rats in this study (n=12) were exposed to high light conditions during
170
training, with half of these (i.e. n=6) exposed again to high light conditions
171
during testing (‘HH’), whilst the remainder were switched to low light conditions
172
during testing (‘HL’). The other 12 rats were exposed to low light conditions
173
during training, and then either continued to be exposed to low light during
174
testing (‘LL’; n=6) or were switched to high light conditions during testing
175
(‘LH’). The inclusion of both HL and LH treatments was important to test for the
176
contrasting predictions proposed by a light-induced change in emotional state
177
(namely that LH rats should be more anxious than HL rats) and that of a non-
178
emotional learning/context-based explanation, that would predict that any change
9
179
in light conditions (in either direction) would result in a similar performance
180
decrement [e.g. 24,25].
181
182
Rats were habituated to the apparatus, then trained and tested in two replicates
183
separated by a two day interval. We randomly allocated rats to the different
184
treatments, and they were counter-balanced between replicates and in the order
185
of testing.
186
187
Procedure
188
Habituation
189
Before habituation to the radial arm maze, we gave the rats prior exposure to the
190
food used as a reward in the task (Dustless Precision Pellets, 45mg, Bio-Serv),
191
placing nine pellets in each cage of three rats for three consecutive days. Rats
192
were then habituated to the apparatus on three occasions in order to ensure
193
familiarity with the test conditions, with obtaining food in the apparatus, with
194
being enclosed in the maze hub, and with all five maze arms (so that the ‘probe’
195
arms were not novel at testing). For each habituation session we placed
196
individual rats into the central maze hub with all the arm doors closed, and with
197
10 randomly scattered food pellets, for two minutes, and then opened the doors
198
of all five maze arms for a further three minutes. No food pellets were placed in
199
any of the maze arms to avoid associations between particular arms and the
200
presence of food being formed before training/testing commenced. We recorded
201
the number of pellets eaten (max. 10), the number of faecal boli produced (used
202
elsewhere as a measure of stress/anxiety (e.g. Ferre et al., 1995)) and the number
203
of arms visited (defined as a rat reaching the end of the arm).
10
204
205
Training
206
After habituation, we started training the rats. In each training trial only one goal
207
pot was present, either in the rewarded maze arm (containing four pellets) or in
208
the ‘aversive’ arm (containing one quinine-soaked pellet). We soaked the pellets
209
in quinine by briefly placing them into a 2% quinine sulphate (SIGMA) solution
210
before allowing them to dry overnight. The position of the rewarded and
211
‘aversive’ arms was balanced between individuals and treatments. During
212
training, subjects received 12 trials per day, half rewarded and half ‘aversive’.
213
214
The training schedules/sequences for each day were as follows. Day One: in
215
order to make it easier for the rats to learn the discrimination, for trials 1-8 the
216
goal pot was in the same location for two consecutive trials and was then placed
217
in the opposite location for the next two trials, always starting off with the
218
rewarded location for the first two trials (i.e. ++--++--). For trials 9-12, the goal
219
pot changed location with each trial. Day Two: we used a pseudo-random
220
sequence with no more than two consecutive presentations of the goal pot in the
221
same location, and equal numbers of both locations in trials 1-6 and trials 7-12
222
(e.g. ++--+-+-++--).
223
224
Before each trial all maze arms were cleaned with 70% alcohol. The goal pot was
225
also cleaned before being placed at the end of the appropriate arm, and contained
226
either four pellets or one quinine-soaked pellet according to the training/testing
227
schedule. Each rat was removed from its home cage in the housing room before
228
being transported to the test room in a clean cage. The rat was then placed into
11
229
the central maze hub for the 2min inter-trial interval (ITI), initially positioned
230
facing the intra-maze cue (the white card). Once the 2min ITI had elapsed, the
231
appropriate guillotine door (i.e. either the door of the rewarded or aversive arm)
232
was opened and the rat was able to enter that maze arm. We then recorded the
233
time taken for the nose of the rat to become level with the goal pot (because at
234
that point it could see the difference between one pellet (aversive) and four
235
pellets).
236
237
Once the rat had reached the goal pot, it was allowed a few seconds to eat the
238
reward if it chose, before being returned to the start box for the 2min ITI during
239
which time the maze was cleaned and prepared for the next trial. The first trial of
240
the first training day was open-ended and continued until the rat had eaten the
241
food pellets. For the rest of the trials there was a cut-off point of 2mins, and if the
242
rat failed to reach the goal pot in this time, it was returned to the start box for the
243
2min ITI and the arena prepared for the next trial as normal. Trials in which rats
244
failed to reach the goal pot within the 2min cut-off were not repeated. Once the
245
rat had completed all 12 trials it was transported back to the housing room and
246
returned to its home cage. The central maze hub, as well as the floor and walls of
247
each of the maze arms, were then cleaned before the next rat was collected.
248
249
Testing
250
Once the rats had successfully discriminated between the reference maze arms,
251
as determined by showing a significant difference in their latency to approach the
252
rewarded and ‘aversive’ goal pots, they were tested for three days during which
253
subjects were exposed to each of the three ambiguous maze arms once per day,
12
254
interspersed within a sequence of exposures to the rewarded and ‘aversive’
255
reference maze arms. The testing schedule consisted of 13 trials in total, with
256
five rewarded trials, five ‘aversive’ trials, and the three ambiguous locations (one
257
trial each). The three ambiguous trials were positioned at trial 5, trial 9 and trial
258
13, and the order in which they were presented was counterbalanced over the
259
three test days. The overall sequence consisted of alternated single rewarded and
260
‘aversive’ trials, starting either with a rewarded trial or an ‘aversive’ trial,
261
counterbalanced between treatments. This testing schedule/sequence was
262
designed so that there were equal numbers of ambiguous trials preceded by
263
rewarded trials as there were preceded by ‘aversive’ trials, and that this was the
264
same for all treatments.
265
266
The goal pots at the end of the ambiguous maze arms contained no food pellets,
267
and so we only carried out three trials for each of the ambiguous maze arms in
268
order to minimise the opportunity for associations to develop. Latency to reach
269
the goal pot was recorded in all ambiguous trials.
270
271
Data analysis
272
Unless specifically mentioned in the text, all data met the requirements for
273
parametric tests (e.g. normality, homogeneity of variance etc.,) either in an
274
untransformed or transformed state. The statistics package used was SPSS
275
version 14. This study was carried out in two replicates separated by a two day
276
interval, with treatment groups equally balanced across the replicates. The
277
training data were analysed separately for both replicates, since this analysis was
278
conducted during training to determine when the rats were ready for testing.
13
279
Testing, however, was analysed with both replicates combined. For training, the
280
data for high and low level lighting were compared separately (i.e. as Treatments
281
H or L) because at this point the rats had only experienced either high or low
282
light levels. However, for testing, Treatment was modelled with four levels (i.e.
283
as Treatments HH, HL, LH, LL) so that the results incorporated the complete
284
experience of each rat, in terms of the light levels to which they had been
285
exposed, taken together over the course of both training and testing.
286
287
288
Results
289
290
Habituation
291
Only during the first habituation trial were enough faeces produced to be able to
292
carry out a statistical analysis. Using a one-way ANOVA, we found that there
293
was no difference between the treatments (high vs. low light level) in the number
294
of faecal boli produced during this first habituation trial (F3, 20=0.237, P>0.1). All
295
the rats ate all of the food pellets in each of the three habituation trials. The rats,
296
regardless of treatment, showed an increase over the three habituation trials in
297
the number of visits made to the maze arms (F2, 40=3.566, P=0.038), with post-
298
hoc tests indicating that significantly more visits to maze arms were made in the
299
third trial than in the previous two. There were no significant differences
300
between treatments nor a significant interaction between treatment and trial
301
(P>0.1) for maze arm visits.
302
303
Training
14
304
For the training analysis we calculated the average latency to access the goal pot
305
on the six rewarded trials and on the six ‘aversive’ trials for each rat/day, with
306
the exception of the first day of training in which the open-ended first trial (to the
307
rewarded location) and the first trial to the ‘aversive’ location were both
308
excluded. This was because for both these trials the reward outcome was
309
unknown to the subjects as it was their first experience of either location. One rat
310
from the HL treatment (in the second replicate) was removed from the
311
experiment because it failed to eat from the rewarded location following
312
exposure to the ‘aversive’ food. We continued to train the rats until they showed
313
a clear difference in their average latency to reach the rewarded and ‘aversive’
314
locations, and for both replicates this was clearly observed after the second day
315
of training (see Figure Two). We used a repeated measures General Linear
316
Model (GLM) with Treatment (high light level vs. low light level) as a between
317
subject factor, and Location (rewarded vs. ‘aversive’) and Day (1&2) as within
318
subjects factors. For both replicates (analysed separately) we observed a
319
significant Day*Location interaction (Replicate One: F1, 10=7.48, P=0.021;
320
Replicate Two: F1, 9=9.48, P=0.013), but no significant differences in the time
321
taken to reach the goal pot between the treatments, either as a main effect or
322
interaction (P>0.1).
323
324
Post-hoc analysis of the Day*Location interactions revealed an increasing
325
difference in the latency to approach the two locations over time, predominantly
326
due to the increased speed in running to the rewarded location over the two days,
327
and with more variation in the latency to approach the aversive location (paired t-
328
tests: Replicate One: D1 rewarded vs. D1 ‘aversive’ t11=-2.56, P=0.026; D2
15
329
rewarded vs. D2 ‘aversive’ t11=-4.67, P=0.001; D1 rewarded vs. D2 rewarded
330
t11=5.46, P<0.001; D1 ‘aversive’ vs. D2 ‘aversive’ t11=0.11, P>0.1; Replicate
331
Two: D1 rewarded vs. D1 ‘aversive’ t10=0.64, P>0.1; D2 rewarded vs. D2
332
‘aversive’ t10=-2.38, P=0.038; D1 rewarded vs. D2 rewarded t10=5.89, P<0.001;
333
D1 ‘aversive’ vs. D2 ‘aversive’ t10=0.51, P>0.1). Testing was therefore
334
implemented after the second day of training.
335
336
Figure Two
337
338
Testing
339
Testing was carried out over three days for each rat, with five rewarded trials,
340
five ‘aversive’ trials, and one trial for each of the three ‘probe’ locations per day.
341
For each rat, we calculated the average latency for the 15 rewarded trials, the
342
average latency for the 15 ‘aversive’ trials, and the average latency over the three
343
trials for each of the three different ‘probe’ locations (see Figure Three). Probe
344
and reference locations were analysed separately because of the difference
345
between them in the number of trials used to calculate the means.
346
347
Figure Three
348
349
We used a repeated measures GLM to compare the latency to approach the two
350
‘reference’ locations (rewarded and ‘aversive’) to determine whether the subjects
351
still accurately discriminated between the locations during testing, and whether
352
or not there was any treatment effect. Data were log-transformed, and we
353
included Treatment (HH, HL, LH, LL, between subjects) and Location (rewarded
16
354
vs. ‘aversive’, within subjects) as factors. As expected, we found a highly
355
significant effect of location (F1, 19=63.32, P<0.001), with subjects taking
356
significantly longer to approach the ‘aversive’ location than the rewarded
357
location. There was no overall effect of Treatment nor any interactions (P>0.1).
358
359
Our next analysis was to determine whether or not the animals responded
360
differently to the probe locations during testing, and whether this response
361
differed between the four treatments. In order to take into account individual
362
differences in performance (i.e. the latency to approach the reference locations),
363
we calculated the average value between the time taken to reach the rewarded
364
and aversive locations during testing for each rat, and included this as a covariate
365
in the analysis [e.g. 8]. We found a significant Treatment effect (F3,18=3.228,
366
P=0.047), indicating that there was a difference between the treatments in the
367
latency to approach the probe locations. This did not depend on the specific
368
probe location (Probe*Treatment interaction (F6,36=0.357, P=0.901)), and there
369
was no main effect of Probe location (repeated measures GLM: F2,36=0.48,
370
P=0.623). Post-hoc pairwise investigation of the overall Treatment effect using
371
Bonferroni adjustment revealed that rats in the HL treatment ran significantly
372
faster to the probe locations (P=0.043) than rats in the LH treatment (see Figure
373
Four). There were no further significant differences between the treatments
374
(P>0.1).
375
376
Figure Four
377
378
Discussion
17
379
380
Habituation
381
The rats quickly habituated to the experimental apparatus, as demonstrated by
382
their increased investigation of the maze arms over time. The fact that all the
383
food pellets were eaten in each of the habituation trials, together with the low
384
levels of defaecation, suggests that the apparatus did not induce high levels of
385
anxiety during this habituation period. The lack of a treatment difference (high
386
light level vs. low light level) at this stage suggests that this initial contrast in
387
light levels did not affect the behaviours measured during habituation.
388
389
Training
390
The results demonstrated that after two days, the rats were able to discriminate
391
between the rewarded and ‘aversive’ locations based on the time taken to
392
approach the goal pot, with an increasing difference across training days as a
393
consequence of the rats speeding up their approach to the rewarded location. This
394
confirms the findings of a previous study in which we used location as the basis
395
for a spatial judgement task [8]. In the current study, our new technique reduced
396
the time required to learn the discrimination (from six to two days). As
397
mentioned earlier (see ‘Apparatus’), the design of the current study differed in a
398
number of aspects to our previous study [8]. For example, in Burman et al., [8]
399
the stimuli were either rewarded or unrewarded, whereas in the current study we
400
used rewarded versus ‘aversive’ stimuli. These modifications appeared to be
401
reflected in faster learning speeds. The difference in reward contingency may
402
also explain differences in the ‘pattern’ of discrimination learning between the
403
two experiments. In Burman et al., [8] the rats initially ran quickly to both
18
404
locations, before slowing their approach to the unrewarded location (a gradual
405
recognition of the absence of a food reward), whereas in the current study the
406
rats showed an initially slow approach to both locations before increasing their
407
approach to the ‘rewarded’ location (a gradual recognition of the presence of a
408
non-aversive food reward).
409
410
There was no difference in training performance between the two
411
treatments (high light level vs. low light level) suggesting that there was no
412
difference in either the level of food motivation, learning ability or general
413
locomotor activity as a consequence of being exposed to the different light
414
levels. Any differences between the treatments during testing were therefore
415
unlikely to be due to alterations in motivational state or general activity. In terms
416
of visual perception, it also does not appear that the task was either more or less
417
difficult to perform in the two different light levels. There was also little
418
difference between the two replicates in the time taken to learn the task, and both
419
replicates showed a similar learning pattern (see above).
420
421
Testing
422
During testing the subject rats continued to discriminate accurately between the
423
rewarded and ‘aversive’ reference locations, and this ability was shown not to be
424
influenced by treatment. This presents further evidence, additional to that
425
gathered during training, that any treatment effects on the subjects’ response to
426
the ambiguous probe locations was less likely to have arisen as a consequence of
427
general changes in motivation rather than due to a specific influence on the
428
appraisal of the ambiguous probe locations. This includes any motivational
19
429
influences arising specifically due to the change in light levels which occurred
430
for half the rats between training and testing (in comparison to the unchanged
431
light levels received by all subjects during training).
432
433
In line with our initial predictions, we found that rats tested under high light
434
conditions after having previously experienced the apparatus under low light
435
conditions (LH) and those tested under low light conditions after previously
436
experiencing the apparatus under high light conditions (HL) differed most
437
strongly in their judgement of the ambiguous stimuli, with HL rats running faster
438
to all probe locations. However, in contrast to our predictions, those animals
439
trained and tested in high light levels (HH) did not show a more negative
440
judgment of the ambiguous stimuli than those trained and tested in low light
441
levels (LL).
442
443
The difference between HL and LH rats indicates that a change in light
444
conditions per se resulting in a performance decrement (see Methods
445
‘Treatments’) was unlikely to be responsible for the findings, but rather that a
446
specific difference in emotional state was a more likely explanation. Being
447
switched from a high light to a low light level likely resulted in a relative
448
decrease in anxiety compared to those animals switched from a low to a high
449
light level, and this may have resulted in the less negative (or more positive)
450
judgement of the ambiguous stimuli. Certainly, it appears that exposure to
451
sudden darkness can result in a reduced state of anxiety in rats [e.g. 19], although
452
sudden darkness is a more extreme change in light levels than was used in the
453
current study. It is possible that a form of relief was experienced by those
20
454
animals in the HL treatment following the omission of a more anxiety-inducing
455
light level [e.g. 26]. The observed results could also be explained in a similar
456
way to incentive contrast theory [e.g. 27], where emotional state and subsequent
457
behaviour is determined by relative (and contrasting), rather than absolute,
458
experience. If this were the case, then experiencing a change in light levels is
459
likely to have, at least temporarily, more of an influence on emotional state than
460
exposure to unchanging illumination. Changes in motivation or general activity
461
due to the change in light levels could possibly account for the results observed
462
but, if this was the explanation, we would also have expected to see differences
463
in responses of HL and LH rats to the unambiguous training locations and, as
464
discussed above, these were not observed.
465
466
It is worth taking time to consider why we did not observe the predicted
467
difference between the HH and LL treatments in their judgement of the
468
ambiguous locations. One possibility is that, on their own, the HH and LL
469
treatments were not strong enough to induce a measurable difference in
470
emotional state, as tentatively demonstrated by the lack of difference during
471
habituation, and only became strong enough once a contrast was established by
472
changing the light levels at testing. A further possibility is that those rats tested in
473
the unchanging light conditions (HH & LL) had become habituated to the anxiety
474
inducing effect of the light level over the course of training (two days) thereby
475
‘normalizing’ their subsequent response at testing, whereas those rats that
476
changed light levels following training (HL & LH) had a ‘refreshed’ reaction to a
477
light level that they only experienced for the first time at testing.
478
21
479
Our present finding extends previous research [e.g. 4,5-9] into the use of
480
cognitive bias to assess animal emotions, demonstrating that acute/short-term
481
inductions of anxiety can generate an apparent bias in cognitive processing, in
482
terms of judgements of ambiguity. It thus appears that such cognitive bias tasks
483
may be able to identify different, but similarly valenced, emotional states, as
484
proposed by Paul et al. [3]. This would help advance understanding of the
485
affective (emotional) mechanisms underpinning cognitive bias, as well as
486
methods for the assessment of animal emotion and welfare.
487
488
Unlike other studies [e.g. 4,8], we found no difference between treatments in
489
their judgement of specific ambiguous probe locations (e.g. either the probe
490
nearest the ‘negative’ end or the probe nearest the ‘positive’ end), but instead
491
found an overall difference inclusive of all three probe locations. One possible
492
reason for this is that the probe locations in this study were more easily
493
distinguishable to the rats than in a previous study [8] because they were
494
positioned further apart from one another and from the reference locations (45˚
495
rather than 15˚). It could, therefore, have been that all three probe locations were
496
viewed similarly, as being of ambiguous outcome (i.e. they might contain either
497
palatable or aversive food), rather than being of ambiguous location (i.e. they
498
were not confused with the actual reference locations).
499
500
501
Conclusion
502
To conclude, we observed a treatment difference in the judgement of three
503
ambiguous locations in a judgement bias task, with rats switched from high
22
504
(60W) to low (10W) light levels displaying a more positive judgement of
505
ambiguous locations compared to rats switched from low to high light levels.
506
This result suggests that the judgement bias technique might be useful as an
507
indicator of acute changes in anxiety and other emotional states, as well as
508
allowing differentiation between acute and more chronic negative emotional
509
states such as depression. Finally, as a quickly learned task, it has the potential
510
benefit of being adaptable for a wide range of animal species.
511
512
513
Acknowledgements
514
515
This work was funded by the BBSRC Animal Welfare Initiative.
516
517
23
518
Figure One:
519
520
521
Visual cue
Guillotine door
522
523
524
Rewarded location
Probe location
‘Aversive’ location
525
526
527
528
529
24
530
Figure Two:
531
532
Latency to approach (s)
25.00
20.00
Rewarded R1
15.00
Aversive R1
Rewarded R2
10.00
Aversive R2
5.00
0.00
1
Training Day
533
534
535
25
2
536
Figure Three:
537
Latency to approach (s)
538
16.00
***
14.00
12.00
10.00
8.00
6.00
4.00
2.00
0.00
Aversive
Probe
nearest
aversive
Middle
probe
Location
539
540
541
26
Probe
nearest
rewarded
Rewarded
542
Figure Four:
543
544
*
545
546
547
548
549
27
550
551
Figure One: A diagram of the experimental apparatus, showing the two reference
552
locations (either rewarded or aversive), the three ambiguous probe locations, the
553
guillotine doors and the position of the visual cue.
554
555
Figure Two: A graph displaying the subjects’ ability to discriminate between the
556
rewarded and aversive locations over the two days of training, as determined by
557
differences in latency to approach (seconds). Data are presented for each of the
558
two replicates (R1 & R2), and averaged for treatment. Data are means ± standard
559
error of the mean.
560
561
Figure Three: A graph displaying the latency for the subjects to approach
562
(seconds) the five different locations (either reference or probe locations) during
563
testing (averaged for the three test days) and averaged for treatment. Data are
564
combined for the two replicates. Data are means ± standard error of the mean.
565
***P<0.001
566
567
Figure Four: A graph displaying the difference between the treatments (HH, HL,
568
LH & LL) in the latency to approach (seconds) the ambiguous probe locations
569
(averaged for the three probe locations) during testing (averaged for the three test
570
days). Data are combined for the two replicates. Data are presented as estimated
571
marginal means from the GLM output in order to account for the use of a
572
covariate. *P<0.05
573
574
28
575
References
576
1. Eysenck, M. W.; Mogg, K.; May, J.; Richards, A.; Mathews, A. Bias in
577
interpretation of ambiguous sentences related to threat in anxiety. J. Abnorm.
578
Psychol. 1991, 100: 144-150.
579
580
2. Mogg, K.; Bradley, B. P. A cognitive-motivational analysis of anxiety. Behav.
581
Res. Ther. 1998, 36: 809-848.
582
583
3. Paul, E. S.; Harding, E. J.; Mendl, M. Measuring emotional processes in
584
animals: The utility of a cognitive approach. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2005, 29:
585
469-491.
586
587
4. Harding, E. J.; Paul, E. S.; Mendl, M. Animal behavior - cognitive bias and
588
affective state. Nature. 2004, 427: 312-312.
589
590
5. Bateson, M.; Matheson, S. M. Performance on a categorisation task suggests
591
that removal of environmental enrichment induces 'pessimism' in captive
592
european starlings (sturnus vulgaris). Anim. Welf. 2007, 16: 33-36.
593
594
6. Matheson, S. M.; Asher, L.; Bateson, M. Larger, enriched cages are associated
595
with 'optimistic' response biases in captive european starlings (sturnus vulgaris).
596
Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2007, 109: 374-383.
597
598
7. Burman, O.; Parker, R.; Paul, E.; Mendl, M. Sensitivity to reward loss as an
599
indicator of animal affect and welfare. Biology Letters. 2008, 4: 330-333.
29
600
601
8. Burman, O.; Parker, R.; Paul, E.; Mendl, M. A spatial judgement task to
602
determine background emotional state in laboratory rats (rattus norvegicus).
603
Anim. Behav. 2008, 76: 801-809.
604
605
9. Mendl, M.; Burman, O.; Parker, R.; Paul, E. Cognitive bias as an indicator of
606
animal emotion and welfare: Emerging evidence and underlying mechanisms.
607
Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. In press.
608
609
10. Harkin, A.; Houlihan, D. D.; Kelly, J. P. Reduction in preference for
610
saccharin by repeated unpredictable stress in mice and its prevention by
611
imipramine. J. Psychopharmacol. (Oxf). 2002, 16: 115-123.
612
613
11. Beck, A. T.; Rush, A. J.; Shaw, B. F.; Emery, G. Cognitive therapy of
614
depression: A treatment manual. New York: Guilford Press; 1979.
615
616
12. Blanchette, I.; Richards, A.; Cross, A. Anxiety and the interpretation of
617
ambiguous facial expressions: The influence of contextual cues. Q. J. Exp.
618
Psychol. 2007, 60: 1101-1115.
619
620
13. Valle, F. P. Effects of strain, sex, and illumination on open-field behavior of
621
rats. Am. J. Psychol. 1970, 83: 103-&.
622
30
623
14. Cosquer, B.; Kuster, N.; Cassel, J. C. Whole-body exposure to 2.45 ghz
624
electromagnetic fields does not alter 12-arm radial-maze with reduced access to
625
spatial cues in rats. Behav. Brain Res. 2005, 161: 331-334.
626
627
15. Garcia, A. M. B.; Cardenas, F. P.; Morato, S. Effect of different illumination
628
levels on rat behavior in the elevated plus-maze. Physiol. Behav. 2005, 85: 265-
629
270.
630
631
16. Walker, D. L.; Davis, M. Anxiogenic effects of high illumination levels
632
assessed with the acoustic startle response in rats. Biol. Psychiatry. 1997, 42:
633
461-471.
634
635
17. Grillon, C.; Pellowski, M.; Merikangas, K. R.; Davis, M. Darkness facilitates
636
the acoustic startle reflex in humans. Biol. Psychiatry. 1997, 42: 453-460.
637
638
18. File, S. E.; Seth, P. A review of 25 years of the social interaction test. Eur. J.
639
Pharmacol. 2003, 463: 35-53.
640
641
19. Nasello, A. G.; Machado, C.; Bastos, J. F.; Felicio, L. F. Sudden darkness
642
induces a high activity low anxiety state in male and female rats. Physiol. Behav.
643
1998, 63: 451-454.
644
645
20. Nasello, A. G.; Sassatani, A. S.; Ferreira, F. S.; Felicio, L. F.; Tieppo, C. A.
646
Modulation by sudden darkness of apomorphine-induced behavioral responses.
647
Physiol. Behav. 2003, 78: 521-528.
31
648
649
21. Bert, B.; Felicio, L. F.; Fink, H.; Nasello, A. G. The use of sudden darkness
650
in mice: A behavioural and pharmacological approach. Psychopharmacology
651
(Berl). 2005, 179: 846-853.
652
653
22. MacLeod, A. K.; Tata, P.; Kentish, J.; Jacobsen, H. Retrospective and
654
prospective cognitions in anxiety and depression. Cognition & Emotion. 1997,
655
11: 467-479.
656
657
23. Hale, M. W.; Bouwknecht, J. A.; Spiga, F.; Shekhar, A.; Lowry, C. A.
658
Exposure to high- and low-light conditions in an open-field test of anxiety
659
increases c-fos expression in specific subdivisions of the rat basolateral
660
amygdaloid complex. Brain Res. Bull. 2006, 71: 174-182.
661
662
24. Burman, O. H. P.; Mendl, M. The effects of environmental context on
663
laboratory rat social recognition. Anim. Behav. 1999, 58: 629-634.
664
665
25. Burman, O. H. P.; Mendl, M. Recognition of conspecific odors by laboratory
666
rats (rattus norvegicus) does not show context specificity. J. Comp. Psychol.
667
2002, 116: 247-252.
668
669
26. Rolls, E. T. Emotion explained. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2005.
670
671
27. Flaherty, C. Incentive relativity. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press;
672
1996.
32
673
33
Download