Physiology & Behavior (2009) Volume 98: 345-350 10.1016/j.physbeh.2009.06.012 Final Revision – NOT EDITED by the journal Anxiety-induced cognitive bias in non-human animals OLIVER H. P. BURMAN, RICHARD M. A. PARKER, ELIZABETH S. PAUL, MICHAEL .T. MENDL University of Bristol, Bristol, BS40 5DU, U.K. Corresponding author: Oliver H. P. Burman Word count: 5019 1 1 OLIVER H. P. BURMAN, RICHARD M. A. PARKER, ELIZABETH S. PAUL, 2 MICHAEL .T. MENDL. Anxiety-induced cognitive bias in non-human animals. 3 PHYSIOL BEHAV. - As in humans, ‘cognitive biases’ in the way in which 4 animals judge ambiguous stimuli may be influenced by emotional state and 5 hence a valuable new indicator of animal emotion. There is increasing evidence 6 that animals experiencing different emotional states following exposure to long- 7 term environmental manipulations show contrasting biases in their judgement of 8 ambiguous stimuli. However, the specific type of induced emotional state is 9 usually unknown. We investigated whether a short-term manipulation of 10 emotional state has a similar effect on cognitive bias, using changes in light 11 intensity; a treatment specifically related to anxiety induction. Twenty-four male 12 rats were trained to discriminate between two different locations, in either high 13 (‘H’) or low (‘L’) light levels. One location was rewarded with palatable food 14 and the other with aversive food. Once the rats had shown spatial discrimination, 15 by running significantly faster to the rewarded location, they were tested with 16 three ambiguous locations intermediate between the rewarded and aversive 17 locations, and their latency to approach each location recorded. Half the rats were 18 tested in the same light levels as during training, the remainder were switched. 19 Rats switched from high to low light levels (putatively the least negative 20 emotional manipulation) ran significantly faster to all three ambiguous probes 21 than those rats switched from low to high light levels (putatively the most 22 negative manipulation). This suggests that the judgement bias technique might be 23 useful as an indicator of short-term changes in anxiety for non-human animals. 24 2 25 Keywords: Emotion; Cognition; Anxiety; Cognitive Bias; Emotional state; 26 Behaviour; Animal Welfare 27 28 3 29 Introduction 30 Humans experiencing different background emotional states display contrasting 31 cognitive-affective biases (henceforth referred to as ‘cognitive bias’) in their 32 judgement of ambiguous stimuli. For example, people in a negative state tend to 33 make negative judgements about ambiguous situations or stimuli [e.g. 1,2,3]. 34 Since Harding et al. [4] first demonstrated a novel technique to determine the 35 emotional state of non-human animals by measuring changes in judgement bias, 36 there has been considerable interest in the further development and validation of 37 this technique [e.g. 5,6-9]. Compared to many existing behavioural and 38 physiological measures of non-human emotional state, one potential advantage of 39 a cognitive bias approach is the ability to discriminate between similarly- 40 valenced emotional states such as depression and anxiety [3]. However, this 41 potential benefit has yet to be demonstrated. 42 43 Previous studies have manipulated emotional state by using unpredictable 44 housing events, each designed to be mildly stressful [rats: 4], or the 45 absence/removal of enrichment [starlings: 5,6, rats: 7,8]. However, these 46 affective (emotional) manipulations have been relatively long-term/chronic in 47 duration (e.g. enrichment absence: 21 days [8]; 7 days [5]; unpredictable 48 housing: 9 days [4]), and have induced negative emotional states that, whilst of 49 unknown specificity, are perhaps more likely to be related to states of depression 50 rather than anxiety (e.g. unpredictable housing: [10]; enrichment 51 absence/removal: depression and the experience of loss in humans, [11]). It is 52 therefore of considerable interest to investigate whether cognitive bias tasks in 53 non-human animals can be influenced by acute anxiety-related emotions as well 4 54 as the more chronic depression-like mood states studied previously, as has been 55 demonstrated in humans [e.g. 12]. 56 57 The current study addresses this issue by employing a treatment designed to 58 induce an acute/short-term change in anxiety. This treatment involves varying 59 the level of ambient light, since higher light levels are considered increasingly 60 anxiety inducing for a nocturnal species such as the laboratory rat [e.g. 13,14,15]; 61 presumably as an evolved response to increased threat of predation. For example, 62 the acoustic startle reflex in rats is potentiated by exposure to high light levels 63 [16] whereas in humans it is potentiated by exposure to darkness [17]. Light 64 levels can therefore be manipulated in order to generate different levels of 65 anxiety, as evidenced by the use of different light levels during anxiety testing 66 [e.g. 18] depending on whether anxiogenic or anxiolytic treatment effects are to 67 be studied. There is also evidence that a change/switch in light levels can itself 68 influence emotional state, for example, sudden darkness can result in clear 69 anxiolytic effects on rodent behaviour [e.g. 19,20,21]. For this reason, we 70 decided to investigate the effect of both constant and changing light levels on 71 cognitive bias in this study. Light intensity satisfies the requirement for an 72 appropriate affective (emotional) manipulation, namely that there is a previously 73 demonstrated effectiveness for inducing a change in emotional state (specifically 74 anxiety), therefore allowing the construction of clear predictions concerning the 75 potential impact of our treatment on cognitive bias. 76 77 The aim of this study was therefore to test the prediction that rats exposed to high 78 light levels will judge ambiguous stimuli negatively in comparison to those rats 5 79 exposed to low light levels, due to being in a putative negative emotional state 80 (anxiety). Furthermore, we predicted that rats tested in high light levels following 81 previous experience of the apparatus under low light conditions would be most 82 anxious and those tested under low light levels after experiencing high light 83 conditions would be least anxious, and hence these two groups of rats would 84 show the strongest contrast in their judgements of ambiguous stimuli. 85 86 In addition to choosing the appropriate treatment necessary to induce the desired 87 emotional state, it is also important to select an appropriate reward contingency 88 for the cognitive bias task that is being used as a proxy indicator of that 89 emotional state. It has been proposed that depression, being related to the 90 experience of loss [11], may be more commonly associated with a decreased 91 expectation of positive events, whereas anxiety, being threat-related [11], may be 92 more commonly associated with an increased expectation of negative events 93 [22]. In a previous study [8], we used both a treatment (loss of enrichment) most 94 likely to induce a depression-like state, and a reward contingency 95 (presence/absence of a reward) most likely to be sensitive to cognitive biases 96 associated with that state. In contrast, in this study we employ a treatment 97 designed to induce a change in anxiety (light levels), and a reward contingency 98 designed to be most sensitive to cognitive biases likely to be associated with that 99 state, namely the presence/absence of a negative event (the perceived ‘threat’ of 100 ingesting an aversive food). 101 102 103 Materials and Methods 6 104 105 Subjects 106 This work was carried out under UK Home Office licence (PPL 30/2249). We 107 used twenty four male Lister-hooded rats (Harlan, UK), approximately nine 108 months old at the start of testing. The rats had previously been used (at least three 109 months previously) in studies of incentive contrast and cognitive bias [7,8], and 110 so individuals were randomly allocated between the different treatments in the 111 current study in order to minimize any potential influence of previous 112 experience. The rats were housed in groups of three in standard cages (33cm X 113 50cm X 21cm) on a 12hr reversed light cycle, lights off 0800-2000, with food 114 (Harlan Teklad Laboratory Diet) and water available ad libitum. The housing 115 room was lit with a 60W (380 lumen) red light bulb allowing researcher 116 visibility. Rats could be individually identified by natural variation in their coat 117 markings. All the cages were provided with identical enrichment items (e.g. 118 nesting material, shelter). 119 120 Apparatus 121 In a different room to that in which the rats were housed, we positioned an eight 122 arm radial maze (arm length 70cm, arm width 10cm, hub diameter & height 123 30cm, Panlab) made of black Perspex with manually operated guillotine doors 124 leading from the decision area to each arm. The whole maze was raised 100cm 125 above the ground. Only one side of the maze was used (5 arms; see Figure One). 126 For the other side of the maze, the three unused arms had their doors 127 permanently closed, and a piece of white card (width 10cm, height 30cm) was 128 placed in front of the central unused arm (see Figure One) to provide a visual 7 129 landmark within the central maze hub. There were recessed goal pots (width 130 4cm, depth 3cm) located 6cm from the end of each arm; these could be removed 131 and cleaned between trials. 132 133 Figure One 134 135 In each trial, one goal pot was placed at the end of one of the five different maze 136 arms. The two ‘reference’ arms (rewarded or ‘aversive’) were positioned at 180˚ 137 from each other. The three ambiguous ‘probe’ arms were positioned at 138 equidistant angles between the two reference locations, each separated by 45˚, 139 such that one probe arm was located midway between the two reference locations 140 (90˚), and the other two probe arms halfway between the central probe arm and 141 each reference arm (45˚ & 135˚; see Figure One). To let the rats into the arms 142 from the maze hub, guillotine doors located at the entrance to each arm were 143 opened/closed manually using a pulley system operated from behind a screen so 144 that the researcher was not visible to the subjects during training/testing. Also 145 behind the screen were a video and monitor linked to an overhead video camera 146 allowing the subjects to be recorded and their behaviour observed remotely. 147 148 The apparatus was based on that used in a previous judgement bias task [8] with 149 a few modifications. Firstly, the distance from the start box to the goal pot was 150 shorter (70cm vs. 80cm). Secondly, because the arms were enclosed, the subject 151 only had the option to move to the goal pot or not, and was not able to investigate 152 other locations. Thirdly, the angle between the locations was 45˚ rather than 15˚. 153 Finally, the location of the door to each arm acted as the cue for the subject to 8 154 approach the goal pot or not, whereas in the previous study it was the location of 155 the goal pot. All of these changes were made in an attempt to decrease the time 156 taken for the rats to learn the task, thus shortening the duration of the whole 157 cognitive bias test. 158 159 Treatments 160 For this study we manipulated the level of illumination of the test apparatus to 161 induce a change in emotional state (see Introduction). We illuminated the test 162 apparatus by suspending a light bulb 1m above the centre of the maze. A 60W 163 bulb was used to produce higher (white) light levels (700 lumen, 100lux (centre 164 of maze), 65lux (end of arms)), designed to induce a state of relatively moderate 165 anxiety, whilst a 10W bulb was used to produce lower (white) light levels (50 166 lumen, 15lux (centre of maze), 10lux (end of arms)), designed to induce a state 167 of relatively low anxiety [e.g. 14,16,23]. 168 169 Half of the rats in this study (n=12) were exposed to high light conditions during 170 training, with half of these (i.e. n=6) exposed again to high light conditions 171 during testing (‘HH’), whilst the remainder were switched to low light conditions 172 during testing (‘HL’). The other 12 rats were exposed to low light conditions 173 during training, and then either continued to be exposed to low light during 174 testing (‘LL’; n=6) or were switched to high light conditions during testing 175 (‘LH’). The inclusion of both HL and LH treatments was important to test for the 176 contrasting predictions proposed by a light-induced change in emotional state 177 (namely that LH rats should be more anxious than HL rats) and that of a non- 178 emotional learning/context-based explanation, that would predict that any change 9 179 in light conditions (in either direction) would result in a similar performance 180 decrement [e.g. 24,25]. 181 182 Rats were habituated to the apparatus, then trained and tested in two replicates 183 separated by a two day interval. We randomly allocated rats to the different 184 treatments, and they were counter-balanced between replicates and in the order 185 of testing. 186 187 Procedure 188 Habituation 189 Before habituation to the radial arm maze, we gave the rats prior exposure to the 190 food used as a reward in the task (Dustless Precision Pellets, 45mg, Bio-Serv), 191 placing nine pellets in each cage of three rats for three consecutive days. Rats 192 were then habituated to the apparatus on three occasions in order to ensure 193 familiarity with the test conditions, with obtaining food in the apparatus, with 194 being enclosed in the maze hub, and with all five maze arms (so that the ‘probe’ 195 arms were not novel at testing). For each habituation session we placed 196 individual rats into the central maze hub with all the arm doors closed, and with 197 10 randomly scattered food pellets, for two minutes, and then opened the doors 198 of all five maze arms for a further three minutes. No food pellets were placed in 199 any of the maze arms to avoid associations between particular arms and the 200 presence of food being formed before training/testing commenced. We recorded 201 the number of pellets eaten (max. 10), the number of faecal boli produced (used 202 elsewhere as a measure of stress/anxiety (e.g. Ferre et al., 1995)) and the number 203 of arms visited (defined as a rat reaching the end of the arm). 10 204 205 Training 206 After habituation, we started training the rats. In each training trial only one goal 207 pot was present, either in the rewarded maze arm (containing four pellets) or in 208 the ‘aversive’ arm (containing one quinine-soaked pellet). We soaked the pellets 209 in quinine by briefly placing them into a 2% quinine sulphate (SIGMA) solution 210 before allowing them to dry overnight. The position of the rewarded and 211 ‘aversive’ arms was balanced between individuals and treatments. During 212 training, subjects received 12 trials per day, half rewarded and half ‘aversive’. 213 214 The training schedules/sequences for each day were as follows. Day One: in 215 order to make it easier for the rats to learn the discrimination, for trials 1-8 the 216 goal pot was in the same location for two consecutive trials and was then placed 217 in the opposite location for the next two trials, always starting off with the 218 rewarded location for the first two trials (i.e. ++--++--). For trials 9-12, the goal 219 pot changed location with each trial. Day Two: we used a pseudo-random 220 sequence with no more than two consecutive presentations of the goal pot in the 221 same location, and equal numbers of both locations in trials 1-6 and trials 7-12 222 (e.g. ++--+-+-++--). 223 224 Before each trial all maze arms were cleaned with 70% alcohol. The goal pot was 225 also cleaned before being placed at the end of the appropriate arm, and contained 226 either four pellets or one quinine-soaked pellet according to the training/testing 227 schedule. Each rat was removed from its home cage in the housing room before 228 being transported to the test room in a clean cage. The rat was then placed into 11 229 the central maze hub for the 2min inter-trial interval (ITI), initially positioned 230 facing the intra-maze cue (the white card). Once the 2min ITI had elapsed, the 231 appropriate guillotine door (i.e. either the door of the rewarded or aversive arm) 232 was opened and the rat was able to enter that maze arm. We then recorded the 233 time taken for the nose of the rat to become level with the goal pot (because at 234 that point it could see the difference between one pellet (aversive) and four 235 pellets). 236 237 Once the rat had reached the goal pot, it was allowed a few seconds to eat the 238 reward if it chose, before being returned to the start box for the 2min ITI during 239 which time the maze was cleaned and prepared for the next trial. The first trial of 240 the first training day was open-ended and continued until the rat had eaten the 241 food pellets. For the rest of the trials there was a cut-off point of 2mins, and if the 242 rat failed to reach the goal pot in this time, it was returned to the start box for the 243 2min ITI and the arena prepared for the next trial as normal. Trials in which rats 244 failed to reach the goal pot within the 2min cut-off were not repeated. Once the 245 rat had completed all 12 trials it was transported back to the housing room and 246 returned to its home cage. The central maze hub, as well as the floor and walls of 247 each of the maze arms, were then cleaned before the next rat was collected. 248 249 Testing 250 Once the rats had successfully discriminated between the reference maze arms, 251 as determined by showing a significant difference in their latency to approach the 252 rewarded and ‘aversive’ goal pots, they were tested for three days during which 253 subjects were exposed to each of the three ambiguous maze arms once per day, 12 254 interspersed within a sequence of exposures to the rewarded and ‘aversive’ 255 reference maze arms. The testing schedule consisted of 13 trials in total, with 256 five rewarded trials, five ‘aversive’ trials, and the three ambiguous locations (one 257 trial each). The three ambiguous trials were positioned at trial 5, trial 9 and trial 258 13, and the order in which they were presented was counterbalanced over the 259 three test days. The overall sequence consisted of alternated single rewarded and 260 ‘aversive’ trials, starting either with a rewarded trial or an ‘aversive’ trial, 261 counterbalanced between treatments. This testing schedule/sequence was 262 designed so that there were equal numbers of ambiguous trials preceded by 263 rewarded trials as there were preceded by ‘aversive’ trials, and that this was the 264 same for all treatments. 265 266 The goal pots at the end of the ambiguous maze arms contained no food pellets, 267 and so we only carried out three trials for each of the ambiguous maze arms in 268 order to minimise the opportunity for associations to develop. Latency to reach 269 the goal pot was recorded in all ambiguous trials. 270 271 Data analysis 272 Unless specifically mentioned in the text, all data met the requirements for 273 parametric tests (e.g. normality, homogeneity of variance etc.,) either in an 274 untransformed or transformed state. The statistics package used was SPSS 275 version 14. This study was carried out in two replicates separated by a two day 276 interval, with treatment groups equally balanced across the replicates. The 277 training data were analysed separately for both replicates, since this analysis was 278 conducted during training to determine when the rats were ready for testing. 13 279 Testing, however, was analysed with both replicates combined. For training, the 280 data for high and low level lighting were compared separately (i.e. as Treatments 281 H or L) because at this point the rats had only experienced either high or low 282 light levels. However, for testing, Treatment was modelled with four levels (i.e. 283 as Treatments HH, HL, LH, LL) so that the results incorporated the complete 284 experience of each rat, in terms of the light levels to which they had been 285 exposed, taken together over the course of both training and testing. 286 287 288 Results 289 290 Habituation 291 Only during the first habituation trial were enough faeces produced to be able to 292 carry out a statistical analysis. Using a one-way ANOVA, we found that there 293 was no difference between the treatments (high vs. low light level) in the number 294 of faecal boli produced during this first habituation trial (F3, 20=0.237, P>0.1). All 295 the rats ate all of the food pellets in each of the three habituation trials. The rats, 296 regardless of treatment, showed an increase over the three habituation trials in 297 the number of visits made to the maze arms (F2, 40=3.566, P=0.038), with post- 298 hoc tests indicating that significantly more visits to maze arms were made in the 299 third trial than in the previous two. There were no significant differences 300 between treatments nor a significant interaction between treatment and trial 301 (P>0.1) for maze arm visits. 302 303 Training 14 304 For the training analysis we calculated the average latency to access the goal pot 305 on the six rewarded trials and on the six ‘aversive’ trials for each rat/day, with 306 the exception of the first day of training in which the open-ended first trial (to the 307 rewarded location) and the first trial to the ‘aversive’ location were both 308 excluded. This was because for both these trials the reward outcome was 309 unknown to the subjects as it was their first experience of either location. One rat 310 from the HL treatment (in the second replicate) was removed from the 311 experiment because it failed to eat from the rewarded location following 312 exposure to the ‘aversive’ food. We continued to train the rats until they showed 313 a clear difference in their average latency to reach the rewarded and ‘aversive’ 314 locations, and for both replicates this was clearly observed after the second day 315 of training (see Figure Two). We used a repeated measures General Linear 316 Model (GLM) with Treatment (high light level vs. low light level) as a between 317 subject factor, and Location (rewarded vs. ‘aversive’) and Day (1&2) as within 318 subjects factors. For both replicates (analysed separately) we observed a 319 significant Day*Location interaction (Replicate One: F1, 10=7.48, P=0.021; 320 Replicate Two: F1, 9=9.48, P=0.013), but no significant differences in the time 321 taken to reach the goal pot between the treatments, either as a main effect or 322 interaction (P>0.1). 323 324 Post-hoc analysis of the Day*Location interactions revealed an increasing 325 difference in the latency to approach the two locations over time, predominantly 326 due to the increased speed in running to the rewarded location over the two days, 327 and with more variation in the latency to approach the aversive location (paired t- 328 tests: Replicate One: D1 rewarded vs. D1 ‘aversive’ t11=-2.56, P=0.026; D2 15 329 rewarded vs. D2 ‘aversive’ t11=-4.67, P=0.001; D1 rewarded vs. D2 rewarded 330 t11=5.46, P<0.001; D1 ‘aversive’ vs. D2 ‘aversive’ t11=0.11, P>0.1; Replicate 331 Two: D1 rewarded vs. D1 ‘aversive’ t10=0.64, P>0.1; D2 rewarded vs. D2 332 ‘aversive’ t10=-2.38, P=0.038; D1 rewarded vs. D2 rewarded t10=5.89, P<0.001; 333 D1 ‘aversive’ vs. D2 ‘aversive’ t10=0.51, P>0.1). Testing was therefore 334 implemented after the second day of training. 335 336 Figure Two 337 338 Testing 339 Testing was carried out over three days for each rat, with five rewarded trials, 340 five ‘aversive’ trials, and one trial for each of the three ‘probe’ locations per day. 341 For each rat, we calculated the average latency for the 15 rewarded trials, the 342 average latency for the 15 ‘aversive’ trials, and the average latency over the three 343 trials for each of the three different ‘probe’ locations (see Figure Three). Probe 344 and reference locations were analysed separately because of the difference 345 between them in the number of trials used to calculate the means. 346 347 Figure Three 348 349 We used a repeated measures GLM to compare the latency to approach the two 350 ‘reference’ locations (rewarded and ‘aversive’) to determine whether the subjects 351 still accurately discriminated between the locations during testing, and whether 352 or not there was any treatment effect. Data were log-transformed, and we 353 included Treatment (HH, HL, LH, LL, between subjects) and Location (rewarded 16 354 vs. ‘aversive’, within subjects) as factors. As expected, we found a highly 355 significant effect of location (F1, 19=63.32, P<0.001), with subjects taking 356 significantly longer to approach the ‘aversive’ location than the rewarded 357 location. There was no overall effect of Treatment nor any interactions (P>0.1). 358 359 Our next analysis was to determine whether or not the animals responded 360 differently to the probe locations during testing, and whether this response 361 differed between the four treatments. In order to take into account individual 362 differences in performance (i.e. the latency to approach the reference locations), 363 we calculated the average value between the time taken to reach the rewarded 364 and aversive locations during testing for each rat, and included this as a covariate 365 in the analysis [e.g. 8]. We found a significant Treatment effect (F3,18=3.228, 366 P=0.047), indicating that there was a difference between the treatments in the 367 latency to approach the probe locations. This did not depend on the specific 368 probe location (Probe*Treatment interaction (F6,36=0.357, P=0.901)), and there 369 was no main effect of Probe location (repeated measures GLM: F2,36=0.48, 370 P=0.623). Post-hoc pairwise investigation of the overall Treatment effect using 371 Bonferroni adjustment revealed that rats in the HL treatment ran significantly 372 faster to the probe locations (P=0.043) than rats in the LH treatment (see Figure 373 Four). There were no further significant differences between the treatments 374 (P>0.1). 375 376 Figure Four 377 378 Discussion 17 379 380 Habituation 381 The rats quickly habituated to the experimental apparatus, as demonstrated by 382 their increased investigation of the maze arms over time. The fact that all the 383 food pellets were eaten in each of the habituation trials, together with the low 384 levels of defaecation, suggests that the apparatus did not induce high levels of 385 anxiety during this habituation period. The lack of a treatment difference (high 386 light level vs. low light level) at this stage suggests that this initial contrast in 387 light levels did not affect the behaviours measured during habituation. 388 389 Training 390 The results demonstrated that after two days, the rats were able to discriminate 391 between the rewarded and ‘aversive’ locations based on the time taken to 392 approach the goal pot, with an increasing difference across training days as a 393 consequence of the rats speeding up their approach to the rewarded location. This 394 confirms the findings of a previous study in which we used location as the basis 395 for a spatial judgement task [8]. In the current study, our new technique reduced 396 the time required to learn the discrimination (from six to two days). As 397 mentioned earlier (see ‘Apparatus’), the design of the current study differed in a 398 number of aspects to our previous study [8]. For example, in Burman et al., [8] 399 the stimuli were either rewarded or unrewarded, whereas in the current study we 400 used rewarded versus ‘aversive’ stimuli. These modifications appeared to be 401 reflected in faster learning speeds. The difference in reward contingency may 402 also explain differences in the ‘pattern’ of discrimination learning between the 403 two experiments. In Burman et al., [8] the rats initially ran quickly to both 18 404 locations, before slowing their approach to the unrewarded location (a gradual 405 recognition of the absence of a food reward), whereas in the current study the 406 rats showed an initially slow approach to both locations before increasing their 407 approach to the ‘rewarded’ location (a gradual recognition of the presence of a 408 non-aversive food reward). 409 410 There was no difference in training performance between the two 411 treatments (high light level vs. low light level) suggesting that there was no 412 difference in either the level of food motivation, learning ability or general 413 locomotor activity as a consequence of being exposed to the different light 414 levels. Any differences between the treatments during testing were therefore 415 unlikely to be due to alterations in motivational state or general activity. In terms 416 of visual perception, it also does not appear that the task was either more or less 417 difficult to perform in the two different light levels. There was also little 418 difference between the two replicates in the time taken to learn the task, and both 419 replicates showed a similar learning pattern (see above). 420 421 Testing 422 During testing the subject rats continued to discriminate accurately between the 423 rewarded and ‘aversive’ reference locations, and this ability was shown not to be 424 influenced by treatment. This presents further evidence, additional to that 425 gathered during training, that any treatment effects on the subjects’ response to 426 the ambiguous probe locations was less likely to have arisen as a consequence of 427 general changes in motivation rather than due to a specific influence on the 428 appraisal of the ambiguous probe locations. This includes any motivational 19 429 influences arising specifically due to the change in light levels which occurred 430 for half the rats between training and testing (in comparison to the unchanged 431 light levels received by all subjects during training). 432 433 In line with our initial predictions, we found that rats tested under high light 434 conditions after having previously experienced the apparatus under low light 435 conditions (LH) and those tested under low light conditions after previously 436 experiencing the apparatus under high light conditions (HL) differed most 437 strongly in their judgement of the ambiguous stimuli, with HL rats running faster 438 to all probe locations. However, in contrast to our predictions, those animals 439 trained and tested in high light levels (HH) did not show a more negative 440 judgment of the ambiguous stimuli than those trained and tested in low light 441 levels (LL). 442 443 The difference between HL and LH rats indicates that a change in light 444 conditions per se resulting in a performance decrement (see Methods 445 ‘Treatments’) was unlikely to be responsible for the findings, but rather that a 446 specific difference in emotional state was a more likely explanation. Being 447 switched from a high light to a low light level likely resulted in a relative 448 decrease in anxiety compared to those animals switched from a low to a high 449 light level, and this may have resulted in the less negative (or more positive) 450 judgement of the ambiguous stimuli. Certainly, it appears that exposure to 451 sudden darkness can result in a reduced state of anxiety in rats [e.g. 19], although 452 sudden darkness is a more extreme change in light levels than was used in the 453 current study. It is possible that a form of relief was experienced by those 20 454 animals in the HL treatment following the omission of a more anxiety-inducing 455 light level [e.g. 26]. The observed results could also be explained in a similar 456 way to incentive contrast theory [e.g. 27], where emotional state and subsequent 457 behaviour is determined by relative (and contrasting), rather than absolute, 458 experience. If this were the case, then experiencing a change in light levels is 459 likely to have, at least temporarily, more of an influence on emotional state than 460 exposure to unchanging illumination. Changes in motivation or general activity 461 due to the change in light levels could possibly account for the results observed 462 but, if this was the explanation, we would also have expected to see differences 463 in responses of HL and LH rats to the unambiguous training locations and, as 464 discussed above, these were not observed. 465 466 It is worth taking time to consider why we did not observe the predicted 467 difference between the HH and LL treatments in their judgement of the 468 ambiguous locations. One possibility is that, on their own, the HH and LL 469 treatments were not strong enough to induce a measurable difference in 470 emotional state, as tentatively demonstrated by the lack of difference during 471 habituation, and only became strong enough once a contrast was established by 472 changing the light levels at testing. A further possibility is that those rats tested in 473 the unchanging light conditions (HH & LL) had become habituated to the anxiety 474 inducing effect of the light level over the course of training (two days) thereby 475 ‘normalizing’ their subsequent response at testing, whereas those rats that 476 changed light levels following training (HL & LH) had a ‘refreshed’ reaction to a 477 light level that they only experienced for the first time at testing. 478 21 479 Our present finding extends previous research [e.g. 4,5-9] into the use of 480 cognitive bias to assess animal emotions, demonstrating that acute/short-term 481 inductions of anxiety can generate an apparent bias in cognitive processing, in 482 terms of judgements of ambiguity. It thus appears that such cognitive bias tasks 483 may be able to identify different, but similarly valenced, emotional states, as 484 proposed by Paul et al. [3]. This would help advance understanding of the 485 affective (emotional) mechanisms underpinning cognitive bias, as well as 486 methods for the assessment of animal emotion and welfare. 487 488 Unlike other studies [e.g. 4,8], we found no difference between treatments in 489 their judgement of specific ambiguous probe locations (e.g. either the probe 490 nearest the ‘negative’ end or the probe nearest the ‘positive’ end), but instead 491 found an overall difference inclusive of all three probe locations. One possible 492 reason for this is that the probe locations in this study were more easily 493 distinguishable to the rats than in a previous study [8] because they were 494 positioned further apart from one another and from the reference locations (45˚ 495 rather than 15˚). It could, therefore, have been that all three probe locations were 496 viewed similarly, as being of ambiguous outcome (i.e. they might contain either 497 palatable or aversive food), rather than being of ambiguous location (i.e. they 498 were not confused with the actual reference locations). 499 500 501 Conclusion 502 To conclude, we observed a treatment difference in the judgement of three 503 ambiguous locations in a judgement bias task, with rats switched from high 22 504 (60W) to low (10W) light levels displaying a more positive judgement of 505 ambiguous locations compared to rats switched from low to high light levels. 506 This result suggests that the judgement bias technique might be useful as an 507 indicator of acute changes in anxiety and other emotional states, as well as 508 allowing differentiation between acute and more chronic negative emotional 509 states such as depression. Finally, as a quickly learned task, it has the potential 510 benefit of being adaptable for a wide range of animal species. 511 512 513 Acknowledgements 514 515 This work was funded by the BBSRC Animal Welfare Initiative. 516 517 23 518 Figure One: 519 520 521 Visual cue Guillotine door 522 523 524 Rewarded location Probe location ‘Aversive’ location 525 526 527 528 529 24 530 Figure Two: 531 532 Latency to approach (s) 25.00 20.00 Rewarded R1 15.00 Aversive R1 Rewarded R2 10.00 Aversive R2 5.00 0.00 1 Training Day 533 534 535 25 2 536 Figure Three: 537 Latency to approach (s) 538 16.00 *** 14.00 12.00 10.00 8.00 6.00 4.00 2.00 0.00 Aversive Probe nearest aversive Middle probe Location 539 540 541 26 Probe nearest rewarded Rewarded 542 Figure Four: 543 544 * 545 546 547 548 549 27 550 551 Figure One: A diagram of the experimental apparatus, showing the two reference 552 locations (either rewarded or aversive), the three ambiguous probe locations, the 553 guillotine doors and the position of the visual cue. 554 555 Figure Two: A graph displaying the subjects’ ability to discriminate between the 556 rewarded and aversive locations over the two days of training, as determined by 557 differences in latency to approach (seconds). Data are presented for each of the 558 two replicates (R1 & R2), and averaged for treatment. Data are means ± standard 559 error of the mean. 560 561 Figure Three: A graph displaying the latency for the subjects to approach 562 (seconds) the five different locations (either reference or probe locations) during 563 testing (averaged for the three test days) and averaged for treatment. Data are 564 combined for the two replicates. Data are means ± standard error of the mean. 565 ***P<0.001 566 567 Figure Four: A graph displaying the difference between the treatments (HH, HL, 568 LH & LL) in the latency to approach (seconds) the ambiguous probe locations 569 (averaged for the three probe locations) during testing (averaged for the three test 570 days). Data are combined for the two replicates. Data are presented as estimated 571 marginal means from the GLM output in order to account for the use of a 572 covariate. *P<0.05 573 574 28 575 References 576 1. Eysenck, M. W.; Mogg, K.; May, J.; Richards, A.; Mathews, A. Bias in 577 interpretation of ambiguous sentences related to threat in anxiety. J. Abnorm. 578 Psychol. 1991, 100: 144-150. 579 580 2. Mogg, K.; Bradley, B. P. A cognitive-motivational analysis of anxiety. Behav. 581 Res. Ther. 1998, 36: 809-848. 582 583 3. Paul, E. S.; Harding, E. J.; Mendl, M. Measuring emotional processes in 584 animals: The utility of a cognitive approach. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2005, 29: 585 469-491. 586 587 4. Harding, E. J.; Paul, E. S.; Mendl, M. Animal behavior - cognitive bias and 588 affective state. Nature. 2004, 427: 312-312. 589 590 5. Bateson, M.; Matheson, S. M. Performance on a categorisation task suggests 591 that removal of environmental enrichment induces 'pessimism' in captive 592 european starlings (sturnus vulgaris). Anim. Welf. 2007, 16: 33-36. 593 594 6. Matheson, S. M.; Asher, L.; Bateson, M. Larger, enriched cages are associated 595 with 'optimistic' response biases in captive european starlings (sturnus vulgaris). 596 Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2007, 109: 374-383. 597 598 7. Burman, O.; Parker, R.; Paul, E.; Mendl, M. Sensitivity to reward loss as an 599 indicator of animal affect and welfare. Biology Letters. 2008, 4: 330-333. 29 600 601 8. Burman, O.; Parker, R.; Paul, E.; Mendl, M. A spatial judgement task to 602 determine background emotional state in laboratory rats (rattus norvegicus). 603 Anim. Behav. 2008, 76: 801-809. 604 605 9. Mendl, M.; Burman, O.; Parker, R.; Paul, E. Cognitive bias as an indicator of 606 animal emotion and welfare: Emerging evidence and underlying mechanisms. 607 Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. In press. 608 609 10. Harkin, A.; Houlihan, D. D.; Kelly, J. P. Reduction in preference for 610 saccharin by repeated unpredictable stress in mice and its prevention by 611 imipramine. J. Psychopharmacol. (Oxf). 2002, 16: 115-123. 612 613 11. Beck, A. T.; Rush, A. J.; Shaw, B. F.; Emery, G. Cognitive therapy of 614 depression: A treatment manual. New York: Guilford Press; 1979. 615 616 12. Blanchette, I.; Richards, A.; Cross, A. Anxiety and the interpretation of 617 ambiguous facial expressions: The influence of contextual cues. Q. J. Exp. 618 Psychol. 2007, 60: 1101-1115. 619 620 13. Valle, F. P. Effects of strain, sex, and illumination on open-field behavior of 621 rats. Am. J. Psychol. 1970, 83: 103-&. 622 30 623 14. Cosquer, B.; Kuster, N.; Cassel, J. C. Whole-body exposure to 2.45 ghz 624 electromagnetic fields does not alter 12-arm radial-maze with reduced access to 625 spatial cues in rats. Behav. Brain Res. 2005, 161: 331-334. 626 627 15. Garcia, A. M. B.; Cardenas, F. P.; Morato, S. Effect of different illumination 628 levels on rat behavior in the elevated plus-maze. Physiol. Behav. 2005, 85: 265- 629 270. 630 631 16. Walker, D. L.; Davis, M. Anxiogenic effects of high illumination levels 632 assessed with the acoustic startle response in rats. Biol. Psychiatry. 1997, 42: 633 461-471. 634 635 17. Grillon, C.; Pellowski, M.; Merikangas, K. R.; Davis, M. Darkness facilitates 636 the acoustic startle reflex in humans. Biol. Psychiatry. 1997, 42: 453-460. 637 638 18. File, S. E.; Seth, P. A review of 25 years of the social interaction test. Eur. J. 639 Pharmacol. 2003, 463: 35-53. 640 641 19. Nasello, A. G.; Machado, C.; Bastos, J. F.; Felicio, L. F. Sudden darkness 642 induces a high activity low anxiety state in male and female rats. Physiol. Behav. 643 1998, 63: 451-454. 644 645 20. Nasello, A. G.; Sassatani, A. S.; Ferreira, F. S.; Felicio, L. F.; Tieppo, C. A. 646 Modulation by sudden darkness of apomorphine-induced behavioral responses. 647 Physiol. Behav. 2003, 78: 521-528. 31 648 649 21. Bert, B.; Felicio, L. F.; Fink, H.; Nasello, A. G. The use of sudden darkness 650 in mice: A behavioural and pharmacological approach. Psychopharmacology 651 (Berl). 2005, 179: 846-853. 652 653 22. MacLeod, A. K.; Tata, P.; Kentish, J.; Jacobsen, H. Retrospective and 654 prospective cognitions in anxiety and depression. Cognition & Emotion. 1997, 655 11: 467-479. 656 657 23. Hale, M. W.; Bouwknecht, J. A.; Spiga, F.; Shekhar, A.; Lowry, C. A. 658 Exposure to high- and low-light conditions in an open-field test of anxiety 659 increases c-fos expression in specific subdivisions of the rat basolateral 660 amygdaloid complex. Brain Res. Bull. 2006, 71: 174-182. 661 662 24. Burman, O. H. P.; Mendl, M. The effects of environmental context on 663 laboratory rat social recognition. Anim. Behav. 1999, 58: 629-634. 664 665 25. Burman, O. H. P.; Mendl, M. Recognition of conspecific odors by laboratory 666 rats (rattus norvegicus) does not show context specificity. J. Comp. Psychol. 667 2002, 116: 247-252. 668 669 26. Rolls, E. T. Emotion explained. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2005. 670 671 27. Flaherty, C. Incentive relativity. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 672 1996. 32 673 33