1 Supplemental Materials “Child Care and Cortisol Across Early Childhood: Context Matters,” by D. Berry et al., 2013, Developmental Psychology. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0033379 Measurement Details Child-Care Quality We fitted a longitudinal confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) model across the four measurement points. The residual variances for common indicators were freely estimated over time, as were the covariances between the latent factors. The model fit the data well (χ2 = 89.41, df = 73, p =.09; CFI = .96, RMSEA = .02). All the factor loadings were statistically significant and in the expected direction, with the standardized loadings ranging between .54 and .85. Each latent variance was statistically significant, and rank-order stability over time was moderate; the standardized covariances between the factors over time ranged between .38 and .54. We aggregated these data by estimating the factor scores at each point, standardizing, and averaging across time points. Cumulative Risk When the target child was 7, 15, 24, and 36 months of age, family income-to-needs ratio was measured by taking reported total household income and dividing by the federal poverty threshold for family size for the given year. Rank-order stability in family income was strong over time (r = .73 to .79, p < .001). Household caregiver education was based on the highest level of education attained by either the primary or secondary caregiver (when available) reported at 7, 15, 24, and 36 months and was scaled as raw years of schooling. Rank-order stability over this period was strong (r = .90 to .98, p < .001). Across this same period, primary caregivers were considered to have a partner, if (a) they were married with the spouse living in the home or (b) they designated a romantic partner or relative (e.g., grandparent of target child) 2 as living in the home and helping with the child. Rank-order stability in partnering between adjacent epoch was also quite strong (r = .77 to .92, p < .001). Average weekly hours of work were based on primary caregiver reports of her and the secondary caregiver’s (if present) weekly work hours at 7, 15, 24, and 36 months, and showed moderate rank-order stability between 7 and 36 months (r = .41 to .49, p < .001). Occupational prestige was scored using the National Opinion Research Center (NORC) coding system (Nakao & Treas, 1994). Higher prestige jobs are scaled as larger numbers. There was substantial rankorder stability in prestige levels across the four measurement periods (r = .76 to .80, p < .001). Household density was calculated based on caregiver reports regarding the number of individuals living in the home, divided by independent observer ratings of the number of rooms in the household. Larger numbers reflect more densely populated households. Independent observers also rated neighborhood noise and neighborhood safety during these home visits. Safety was scored on a 1 (“obviously dangerous”) to 4 (“above average safety”) Likert-type scale. Noise was reverse scored as 1 (“very loud”) to 4 (“very quiet”). Neighborhood safety and noise showed strong positive correlations (r = .51 to .61, p < .001) and were subsequently averaged. Rank-order stability in both household density and neighborhood noise/safety over time ranged from moderate to strong (r = .43 to .82, .47 to .80, p < .001, respectively). Finally, primary caregiver sensitivity was based on observer ratings of positive parenting during a semi-structured play task at 7, 15, 24, and 36 months of age (see Cox, Paley, Burchinal, & Payne, 1999). In this task, the primary caregiver and infant were presented with a set of developmentally appropriate toys, and caregivers were asked to play with their infants as they would normally. Primary caregiver behavior was recorded and subsequently coded for levels of sensitivity, detachment, intrusiveness, positive regard, negative regard, and animation during their interactions with the child. Each was rated on a 5-point Likert-type scale (1 = not at all characteristic, 5 = highly characteristic). Informed by prior work with these data (e.g., Blair et al., 2011a), we created a positive parenting composite at each time point by taking the average of the primary caregiver’s ratings across the sensitivity, animation, positive regard, and detachment (reversed). Two independent raters rated approximately 30% of the caregiver-child interactions; the intraclass correlations for the positive parenting composite ranged between .87 and .91. 3 Rank-order stability in positive parenting was rather strong across the four measurement periods (r = .55 to .65, p < .001). The cumulative risk index was calculated by first averaging scores within a given risk over time. As shown in Table S1, each of the cumulative risk factors showed moderate to rather strong correlations with each other, such that higher levels of risk (i.e., lower levels positively framed variables) were associated with higher risk in the other variables. We then conducted a principal components analysis (PCA) across the eight items. There was clear evidence that the data were best represented by a single component; only one component showed an eigenvalue greater than 1 (i.e., 3.62) and rose substantially above the scree, accounting for 45% of the variability across the items. The PCA loadings were in the expected direction, and ranged from .52 (household density) to .83 (family income). Internal-consistency reliability across the items was reasonable ( = .82). We created a continuous cumulative risk index by reverse-scoring the positively framed variables, standardizing each risk measure, and averaging across the risks. Control Covariates Infant traits. Child gender and race were dummy coded, such that male and African American children were each coded as 1. Temperament. Infant temperament was measured using the estimated factor scores from a CFA-based composite measure comprising three measures (four scales) of behavioral reactivity at 7 months of age: (a) observer ratings of infant behavior during a home visit, using the Irritability scale of an adapted version of the Infant Behavior Record (IBR; Bayley, 1969; Stifter & Corey, 2001); (b) observer ratings of infant reactivity during three moderately stressful tasks (see Blair et al., 2008); and (c) primary caregiver reports of infant behavior across the Distress to Novelty and Distress to Limitations scales of the Infant Behavior Questionnaire (Rothbart, 1981). More detailed descriptions of the model are available from the first author. Mental development. Infant mental development at 7 months of age was measured using the Mental Development Index (MDI) scores on the Bayley Scales of Mental Development (Bayley, 1969). Higher scores represent more developmentally advanced abilities. 4 Caregiver/family traits. When the child was 2 months of age, primary-caregiver reading ability was measured using the KFAST literacy screener (Kaufman & Kaufman, 1994), a standardized measure of functional literacy. Higher scores indicate better reading skills. Caregiver age. The primary caregiver’s age at the birth of the target child as well as her age when she had her first child were included as covariates. Each was scaled in years. Subsidy receipt. Dummy variables representing whether the primary caregiver received Aid to Families with Dependent Children (AFDC) funding or Medicaid funding were also included in the model. For each, receipt of funding is coded as 1. Research site. The NC site was dummy coded as 1. Measurement traits. Time-of-day of the saliva collection was coded on a 24-hour clock at both 7 and 48 months of age. Time since sleep/nap, average amount of sleep per night, and amount of sleep the night prior to the saliva collection were each controlled and scaled in hours. 5 Table S1 Zero-Order Correlations and Factor Loadings From Principal Components Analysis of Variables Included in the Cumulative Risk Index Variable Family income Education Work hr Occ. prestige Constant partner Density Safety Pos. parent Income Education Work hr Occ. prestige Partner — .56** .57** .56** .50** — .42** .58** .41** — .32** .40** — .36** — .41 .47** .48** .39** .39** .51** .24** .32** .37** .28** .39** .40** .16** .35** .48** Density p < .10. PCA loading .833 .782 .645 .725 .661 — .25** .31** Note. Variables are on original scales, prior to being reflected to represent risk. ** Safety — .33** .524 .624 .712