Empirical Properties of a Scale to Assess Writing SelfEfficacy in School Contexts Pajares, Frank.Measurement and Evaluation in Counseling and Development 39. 4 (Jan 2007): 239-249. Turn on hit highlighting for speaking browsers Hide highlighting Abstract (summary) Translate AbstractTranslate Press the Escape key to closeTranslate The psychometric properties of a scale assessing the writing self-efficacy of 1,258 students from Grades 4 to 11 were analyzed with exploratory factor analysis. Two factors emerged, I designating basic grammar skills and I designating more advanced composition skills. The Writing Self-Efficacy Scale (F. Pajares & G. Valiante, 1999) functioned equally well at each academic level assessed. [PUBLICATION ABSTRACT] Full Text Translate Full textTranslate Press the Escape key to closeTranslate Turn on search term navigationTurn on search term navigation Headnote The psychometric properties of a scale assessing the writing self-efficacy of 1,258 students from Grades 4 to 11 were analyzed with exploratory factor analysis. Two factors emerged, I designating basic grammar skills and I designating more advanced composition skills. The Writing Self-Efficacy Scale (F. Pajares & G. Valiante, 1999) functioned equally well at each academic level assessed. Researchers in the area of writing have traditionally focused either on the skills that writers bring to this craft or on the instructional practices teachers use to improve their students' composition skills (e.g., Faigley, 1990; Hairston, 1990). Primarily, these efforts have been aimed at understanding the cognitive processes underlying students' compositions (see Hayes, 2006). More recently, however, researchers have pointed out that the relationship between students' cognitive skills and the manner in which they engage text is mediated by the interpretations students make about the skills they possess (Hidi & Boscolo, 2006; Pajares & Valiante, 2006). To account for this phenomenon, investigators have explored the self-beliefs that underlie student motivation in writing. Findings from this avenue of inquiry have led researchers to suggest that students' beliefs about their own writing competence, or self-efficacy beliefs, are instrumental to their ultimate success as writers (see Pajares, 2003). As a consequence, a number of researchers have explored the relationship between students' self-efficacy beliefs about writing, other motivation variables related to writing, and various writing outcomes. Research findings have consistently shown that writing self-efficacy beliefs and writing performances are related. Most early self-efficacy studies were conducted on college students, and effect sizes for the influence of writing self-efficacy on writing performance ranged from beta = .32 to beta = .42 (e.g., McCarthy, Meier, & Rinderer, 1985; Meier, McCarthy, & Schmeck, 1984). Another consistent finding was that neither writing apprehension nor other motivation variables were typically predictive of writing performance in regression models that included self-efficacy. More recent findings support these results (Bruning & Horn, 2000; Pajares, 2003; Pajares & Valiante, 1997, 1999, 2001, 2006; Rankin, Bruning, & Timme, 1994; Wachholz & Etheridge, 1996; Zimmerman & Bandura, 1994). In general, results reveal that writing self-efficacy makes an independent contribution to the prediction of writing outcomes and mediates between previous and subsequent achievement in writing. This is the case even when powerful covariates such as preassessed writing ability or previous writing performance are included in statistical models. Standardized regression coefficients and direct effects between writing self-efficacy and writing outcomes in multiple regression and path analyses that control for such preperformance assessments have ranged from .19 to .40. Correlations between writing self-efficacy and writing performances typically range from .30 to .50, often depending on the age and competence of the students. Writing self-efficacy is also associated with motivation constructs that are themselves related to writing performances. These concepts have been prominent in self-efficacy studies and have included writing self-concept (see Grain. 1996; Marsh, 1989), writing apprehension (Pajares & Valiante. 1997, 1999, 2001), self-efficacy for self-regulation (Harris & Graham, 1992; Zimmerman & Bandura, 1994), students' perceived value of writing (Shell, Colvin, & Burning, 1995; Shell, Murphy, & Bruning, 1989), and achievement goal orientations (Pajares, Britner, & Valiante, 2000). The relationship between gender and self-efficacy has been a focus of research in the area of writing, and findings have not been consistent. Some researchers have found no gender differences in writing self-efficacy across academic grades (Shell et al., 1995). Others have found that girls report stronger confidence in their writing capabilities than do boys, at least through middle school (Pajares. Miller, & Johnson, 1999; Pajares & Valiante, 2001). These differences may begin at early ages (Grain, 1996). Researchers have observed that girls experience a drop in their academic motivation and perceptions of competence as they reach high school (Bruning & Horn, 2000), perhaps because they begin to encounter classroom structures that emphasize a masculine form of discourse (Cleary, 1996). Some findings have shed light on the development of writing self-efficacy beliefs as a function of schooling. Shell et al. (1995) assessed the writing self-efficacy of students in Grades 4, 7, and 10 and found no differences in their confidence that they possessed various grammar, usage, and composition skills. Again, because older students are in better possession of those skills, one wonders why confidence in skills does not increase proportionately. Other researchers have reported that students in the 1st year of middle school have stronger confidence in their writing skills than do students in subsequent middle school grades even though older students have greater writing competence (Pajares & Valiante, 1999). This pattern of decreasing confidence in language arts skills is consistent with findings from expectancy-value researchers who have reported that students' self-concepts of ability in English decrease from the start of Grade 6 to the end of Grade 7 (Wigfield, Eccles, MacIver, Reuman, & Midgley, 1991). ASSESSING WRITING SELF-EFFICACY Bandura (2006) has provided clear guidelines regarding how self-efficacy beliefs should be operationalized and measured. Because efficacy beliefs vary in level, strength, and generality, these dimensions are important in determining how instruments should be constructed. Imagine that a researcher is interested in assessing the essay-writing selfefficacy of middle school students and wishes to compare that with students' capability to write an essay, in any given domain, there are different levels of task demands that researchers may tap. In writing an essay, these can range from the lower level of writing a simple sentence with proper punctuation to the higher level of organizing sentences into a paragraph so as to clearly express an idea. Students are asked to rate the strength of their belief in their capability to perform each of the levels identified. Hence, a self-efficacy scale should provide multiple items of varying difficulty that collectively assess the domain of essay writing. In addition, items should be prototypical of essay writing rather than minutely specific features of writing (e.g., confidence to form letters), and they should be worded in terms of can, a judgment of capability, rather than ïé will, a statement of intention. The Writing Self-Efficacy Scale (WSES; Pajares & Valiante. 1999) has been much used in studies of writing self-efficacy. Items on this scale ask students to provide judgments of their confidence in their ability to successfully perform grammar, usage, composition, and mechanical writing skills, such as correctly punctuating a one-page passage or organizing sentences into a paragraph so as to clearly express a theme (see Shell et al., 1995, and Shell et al., 1989, for an eight-item self-efficacy scale using similar items). In the WSES, students provide their judgments by filling in any number from O to 100 as a measure of their self-efficacy for each skill or task. Pajares, Hartley, and Valiante (2001) found that a writing skills self-efficacy scale with a 0-100 response format was psychometrically stronger than one with a traditional Likert format. In analyses predicting middle school students' grade point average (GPA) in language arts and teacher ratings of their students' writing competence, the 0-100 scale predicted both outcomes, whereas the Likert-scale assessment did not. Also, 37% of the variance in GPA and 28% of the variance in teacher ratings was associated uniquely with the 0-100 scale, whereas the variance associated uniquely with the Likert scale was negligible for each dependent variable. This is consistent with Bandura's (1997) caution that "including too few steps loses differentiating information because people who use the same response category would differ if intermediate steps were included" (p. 44). Because neither a Likert-type scale nor a 0-100 scale is more difficult or longer than the other, using a format that adds predictive utility is especially warranted. Although the WSES scores have been found to have acceptable stability and internal consistency in prior samples, its factor structure has not been examined. Determining the sources of variance underlying individual differences on an instrument is a necessary step in test validation. Therefore, a factor analysis is required to substantiate the results from the various studies that have made use of this scale. The purpose of this study was to conduct a factor analysis of the WSES to determine the homogeneity of its item pool. Because the scale has been used with students in elementary, middle, and high school, analyses were conducted at each of the three academic levels. Because this is the first attempt to examine the latent correlational structure of the WSES, an exploratory factor analysis was conducted. To provide construct validation, the relationship between scores on the WSES and scores from scales of writing motivation often used in studies of writing was also examined. These were writing self-concept, self-efficacy for self-regulated learning, achievement goal orientation in writing, writing apprehension, and value of writing. Teachers also provided ratings of their students' writing capability, and these were used as measures of writing achievement. METHOD Participants Participants were 1,258 students (633 girls, 625 boys) enrolled in Grades 4 to 11 and attending one public elementary school (Grades 4 and 5; n = 296 [24%]) in the South, one middle school (Grades 6, 7, and 8; n = 497 [39%]) in the Northeast, and one high school (Grades 9, 10, and 11; n = 465 [37%]) in the South. Students' ages ranged from 9 to 18. The socioeconomic status of the schools and of the area that the schools served was largely middle class, and students were primarily White. The sample consisted of only students in regular education classes; that is, gifted, special education, and English as a second language students were not included. The study took place during the second semester of the academic year. Instruments Each student completed a 10-page instrument tapping writing attitudes and motivation variables. Instruments were administered to groups in individual language arts classes during one period. Students completed the instrument one item at a time as that item was read aloud by the administrator. Instruments and variables used in the present study to assess the motivation variables have been used by researchers in numerous investigations of writing self-efficacy and motivation (e.g., Pajares et al., 1999; Pajares & Valiante, 1997, 1999, 2001; Shell et al., 1995; Shell et al., 1989; Zimmerman & Bandura. 1994). With the exception of the WSES, items asked students to provide judgments about how true or false they found a particular statement along a 6-point Likert-type continuum. Writing self-efficacy was operationalized as students'judgments of their capability to execute the various composition, grammar, usage, and mechanical skills required to write an effective essay appropriate to their academic level. The WSES consisted of 10 items asking students how sure they were that they could perform specific writing skills on a scale from 0 (no chance) to 100 (completely certain). Items previously used by Shell el al. (1989) served as a template for discussion with language arts teachers that resulted in the WSES (see Pajares & Valiante, 1999, 2001; Pajares et al., 2001). Note that the same items were presented to elementary school, middle school, and high school students. This is possible because, when assessing self-efficacy beliefs related to grammar and composition, items can be written so that they are equally appropriate across academic levels. For example, an item asking students to provide a judgment of confidence to "write simple sentences with good grammar" is as relevant to a student in Grade 4 as it is to a student in Grade 11. Language arts teachers across each of the levels attested to the fact that the self-efficacy items were appropriate for their students. Using a similar measure, Pajares and Valiante (1997) reported coefficient alpha reliability of .88 and above .68 correlations between items and scale scores on a sample of Grade 5 students; Pajares et al. (1999) reported a coefficient alpha of .85 with students in Grades 3, 4, and 5; and Pajares and Valiante (1999, 2001) reported a coefficient alpha of .92 with middle school students. To test the construct validity of the WSES, academic motivation scales that have been extensively used in studies of writing were selected. Achievement goal orientation in writing was assessed using a scale provided by Middleton and Midgley (1997), which was derived from the Patterns of Adaptive Learning Survey. The self-efficacy for self-regulated learning scale was adapted from Bandura's (2006) Children's Multidimensional Self-Efficacy Scales, which assess students' judgments of their capability to learn in various academic courses and to use various self-regulated learning strategies. Writing self-concept was assessed with six items adapted from Marsh's (1990) Academic Self-Description Questionnaire. Writing apprehension was measured with a scale used by Pajares et al. (1999). Finally, the scale measuring value of writing was composed of items assessing perceived importance, interest, and enjoyment of writing (see Seegers & Boekaerts, 1996). The motivation variables were all assessed in a Likert format. To be consistent with the manner in which the Likert version of one of the self-efficacy scales was assessed, students were provided with a response option consisting of 6 points on the Likert scale. Analysis An exploratory factor analysis was conducted to identify the latent constructs underlying the items on the WSES following factor analysis guidelines recommended by Fabrigar, Wegener, MacCallum, and Strahan (1999). The maximum likelihood method of factor extraction (Jöreskog & Lawley, 1968) was used because this method is believed to produce the best parameter estimates (Pedhazur, 1982). Multiple criteria were used for selecting the number of factors: (a) Cattell's (1966) scree test, (b) a parallel analysis technique that compares the eigenvalues of the reduced correlation matrices and those expected for random data, (c) the percentage of common variance explained by each factor using the weighted reduced correlation matrix, and (d) the interpretability of the rotated factors. Because the factors were expected to be intercorrelated, the oblique rotation was chosen. RESULTS Table 1 provides mean self-efficacy scores and standard deviations by school level and for the full sample. Boys and girls differed significantly at the middle school level, with girls reporting higher writing self-efficacy than boys (M = 78.1 for girls, M = 11.9 for boys; d = .35), and for the full sample (M= 78.5 for girls, M = 75.1 for boys; d = .21). Consistent with guidelines provided by Fabrigar et al. (1999), at least four measured variables were used for each common factor expected to emerge, and the samples met the minimal sizes recommended by MacCallum, Widaman, Zhang, and Hong (1999, p. 96) for adequate performance. To assess the tenability of the normality assumption required by the maximum likelihood method of extraction (Jöreskog & Lawley, 1968), two distributional properties of the items were used. Skewness and kurtosis of the items were (a) -1.25 and 1.67, respectively, for the full sample; (b) -1.44 and 2.72, respectively, for the elementary school sample; (c) -1.23 and 1.47, respectively, for the middle school sample; and (d) -1.12 and 1.26, respectively, for the high school sample. These estimates were well below the thresholds (skewness > 2, kurtosis > 7) for concerns about the adequacy of the estimation method (West, Finch, & Curran, 1995). For the elementary school analysis, the skewness and the kurtosis of the items were between -2.21 and -1.36, |M| = 1.78, and between 1.39 and 5.82, |M| = 3.34, respectively. For the middle school analysis, the skewness and the kurtosis of the items were between -1.75 and -0.95, |M| = 1.36, and between 0.23 and 2.95, |M| = 1.42, respectively. For the high school analysis, the skewness and the kurtosis of the items were between -1.72 and -0.82, |M| = 1.30, and between -1.77 and 2.71, |M| = 1.26, respectively. All the estimates except one did not exceed the thresholds (skewness > 2, kurtosis > 7) for concerns about the adequacy of the estimation method. The minimum skewness (-2.21) of the items in the elementary school analysis exceeded only slightly the threshold by a magnitude of 0.21. An examination of individual items showed that 2 out of the 10 items displayed skewness bordering the threshold. Given the minimal differences detected, the original metric was used, with no transformations of item scores. Each of the retention rules suggested a two-factor solution for the full sample and for each academic level (elementary school, middle school, high school). For the full sample, two eigenvalues from the weighted reduced correlation matrix (13.0 and 1.9) accounted for 99% of the variance; subsequent values were 0.25 or lower. Table 2 shows the standardized regression coefficients from the rotated pattern matrix, communality estimates, interfactor correlations, and proportion of variance explained for each factor. Coefficients from the pattern matrix show the relationship between an item and a factor when holding all other items constant. Coefficients of .40 or higher are typically considered strong enough to demonstrate that the item relates to the common factor (Jõreskog & Lawley, 1968). The items loading on Factor 1 (Items 1-5) showed standardized coefficients ranging from .64 to .78; those loading on Factor 2 (Items 6-10) had coefficients ranging from .47 to .86. None of the items cross-loaded, and interfactor correlation was .65. The Cronbach's alpha coefficient for the full-scale scores of the WSES was .91 (.88 for Factor 1 and .86 for Factor 2). Henson (2001) suggested that, for general research purposes, alpha values above .80 are acceptable for research purposes, whereas values above .90 are preferred if scores are to be used for clinical or educational decisions (see also Nunnally & Bernstein, 1994). Separate factor analyses were conducted for each of the subsamples of students by academic level (elementary school, middle school, high school). As was the case for the full sample, the retention rules were consistent in suggesting a two-factor solution at each level of schooling. To determine the number of common factors at each level of schooling, a parallel analysis (Montanelli & Humphreys, 1976) was also conducted. In this analysis, the eigenvalues of the reduced correlation matrices and those expected for random data were computed and compared using the principal axis/common factor analysis approach (see O'Connor, 2000). The principal axis/common factor analysis was chosen because it assumes an implicit underlying factor model by first estimating the communalities before identifying the factor solution (Sharma, 1996). For the elementary school analysis, 2 of the first 10 eigenvalues obtained from the random matrix (0.43, 0.31, 0.22, 0.15, 0.09, 0.03, -0.03, -0.08, -0.13, -0.18) were less than their counterparts estimated from the reduced correlation matrix (4.22, 0.52, 0.18, 0.04, -0.02, -0.04, -0.10, -0.11, -0.15, -0.22). For the middle school analysis, again 2 of the first 10 eigenvalues estimated from the random matrix (0.32, 0.23, 0.16, 0.11, 0.06, 0.02,-0.02,-0.06,-0.10,-0.15) were smaller than those from the reduced correlation matrix (5.38, 0.62, 0.14, 0.08, -0.03, -0.08, -0.10, 0.11, -0.12, -0.15). The same pattern was again detected in the high school analysis: 2 of the first 10 eigenvalues obtained from the random matrix (0.33, 0.24, 0.17, 0.12, 0.07, 0.02, -0.02, -0.07, -0.11, -0.15) were smaller than those from the reduced correlation matrix (5.04, 0.72, 0.22, 0.03, -0.02, -0.05, -0.05, -0.13, -0.13, -0.19). Results of all three analyses suggested two factors should be retained for each group. Table 2 shows the standardized regression coefficients from the rotated pattern matrix, communality estimates, interfactor correlations, and proportion of variance explained for the two-factor solutions for each of the three school levels. Note that the factors obtained from the elementary school students were reversed from those obtained from the middle school and high school students. At the elementary school level, the basic skills items (Items 1-5) accounted for a greater proportion of the variance. An examination of the items reveals that the items related to Factor 1 tapped grammar and usage skills (spelling, punctuation, parts of speech, grammar, singulars and plurals, verb tenses), whereas the items related to Factor 2 tapped more complex composition skills (constructing and structuring paragraphs and essays, using topic sentences, developing ideas). It is not surprising that the WSES items loaded on two factors or that, for middle and high school students, the composition items (Items 6-10) accounted for the greater proportion of variance (88% and 85%, respectively), whereas for the elementary school students, the basic skills items accounted for the greater proportion of variance (88%). As can be expected, the basic skills items had greater salience for the younger, less experienced students, whereas the composition items had greater salience for the older students who were more experienced in essay writing. The pattern loadings were also generally stronger for the middle school and high school sample groups. Factor analyses by gender, within and across school levels, were also conducted, but the results were similar to those obtained at each school level. The only exception was that for high school girls, Items 4 and 5 cross-loaded on both factors. To determine whether the constructs measured by the items in the WSES were distinguishable from related self-perception constructs, such as self-concept, three exploratory factor analyses were conducted using items from the WSES and from the writing self-concept, self-efficacy for self-regulated learning, or writing apprehension instruments. In each case, the self-efficacy items held their structure and loaded separately from items on these scales. Finally, to examine the construct validity of the WSES, its items were correlated with those from prominent motivation scales often used in studies of writing. As Table 3 illustrates, both Factors 1 and 2 and the full-scale scores correlated significantly with each of the variables, suggesting that items on the WSES have strong construct validity. DISCUSSION Students' self-efficacy beliefs play a critical role in their academic choices and career interests (Hackett, 1995). Zimmerman, Bonner, and Kovach (1996) urged teachers and counselors to be cognizant of their students' self-efficacy beliefs and to ensure that attention to these beliefs becomes foundational to their counseling practices and instructional strategies carried out in classrooms and schools. Teachers and counselors can use results of such assessments both to evaluate the effectiveness of academic programs and intervention strategies and to monitor student progress. Assessing students' self-efficacy beliefs can provide important insights about students' academic motivation, behavior, and future choices. In many cases, unrealistically low selfefficacy perceptions, not lack of capability or skill, can be responsible for maladaptive academic behaviors, avoidance of courses and careers, and diminishing school interest and achievement. This is as true in the area of writing as it is in other academic areas. Students who lack confidence in their writing skills are less likely to engage in tasks in which writing is required, and they will more quickly give up in the face of difficulty. In such cases, in addition to continued skill improvement, teachers and counselors must work to identify their students' inaccurate self-beliefs and implement interventions to challenge these self-beliefs. When problematically low self-efficacy is identified, students can be helped to develop a better understanding of their potential to succeed in a desired path or academic area. Often, low self-efficacy is due to an inaccurate understanding of what skills a task or activity demands. In such cases, young people can be helped to better understand what abilities and skills a course of action will actually require. Identifying, challenging, and altering low self-efficacy is essential to success and adaptive functioning. Schools have the dual responsibility of increasing students' competence and confidence. When academic difficulties erode students' self-efficacy in their writing capability, it will be difficult to improve this capability without altering the self-efficacy beliefs that are instrumental in creating and nurturing it. In concert with Bandura's (1986) contention that belief and behavior influence each other reciprocally, self-efficacy beliefs and writing competence work in tandem, and improving one requires improving the other. To this end, Bandura wisely argued that educational practices should be gauged not only by the skills and knowledge they impart for present use but also by what they do to children's beliefs about their capabilities, which affects how they approach the future. Students who develop a strong sense of self-efficacy are well equipped to educate themselves when they have to rely on their own initiative, (p. 417) Assessing students' self-efficacy beliefs is warranted because these beliefs are not always self-evident. Capable individuals often hold deep insecurities that they will not readily admit. Moreover, students themselves need to be able to assess their own self-efficacy beliefs so that they may use this information to gauge their own sense of confidence as they select tasks, activities, and self-regulatory strategies to improve their academic achievement (Zimmerman et al., 1996). Students confident in their capability are more likely to engage in these strategies than are students beset with self-doubt. Theoretical tenets cannot be effectively tested without reliable and valid self-efficacy instruments. Researchers who wish to obtain insights into the nature of academic selfefficacy, and its relationship to academic competence, must use valid and reliable measures that faithfully reflect the self-efficacy construct and its role in the broader structure of social cognitive theory. This is especially important in the field of academic motivation in which self-efficacy beliefs have often been operationalized and measured in a manner that either bears little resemblance to the construct put forth by Bandura (1986) or resembles other, less context-specific beliefs such as self-concept or expectancy perceptions. These assessments have often led to confounded results and inconsistent findings (see Bandura. 2006; Pajares, 1996, 1997). In addition, organizations such as the National Association of Educational Progress and the Programme for International Student Assessment regularly include assessments tapping students' motivational beliefs in their studies of academic competence across domains. Items from scales such as the WSES would be especially appropriate in these assessments as researchers move toward more comprehensive national and international investigations of writing competence and motivation. Results of the factor and reliability analyses conducted in this study reveal that the WSES is psychometrically sound and can be confidently used to assess writing self-efficacy with children from Grade 4 through high school. The scale proved equally reliable with girls and with boys and across grade levels. Also, although researchers can analyze the two factors separately in studies of writing self-efficacy, the general lack of a discriminant pattern of correlations between the two factors on the one hand and the motivation and achievement variables on the other provides support for the use of the WSES as a unidimensional scale. Moreover, as Pajares et al. (2001) contended, it is clear that even very young children at the elementary school level can make a discriminating judgment using a 0-100 scale. I concur with Bandura's (1997) warning that "including too few steps loses differentiating information because people who use the same response category would differ if intermediate steps were included" (p. 44). Researchers including measures of self-efficacy in studies of academic motivation should do so with attention to Bandura's assessment guidelines. Teachers, counselors, and researchers wishing to assess students' self-efficacy beliefs about writing (or any academic subject) would profit from doing so using scales with high predictive utility. It should be kept mind, of course, that reliability is a characteristic of scores from a specific sample of individuals who completed an instrument at a particular time in particular situations. Researchers should continue to refine writing self-efficacy scales such as the WSES with the aim of further enhancing their psychometric properties and their applicability to other student populations and settings. Multigroups confirmatory factor analyses should also be conducted to further establish that the WSES demonstrates factorial invariance across groups (e.g., Chen, Sousa, & West, 2005). References REFERENCES Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action: A social cognitive theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The exercise of control. New York: Freeman. Bandura, A. (2006). Guidelines for creating self-efficacy scales. In F. Pajares & T. Urdan (Eds.), Adolescence and education: Vol. 5. Self-efficacy beliefs of adolescents (pp. 307337). Greenwich. CT: Information Age Publishing. Bruning. R., & Horn, C. (2000). Developing motivation to write. Educational Psychologist, 35, 25-38. Cattell, R. B. (1966). The scree test for the number of factors. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 1, 245-276. Chen, F. F., Sousa, K. H., & West, S. G. (2005). Testing measurement invariance for a second-order factor model. Journal of Structural Equation Modeling, 12, 471-492. Cleary, L. M. (1996). I think I know what my teachers want now: Gender and writing motivation. English Journal, 85(1), 50-57. Crain, R. M. (1996). The influence of age, race, and gender on child and adolescent multidimensional self-concept. In B. A. Bracken (Ed.), Handbook of self-concept: Development, social, and clinical considerations (pp. 395-420). New York: Wiley. Fabrigar, L. R., Wegener. D. T., MacCallum. R. C., & Strahan, E. J. (1999). Evaluating the use of exploratory factor analysis in psychological research. Psychological Methods, 4, 272299. Faigley, L. (1990). Competing theories of process: A critique and a proposal. In R. L. Graves (Ed.), Rhetoric and composition (pp. 38-53). Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann. Hackett. G. (1995). Self-efficacy in career choice and development. In A. Bandura (Ed.), Self-efficacy in changing societies (pp. 232-258). New York: Cambridge University Press. Hairston, M. (1990). The winds of change: Thomas Kuhn and the revolution in the teaching of writing. In R. L. Graves (Ed.), Rhetoric and composition (pp. 3-15). Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann. Harris, K., & Graham, S. (1992). Self-regulated strategy development: Apart of the writing process. In M. Pressley, K. Harris, & J. Guthrie (Eds.), Promoting academic competence and literacy in school (pp. 277-309). San Diego. CA: Academic Press. Hayes, J. (2006). A cognitive theory of writing development. In C. A. Macarthur, S. Graham, & J. Fitzgerald (Eds.), Handbook of writing research (pp. 28-10). New York: Guilford Press. Henson, R. K. (2001). Understanding internal consistency reliability estimates: A conceptual primer on coefficient alpha. Measurement and Evaluation in Counseling and Development, 34, 177-189. Hidi, S., & Boscolo, P. (2006). Motivation in writing. In C. A. Macarthur, S. Graham, & J. Fitzgerald (Eds.), Handbook of writing research (pp. 140-157). New York: Guilford Press. Joreskog, K. G., & Lawley, D. N. (1968). New methods in maximum likelihood factor analysis. British Journal of Mathematical and Statistical Psychology, 21, 85-96. MacCallum, R. C., Widaman, K. F., Zhang, S., & Hong, S. (1999). Sample size in factor analysis. Psychological Methods, 4, 84-99. Marsh, H.W.(1989). Age and sex effects in multiple dimensions of self-concept: Preadolescence to adulthood. Journal of Educational Psychology, 81. 417-430. Marsh, H. W. (1990). The structure of academic self-concept: The Marsh-Shavelson Model. Journal of Educational Psychology, 82, 623-636. McCarthy, P., Meier, S., & Rinderer, R. (1985). Self-efficacy and writing. College Composition and Communication, 36, 465-471. Meier, S., McCarthy. P. R., & Schmeck, R. R. (1984). Validity of self-efficacy as a predictor of writing performance. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 8, 107-120. Middleton, M. J., & Midgley, C. (1997). Avoiding the demonstration of lack of ability: An underexplored aspect of goal theory. Journal of Educational Psychology, 89, 710-718. Montanelli, R. G., Jr., & Humphreys, L. G. (1976). Latent roots of random data correlation matrices with squared multiple correlations on the diagonal: A Monte Carlo study. Psychometrika, 41, 341-348. Nunnally, J, C., & Bernstein, I. H. (1994). Psychometric theory (3rd ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill. O'Connor, B. P. (2000). SPSS and SAS programs for determining the number of components using parallel analysis and Velicer's MAP test. Behavior Research Methods, Instrumentation, and Computers, 32, 396-402. Pajares, F. (1996). Self-efficacy beliefs in academic settings. Review of Educational Research, 66, 543-578. Pajares, F. (1997). Current directions in self-efficacy research. In M. Maehr & P. R. Pintrich (Eds.), Advances in motivation and achievement (Vol. 10. pp. 1-49). Greenwich. CT: JAI Press. Pajares, F. (2003). Self-efficacy beliefs, motivation, and achievement in writing: A review of the literature. Reading and Writing Quarterly, 19, 139-158. Pajares, F., Britner, S. L., & Valiante, G. (2000). Relation between achievement goals and self-beliefs of middle school students in writing and science. Contemporary Educational Psychology; 25, 406-422. Pajares, F., Hartley, J., & Valiante, G. (2001). Response format in writing self-efficacy assessment: Greater discrimination increases prediction. Measurement and Evaluation in Counseling and Development, 33, 214-221. Pajares, F., Miller, M. D., & Johnson, M. J. (1999). Gender differences in writing self-beliefs of elementary school students. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 50-61. Pajares, F., & Valiante, G. (1997). Influence of writing self-efficacy beliefs on the writing performance of upper elementary' students. Journal of Educational Research, 90, 353-360. Pajares. F., & Valiante, G. (1999). Grade level and gender differences in the writing selfbeliefs of middle school students. Contemporary Educational Psychology; 24, 390-405. Pajares. F., & Valiante, G. (2001). Gender differences in writing motivation and achievement of middle school students: A function of gender orientation? Contemporary Educational Psychology: 26, 366-381. Pajares. F., & Valiante, G. (2006). Self-efficacy beliefs and motivation in writing development. In C. A. Macarthur, S. Graham, & J. Fitzgerald (Eds.), Handbook of writing research (pp. 158-170). New York: Guilford Press. Pedhazur, E. J. (1982). Multiple regression in behavioral research: Explanation and prediction (2nd ed.). New York: Harcourt Brace. Rankin, J. L., Bruning, R. H., & Timme, V. L. (1994). The development of beliefs about spelling and their relationship to spelling performance. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 8, 213-232. Seegers, G., & Boekaerts, M. (1996). Gender-related differences in self-referenced cognitions in relation to mathematics. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 27, 215-240. Sharma, S. (1996). Applied muitivariate techniques. New York: Wiley. Shell, D. F., Colvin, C., & Bruning, R. H. (1995). Self-efficacy, attributions, and outcome expectancy mechanisms in reading and writing achievement: Grade-level and achievement-level differences. Journal of Educational Psychology, 87, 386-398. Shell, D. F., Murphy, C. C., & Bruning, R. H. (1989). Self-efficacy and outcome expectancy mechanisms in reading and writing achievement. Journal of Educational Psychology, 81, 91-100. Wachholz, P. B., & Ethendge, C. P. (1996). Writing self-efficacy beliefs of high- and lowapprehensive writers. Journal of Developmental Education, 19, 16-24. West, S. G., Finch, J. F., & Curran, P. J. (1995). Structural equation models with nonnormal variables: Problems and remedies. In R. H. Hoyle (Ed.), Structural equation modeling: Concepts, issues, and application (pp. 56-75). Newbury Park. CA: Sage. Wigfield, A., Eccles. J., MacIver, D., Reuman. D., & Midgley, C. (1991). Transitions at early adolescence: Changes in children's domain-specific self-perceptions and general selfesteem across the transition to junior high school. Developmental Psychology; 27, 552565. Zimmerman, B., & Bandura, A. (1994). Impact of self-regulatory influences on writing course attainment. American Education Research Journal, 31, 845-862. Zimmerman, B. J., Bonner, S., & Kovach, R. (1996). Developing self-regulated learners: Beyond achievement to self-efficacy. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. AuthorAffiliation Frank Pajares, Division of Educational Studies, Emory University. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Frank Pajares, Division of Educational Studies, 1784 N. Decatur Road, Suite 240, Emory University, Atlanta, GA 30322 (e-mail: mpajare@emory.edu). © 2007 American Counseling Association. All rights reserved. Copyright American Counseling Association Jan 2007 Word count: 5708 Indexing (details) Cite CloseSubject Elementary schools; Academic achievement; College students; Motivation; Writing; Studies; Gender differences; Composition; Middle school students; Children & youth Title Empirical Properties of a Scale to Assess Writing Self-Efficacy in School Contexts Author Pajares, Frank Publication title Measurement and Evaluation in Counseling and Development Volume 39 Issue 4 Pages 239-249 Number of pages 11 Publication year 2007 Publication date Jan 2007 Year 2007 Publisher American Counseling Association Place of publication Alexandria Country of publication United States Journal subject Education ISSN 07481756 Source type Scholarly Journals Language of publication English Document type Feature Document feature Tables;References Subfile Academic achievement, Studies, Children & youth, Motivation, College students, Gender differences, Composition, Middle school students, Elementary schools, Writing ProQuest document ID 195601453 Document URL http://search.proquest.com/docview/195601453?accountid=45993 Copyright Copyright American Counseling Association Jan 2007 Last updated 2011-09-08 Database ProQuest Education Journals