Public Attitudes about Supreme Court Decision-Making: Sources of Instability in Beliefs about Legal Realism Michael J. Nelson Assistant Professor of Political Science The Pennsylvania State University Pond Laboratory University Park, PA 16802 mjn15@psu.edu Phone: 515.351.9686 and Steven S. Smith Kate M. Gregg Distinguished Professor of Social Science Professor of Political Science Washington University in St. Louis One Brookings Drive Campus Box 1027 St. Louis, MO 63130-4899 smith@wustl.edu Phone: 314.935.5630 Fax: 314.935.5688 Public Attitudes about Supreme Court Decision-Making: Sources of Instability in Beliefs about Legal Realism Abstract Despite evidence that public perceptions of the U.S. Supreme Court affect its public support, these attitudes are seldom measured and have never before been measured in a panel study. We report new data regarding perceptions of judicial decision-making, the effect of a high profile decision on those beliefs, and the relationship of those beliefs to diffuse support for the Court. Additionally, we find that individuals exhibit predictable and previously unreported variability in attitudes over time. We find that views of justices’ decisions are pliable, that changes in those views are predicted by attitudes about a recent case, and that knowledgeable Americans show no less pliability than others. We find support for the proposition that changes in views of the Court’s decisionmaking are conditioned by attitudes about the issue at stake and political identities that generate motivated reasoning. Public Attitudes about Supreme Court Decision-Making: Sources of Instability in Beliefs about Legal Realism There has been a resurgence of interest in the determinants of public support for the U.S. Supreme Court (Bartels and Johnston 2013; Gibson and Nelson 2014; Christensen and Glick 2014). Because the Court’s support is not replenished by periodic elections and its justices are appointed for life, the legitimacy of the U.S. Supreme Court is of particular concern to scholars (e.g. Gibson 2012). Indeed, if, as scholars suggest, low levels of diffuse support signal a willingness to advance fundamental changes to the Court, a Court with low levels of public support should become increasingly unlikely to exercise its power of judicial review and fulfill its democratic assignment as a check against the legislative and executive branches of government (Caldeira and Gibson 1992; Gibson and Nelson 2014). The literature on diffuse support suggests that perceptions of decisionmaking processes shape overall diffuse support for an institution (Tyler 2006). For the judicial branch, scholars sometimes advance the argument that decisionmaking processes that are perceived are fair and unbiased—a quality called procedural fairness—are one way that courts can improve their public support (Tyler 1990). Conversely, decisions perceived as unprincipled, arbitrary, or reached through unfair procedures may decrease diffuse support.1 As Baird and Gangl write, “perceptions of procedural fairness are the foundation of assessments of institutional legitimacy” (Baird and Gangl 2006, 601). Indeed, this argument has taken hold so strongly among practitioners that the American Judge’s Association now offers its members suggestions on how to foster procedural fairness in their courtrooms (American Judges Association 2007; Burke and Leben 2007).2 Surprisingly, despite the enormous importance of this topic, the scholarly literature on both perceptions of judges’ decision-making practices and the effects of those perceptions on institutional support is rather scant. We know little about how average citizens believe that justices do make decisions and even less about how they believe justices should decide cases. In one of the few examinations of the public’s perceptions of judicial decision-making, Gibson and Caldeira (2011) suggest that Americans—and knowledgeable Americans even 1 There has been some debate on the extent to which perceptions of procedural fairness contribute to diffuse support (Gibson 1989; Tyler 1989; Tyler and Rasinski 1992). 2 To be clear, we are not arguing that perceptions of unbiased decision-making are the only determinant of procedural fairness. Clearly, that is not true (e.g. Tyler 1989). Instead, we argue that such perceptions are one component of procedural fairness. more so—are not likely to turn against the Court on the basis of specific decisions.3 Moreover, because it is based on cross-sectional experimental and observational data, the current literature is unable to make and test claims about the interplay between perceptions of influences on justices’ decisions and support for the U.S. Supreme Court as they evolve over time. We do not know whether diffuse support for the Court or beliefs in legal realism are stable among Americans, or even among knowledgeable Americans. Given that even the staunchest supporters of the Court’s deep reservoir of support posit that a string of unpopular decisions has the potential to undermine the judiciary’s public support (Caldeira and Gibson 1992; Gibson, Caldeira, and Baird 1998), understanding the dynamics of attitudes toward the Court’s support and the public’s perceptions of the Court’s decision making is vital. We take up this challenge in this paper. Our purpose is to provide an appropriate panel-based test of hypotheses about change in views of Court decision-making biases that may be conditioned by evaluations of a prominent Court decision, acquired diffuse support, respondents’ social and political identities, and their level of political knowledge. We outline the hypotheses generated by recent developments in theories of legitimacy and motivated reasoning and their application to public attitudes about the Supreme Court. We describe a panel survey conducted before and after the Supreme Court’s health 3 For an examination of the determinants and consequences of judicial decisionmaking outside of the United States, see Baird (2001). care reform decision of June 2012 and specify testable hypotheses about attitudes about justices’ decisions. Our analysis has two purposes. First, we seek to understand the determinants of cross-sectional views of justices’ decision making and the extent to which these views change in response to a high profile decision. On this count, we find that views of justices’ decisions are pliable, that changes in those views are predicted by attitudes about a recent case, and that knowledgeable Americans show no less pliability than others. Second, we study the consequences of citizens’ views of judicial decision making on the Court’s public support. We find that the balance of opinion is supportive of the Court and supports a realists’ interpretation of the Court, although individuals exhibit predictable variability in attitudes over time. Competing Perspectives on Public Views of the Factors that Influence Justices’ Decisions The science of Americans’ attitudes toward the Supreme Court has been motivated by a concern about the legitimacy of unelected justices. Judges in state courts face some form of check on their service, but federal judges do not face the same constraints so the public’s perceptions of the federal courts are considered particularly important for the courts’ efficacy.4 The legitimacy of the federal courts, it is argued, is enhanced by care in selecting judges, extensive formal 4 Gibson (2012) shows that the electoral process used to select judges in the U.S. states does, on balance, increase the legitimacy of those institutions. procedures, elaborate rulings, reliance on precedent, and use of numerous symbols of justice and fairness (Bickel 1986; Breyer 2010; Dworkin 1986; Fried 2000). If judicial decisions are not mechanical applications of the law, they should at least reflect principled, sincere judgments about how the law ought to be applied (Gibson and Caldeira 2011). Concerns about the bases for judicial opinions are not without consequence. Baird (2001), examining the bases of diffuse support for courts in East and West Germany, finds that West Germans who believed that judges should follow legalistic procedures had higher levels of diffuse support for the Court, although retrospective evaluations of the Court’s actual decision making practices were not related to diffuse support. Likewise, Baird and Gangl (2006) find that perceptions about whether justices follow legalistic or political (e.g. bargaining and compromising) processes when deciding a case have effects on their perceptions of the Court independent of their policy agreement with the Court’s outcome. Of course, understanding of the relationship between perceptions of the factors that influence justices’ decision and support for the Court requires an accurate characterization of those perceptions and the forces that shape them. Here, two theoretical arguments offer guidance. The first, legitimacy and positivity theories, focuses on factors that produce consensus in favor of the Court and suppress change in support that might otherwise be a response to controversial Court decisions. The second emphasizes how motivated reasoning and the biases shaped by political identities may bifurcate attitudes about the Court in response to controversial decisions and limit overall support for the Court. Both perspectives warrant discussion. Legitimacy and Positivity Theories Legitimacy theory concerns the conditions under which individuals find the actions of individuals or institutions proper, just, or authoritative—that is, legitimate (Tyler 2006). Legitimacy produces deference and voluntary compliance, which, in the case of the Court, is concerned essential to its efficacy. It appears that the perception of fair and just procedures, the hallmark of legal argument and processes, is essential to legitimacy (Gibson 2002; Tyler 2000). In contrast, open fights to create biased processes in legislatures and other institutions undermine the perceived legitimacy of their decisions. Rationality, neutrality, and factuality count, too, but elaborate legal processes also contribute to the perception that those tests are met (Tyler 2006). In political science studies of public attitudes about the Court, all based on cross-sectional surveys, have emphasized legitimacy theory and the presence of broad support for the Court (Gibson, Caldeira, and Spence 2003). Gibson and Caldeira (2011, 200) summarize the political science on the subject with the observation that “the Supreme Court profits from a large store of reasonably stable institutional support.” The Court, they argue, is viewed by the public as playing a “non-political” role in the American system. Socialization has long played a central role in accounts of the legitimacy of political institutions (Easton 1965; Sears 1975). In part, socialization involves the acceptance of myths or stereotypes (Baird and Gangl 2006). For some social actors and institutions, this may include acceptance of the fairness or justness of decisions (Tyler 2006). Through socialization, support for an institution can become internalized, serve as the basis for deference, and become difficult to undermine. The legitimacy associated with an institution can become “sticky” (Gibson, Caldeira, and Baird 1998). Naturally, scholars have examined cross-sectional variation in support for the Court. The most important finding is that knowledge of the political system produces support for the Court (Gibson and Nelson 2014). Labeled “positivity theory” by Gibson and Caldeira (2009), the argument is that knowledge of the Court comes with greater exposure to the legitimizing legal processes and symbols associated with the Court. They may be the most fully socialized to the political system. These sophisticated people, who also tend to be engaged and partisan and therefore may be the most important to the Court’s efficacy, are the most supportive of the Court. Gibson and Caldeira (2011) demonstrate that the legitimacy of the U.S. Supreme Court does not depend on a public belief that justices apply autonomous legal principles in a mechanical, non-ideological way. Rather, Americans both admire the role of the Court and also recognize the role that discretion and personal views play a role in decision-making. They find that “as knowledge increases, the connection between the belief that judges are not merely politicians in robes and institutional support increases” (211)—knowledge generates both stronger beliefs in legal realism and more diffuse support. They infer that the Court’s “legitimacy seems to flow from the view that discretion is being exercised in a principled, rather than strategic, way” (Gibson and Caldeira 2011, 213). They attribute this combination of attitudes among Americans to the prominent symbols of “fairness and legality” associated with the Court and its rulings (214). Motivated Reasoning and Identities A long-standing theme of social science is that individuals are subject to a variety of biases in how they perceive, process, and evaluate the world around them. Motivated reasoning theory emphasizes that reasoning processes become biased when they are motivated by goals beyond accuracy (Kunda 1990; Munro, et al., 2002; Tabor and Lodge 2006). While holding an important policy preference or ideological outlook, evaluations of an event may lead people to selectively perceive factors that influenced the outcome. In the case of Court decisions, we would expect individuals’ attitudes about the policy or constitutional principles at stake to influence their evaluations of the justices’ decision-making processes. Simon and Scurich (2011) find that evaluations of judges’ decision-making processes are conditional on decision outcome. They experimentally manipulated a vignette with four types of reasoning reported for judges’ decisions, varying from no rationale provided to multiple, two-sided reasons provided in the reported decisions. They found that panelists were far more likely to find an outcome acceptable if it was accompanied by a ruling with more elaborate reasoning, which comports with the view that the usually elaborate Supreme Court decisions are widely viewed as legitimate. However, Simon and Scurich also found that “participants were indifferent toward the modes of reasoning when they agreed with the outcome of the judges’ decision, but were differentially sensitive to the judicial reasoning when the judge’s decision frustrated their preferred outcome. This finding is consistent with the fact that motivated reasoning influences not only the ultimate conclusion of a decision or inference, but also the procedures, methodologies, or facts that underlie that judgment” (2011, 719). In addition to bias derived from personal goals and evaluations, individuals may adopt attitudes that are generated by identification with prominent social groups. Social identity is the categorization of the self as a member of a social group from which individuals often acquire esteem (Tajfel 1981). Identities often have the power to produce conformity to the group’s prototype goals and beliefs (Turner 1987). A wide variety of group identities have been linked with variation in political beliefs or attitudes (Conover 1984), including ideological, partisan, religious, and ethnic identities. Identification with elites of any these groups might lead to the adoption of their interpretations about nature of justices’ decision-making processes. If these interpretations are simplified and biased, then identifiers of competing groups are likely to accept divergent views of justices’ biases. Party identification and ideological identification are forms of identity that are readily associated with a prominent decision like the PPACA. Party and ideological elites who are openly identified and widely reported with the issue provide ready cues. Less obvious is the role of racial identities, although blacks have been thought to have a group identity that was once shaped by the Warren Court’s role in deciding cases on civil rights and other issues and then was reshaped by the shift to the right in the Rehnquist and Roberts Courts (Gibson and Caldiera 1992; Hetherington and Smith 2007). Whether grounded in individual- or group-derived biases, recent studies of public support for the Court emphasize that ideological objections to the Court and even evaluations of individual cases can generate divergent perceptions of the Court’s legitimacy (Bartels and Johnston 2013; Christenson and Glick 2013; Nelson and Smith 2014; Nicholson and Hansford 2014). As Nelson and Smith (2014) observe, divergent responses to the Court may not change aggregate diffuse support for the Court or even significantly change the cross-sectional correlates of support, but the divergent responses challenge the dominant view that the Court’s reservoir of diffuse public support is stable. They also raise the possibility that belief in realism is more pliable than Gibson and Caldeira suggest. Hypotheses Our view of the limited empirical research on public views of justices’ decision-making processes is that it remains deficient. We structure our analysis in three parts: understanding the cross-sectional determinants of perceptions of the factors that influence justices’ decision, understanding change in those perceptions, and understanding the relationship between perceptions of justices’ decisions and diffuse support for the Court. In the cross-section, we seek to understand the factors that create differences in individuals’ views about the role of personal biases and legal principles in justices’ decisions. Our dependent variable is an individual’s view of realism in justices’ decision making. Knowledge is the only factor highlighted in the literature on relationship between the Court’s legitimacy and beliefs in legal realism (Gibson and Caldeira 2011), but we draw upon the literature on public opinion and motivated reasoning to posit a more complete model of perceived decision-making style that accounts for ideological, partisan, and ethnic identities: perceived style = ß + ß1 knowledge + ß2 ideological identification + ß3 party identification + ß4 ethnic identification + ε The positivity thesis predicts a positive relationship between levels of political knowledge and belief in legal realism, while, in light of the conservative balance of justices on the Court, the motivated reasoning thesis predicts ideological, partisan, and ethnicity effects. These effects are estimated for both before and after the PPACA ruling. Second, we seek to understand how respondents’ perceptions of the factors influencing justices’ decisions changed in response to the 2011 PPACA decision. We model views of justices’ styles in July about how the justices did decide the case controlling for how justices were predicted to decide in May, along with knowledge and policy views: retrospective perceived style = ß + ß1 prospective perceived style + ß2 knowledge + ß3 policy preference + ε The legitimacy/positivity argument suggests that the PPACA decision will not change views of justices’ decision-making approach. We will measure aggregate positive and negative change in perceived style directly. If change occurs, motivated reasoning and identity theories imply, positive evaluations of the PPACA outcome (those who favor the act) should produce a positive change in the way justices’ decision-making styles are viewed, while negative evaluations (those who oppose the act) should produce a negative change in the way justices’ decision-making styles are viewed—that is, ß3 > 0. We have no prediction for ß2 because we have no reason a priori to argue that knowledge is positively or negatively related to a particular direction of change in views about justices’ decision-making styles. Finally, we turn our attention to the consequences of citizens’ perceptions of justices’ decision-making styles on their diffuse support for the Court. The Gibson-Caldeira argument treats perceptions of decision-making styles and knowledge as factors that influence diffuse support for the Court. It is not clear to us that perceptions of decision-making processes are wholly exogenous to diffuse support, but we explore the Gibson-Caldeira model with our panel data. In particular, we estimate this model: diffuse supportt2 = ß + ß1 diffuse supportt1 + ß2 ∆ perceived style + ß3 knowledge + ß4 policy preference + ε The Gibson-Caldeira account predicts that ß2 > 0. Legitimacy theory predicts ß4 = 0, while motivated reasoning and identity theories predict that ß4 > 0. Again, there is no prediction for the simple knowledge effect, ß3. We also reexamine the Baird (2001) argument that diffuse support is affected by normative, rather than retrospective evaluations. Specifically, we estimate an equation that accounts for past diffuse support and both normative and retrospective views of influences on justices’ decisions: diffuse supportt2 = ß + ß1 diffuse supportt1 + ß2 perceived normative style + ß3 perceived retrospective style + ß4 knowledge + ß5 policy preference + ε Baird’s argument suggests that ß2 > 0 and ß3 =0. Data and Measurement Nelson and Smith (2014) illustrate the power of panel data for the study of diffuse support for the Court in their study of changing attitudes surrounding the 2012 decision on the 2010 health care law. They find both substantial continuity in aggregate diffuse support for the Court and predictable shifts in diffuse support in response to the Court decision. They found that knowledge was related to diffuse support in the cross section, but they failed to confirm that knowledge suppresses change in diffuse support. We exploit the same survey, The American Panel Survey (TAPS), to measure attitudes toward justices’ decision-making processes. TAPS is a monthly online survey of about 2000 people. Panelists were first recruited as a national probability sample with an addressed-based sampling frame in the fall of 2011 by GfK Knowledge Networks for the Weidenbaum Center at Washington University. Two replenishment efforts have kept the panel at approximately 2000 panelists. Individuals without internet access were provided a laptop and internet service at the expense of the Weidenbaum Center. In a typical month, over 1700 of the panelists complete the online survey. More technical information about the survey is available at taps.wustl.edu. We queried panelists before and after the Court’s decision in National Federation of Independent Business et al. v. Sebelius, Secretary of Health and Human Services, et al., on June 28, 2012, the case on the Patient Protection and Affordable Care Act (PPACA). The first battery of questions was asked in May 2012, the month before the Supreme Court issued its opinion. Two sets of questions, separated in the survey, addressed the normative (set A) and prospective (set B) kinds of decision making by justices (Table 1). In July 2012, the month following the PPACA decision, panelists were asked a set of questions (set C) about the factors that played a role in justices’ decisions that parallel the expected factors (set B). [Table 1 about here] The questions in sets A, B, and C were identical, with the exception of slight variations in the prompt. The question stem was, “How big of a role do you think each of the following [should, would, did] play in the decision of the Court?” We label the three versions “normative,” “prospective,” and “retrospective.” For each question, panelists were given a four-option response set: “no role”, “a minor role”, “a major role,” and “don’t know.” [Table 2 about here] Table 2 summarizes those responses, presenting the percentage of panelists who felt that a given factor should/will/did play “a major role” in the Court’s decision. From this table, it is clear that the public has strong feelings about the justices’ use of their personal preferences in their decisions. The proportion of the public that thinks that the justice’s ideology should play a major role is less than the proportions for public opinion or executive influence. Similarly, the differences between the public’s prospective and normative views on these indicators are revealing: The public doesn’t like an ideological court but thinks we have one. To model views of justices’ decision making, we combined the seven indicators into a single factor. The scales are reliable, with Chrombach’s alphas of 0.82 (Normative), 0.73 (Prospective), and .68 (Retrospective). For each set of questions, the results indicate that the responses are unidimensional. The eigenvalues for the second dimension are .59 (normative), .90 (prospective), .76 (retrospective). For all three sets, each of the seven variables loads more strongly on the first dimension than any other dimension. Higher values of the scale are associated with a greater belief in extralegal influence on justices’ decision making; lower values indicate a greater belief in legalistic decision-making. [Figure 1 about here] To understand the distribution of citizens’ beliefs, we turn to Figure 1, which shows the density of panelists’ normative, prospective, and retrospective beliefs about the role of extralegal influences in justices’ decision making. The three lines in each panel show the distribution of all panelists, panelists who believed that the individual mandate was constitutional, and those panelists who believed that the mandate was unconstitutional. As the top and center panels show, the distribution of beliefs for panelists’ normative and prospective views is the same for both sub-groups: While the majority of panelists believe there should be little extralegal influence on the Court’s decisionmaking, most panelists believed that the Court would be influenced by factors other than the law. The bottom panel of the figure demonstrates that, after the decision, panelists’ beliefs about extralegal influence are associated with their policy preferences: panelists who believed the law was unconstitutional (the dashed line) were more likely to believe there was more extralegal influence in justices’ decisions than those who believed the law was constitutional. The mean level of extralegal influence among those who believed the mandate was unconstitutional was .15 while the mean among those who believed the law was constitutional was -.05, a statistically significant difference. The key explanatory variable of interest measures panelists’ attitudes toward the individual mandate portion of the PPACA. Panelists were asked, “Concerning the requirement that every American must buy health insurance or pay a fine, do you think that this is constitutional)?” Additionally, we test for the effect of political knowledge on attitudes toward Supreme Court decision making. The measure of political knowledge is an additive scale of the number of correct responses to the eight questions (see Appendix). We also explore whether the effect of health care attitudes on views about Court’s decision making vary according to levels of political knowledge. Findings We begin our analysis with an examination of the underpinnings of citizens’ beliefs about the proper influences on justices’ decisions. To this end, we regressed panelists’ beliefs about normative influences on judicial decision making on political knowledge, party affiliation, ideology, and a series of demographic variables. The estimates of the equation are shown in the first column of Table 3. [Table 3 about here] Three patterns are important. First, contrary to the relationship reported in Caldeira and Gibson (2011), there is an inverse relationship between political knowledge and beliefs about extralegal influences on justices’ decisions. Individuals with high levels of political knowledge tend to believe that legal influences should play a greater role in judicial decision making than do lowknowledge individuals. Second, ceteris paribus, policy preferences do not affect normative beliefs about influences on justices’ decisions; there is no statistically significant effect for view of the mandate’s constitutionality.5 Thus, these results suggest that noninterpretivist views are not only held by liberals. Still, racial/ethnic identities are related to views about the proper influences on justices’ decisions. African Americans are more favorable toward extralegal influences and Hispanic Americans less favorable toward extralegal influences than other Americans. Indeed, the size of the effect for African Americans is the single strongest relationship that we find. To assess the determinants of prospective and retrospective beliefs about justices’ decision making in the PPACA case, we turn to the second two columns of Table 3.6 Two findings are of particular significance. First, just as with 5 As shown in the appendix, this finding extends to other measures of policy preferences: neither liberals and conservatives nor Democrats and Republicans differ in the extent to which extralegal influences should enter into the Court’s decision-making process. 6 The appendix contains a variety of analyses to confirm the robustness of these results. First, we reestimate the model controlling for party identification, and normative beliefs about the role of legal and extralegal factors on justices’ decisions, more knowledge of the political arena tends to produce a stronger belief that justices will and were guided by legal principles in their decisions. Second, the effect of policy views on views of realism changes during the episode. Before the decision, views of the constitutionality of the mandate were unrelated to views of judicial realism; after the decision, those who were unhappy with the decision tended to see more extralegal bias in justices’ decision making. This result confirms the bivariate relationships seen in Figure 1, where the differences between those who believed health care is constitutional and those who did not are greater in the retrospective evaluations than in the prospective evaluations. [Table 4 about here] ideology; the substantive results are the same, though the policy preference effect appears in the ideology coefficient, rather than the health care attitudes coefficient when all three measures are included in the model. Second, readers may be concerned about the exclusion of some demographic variables (e.g. age and gender) are excluded from these cross-sectional models. Models including these variables are shown in the appendix and their omission has no effect on the results discussed here. Third, consider in the appendix the interaction between political knowledge and policy preferences, as suggested by Johnston, Hillygus, and Bartels (2013). Finally, Table 3 includes measures of health care attitudes as the measure of policy preferences. In the appendix, we present a model including health care attitudes rather than attitudes toward constitutionality specifically. In all cases, the results are robust. Next, to examine the change between retrospective and prospective evaluations of the influences on justices’ decisions, we turn to Table 4. Here, we regressed panelists’ perspective beliefs about extralegal influence on judicial decision making in the PPACA case, as well as their policy preferences and their level of political knowledge, on their retrospective beliefs about influences on justices’ decisions. In the second column of the table, we present the results including measures of ethnic and political identities. In both models, one’s prospective views about decision making provide a statistically significant predictor of one’s retrospective views of judicial decision making. There is evidence of motivated reasoning: Individuals who believed that the individual mandate was unconstitutional become more likely (even when controlling for their prospective views about influences on justices’ decisions) to believe that the Court was influenced by nonlegal factors. However, when political and ethnic identities are included in the model, this effect disappears, likely because of multicollinearity between opinions on health care reform, partisanship, and ideology. Indeed, 80 percent of conservatives and 87 percent of Republicans believed that the individual mandate was unconstitutional. There is also evidence of a relationship between knowledge and retrospective views of judicial decision-making. As shown in both models, there is a negative relationship between knowledge and retrospective evaluations, indicating that the more knowledgeable panelists tend to believe that extralegal influences play a lesser role in the PPACA decision than less knowledgeable panelists. Consistent with the Gibson-Caldeira view that more knowledgeable people have more durable views of the Court, more knowledgeable people show less movement in attitudes about the Court than other people. Indeed, the negative correlation between knowledge and change in respondents’ beliefs is due primarily to movement among the less knowledgeable panelists. The overall mean on the prospective and retrospective factors is .12 and .09, respectively, but for the most knowledgeable half of the sample the means are .012 and .006. In contrast, for the less knowledgeable half, the difference is starker: a mean of .345 for respondents’ prospective views and a mean of .253 for their retrospective views. [Table 5 about here] Finally, we probe the effects of change in beliefs about the Court’s decision making on diffuse support for the Court. Table 5 presents OLS estimates the effects of political knowledge, support for health care, and respondents’ perceptions of the Court’s decision-making style. The first column examines change in respondents’ retrospective and prospective evaluations of the Court’s decision. The estimated coefficient is negative, indicating a smaller change in diffuse support among those individuals whose retrospective evaluations of the influences on justices’ decisions assign more extralegal influence than they expected prospectively. Additionally, the estimates confirm the positive relationship between knowledge and diffuse support and replicate Nelson and Smith’s (2014) finding about the effects of the health care decision on change in diffuse support. Additionally, the second column of Table 5 tests Baird’s (2001) findings about the relative importance of retrospective and normative assessments of the influences on justices’ decisions on diffuse support for the Court. Interestingly, our findings are directly contrary to Baird’s. While Baird finds that normative, but not retrospective, evaluations have a direct effect on diffuse support for the Court, we find that retrospective, but not normative, evaluations effect respondents’ evaluations of Supreme Court decision making. The direction of the effect is the same in both our study and in Baird’s: belief in legalistic decision making is associated with higher diffuse support for the Court.7 Conclusion The TAPS panel data on attitudes toward the Court in the PPACA episode have provided a direct test of propositions about the conditioning effects of knowledge, diffuse support for the Court, and motivated reasoning on beliefs about realism in Court decision making. In the aggregate, views of legal realism on the Court did not change greatly during the PPACA episode, but individuallevel change is considerable. We have found substantial support for the 7 To explain the differences between our study and Baird’s, we note two clear differences: (1) we rely on panel data with a salient intervention—the PPACA decision—between the two waves of the panel while Baird’s data is cross-sectional and (2) that the institutional contexts in which the West German Constitutional Court in Baird’s study and the 2011 U.S. Supreme Court in the midst of deciding the PPACA case operate is vastly different. proposition that changes in views of legal realism are conditioned by attitudes about the issue at stake and political identities that generate motivated reasoning. Separating the effects of knowledge and diffuse support proves difficult, but they, too, are related to change in views of legal realism. In the cross-section, greater knowledge is associated with a stronger preference for reliance on legal principles, contrary to the findings in a previous study. Moreover, greater knowledge is associated with less emphasis on extralegal principles—less “realism”—after the decision, controlling for predecision views of realism. These findings fit well with some of the earliest research about attitudes toward judicial decisionmaking (Casey 1974, Murphy and Tanenhaus 1968; Murphy, Tanenhaus, and Kastner 1973). As Gibson, Caldeira, and Baird (1998) put it: It may be that ordinary people who know little about courts have few reasons to believe that judges make decisions differently from any other politicians. Those attentive to courts come to adopt a different view, but not the view of legal realists. Greater awareness is associated with the perception that judges are different, that they rely on law not values in making decisions, that they are "objective" (345). Thus, our results seem to indicate that more political knowledge is not associated with a belief that the Court should or will rely on extralegal factors when making their decisions. We are not as sanguine as Gibson and Caldeira (2011) about how realism and diffuse support for the Court hang together among more knowledgeable Americans. Indeed, in contrast to the findings of Gibson and Caldeira (2011), our results suggest that legal realists tend to be less, rather than more, knowledgeable about politics. Finally, our results suggest that perceptions of the factors influencing justices’ decisions matter. They are not stagnant and they shape citizens’ support for the Court. Both a greater gap between one’s prospective and retrospective evaluations of the Court’s performance and one’s retrospective evaluations of the influences on justices’ decisions are, all else equal, associated with change in diffuse support for the Court. Indeed, both a greater expectations gap and retrospective evaluations tilted more heavily toward extralegal influence are associated with lower diffuse support for the Court, even controlling for one’s policy preferences and level of political knowledge. Our panel study demonstrates the value of monitoring public attitudes at the individual level. Research findings are beginning to accumulate that show that citizens vary systematically, both in the cross section and over time, in their perceptions of influences on justices’ decisions and in their diffuse support for the Court. Of course, the PPACA case, a very salient case, is only one case and its lasting consequences for attitudes about the Court awaits analysis of additional cases. References American Judges Association. 2007. “Special Issue on Procedural Fairness.” Court Review 44(1-2). Baird, Vanessa A. 2001. “Building Institutional Legitimacy: The Role of Procedural Justice.” Political Research Quarterly 54 (#2, June): 333-354. Baird, Vanessa A., and Amy Gangl. 2006. “Shattering the Myth of Legality: The Impact of the Media’s Framing of Supreme Court Procedures on Perceptions of Fairness.” Political Psychology 27 (#4, August): 597-614. Bartels, Brandon L., and Christopher D. Johnston. 2013. “On the Ideological Foundations of Supreme Court Legitimacy in the American Public.” American Journal of Political Science 57 (#1, January): 184-199. Bickel, Alexander M. 1986. The Least Dangerous Branch: The Supreme Court at the Bar of Politics. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press. Breyer, Stephen 2010. Making Our Democracy Work: A Judge’s View. New York, NY: Random House. Burke, Kevin and Steve Leben. 2007. “Procedural Fairness: A Key Ingredient in Public Satisfaction.” A White Paper of the AMeriacn Judges Association” May 1 2014. Available at < http://www.amjudges.org/pdfs/AJAWhitePaper9-26-07.pdf>. Caldeira, Gregory A. and James L. Gibson. 1992. “The Etiology of Public Support for the Supreme Court.” American Journal of Political Science 36 (#3, August): 635-664. Casey, Gregory. 1974. “The Supreme Court and Myth: An Empirical Investigation.” Law & Society Review, 8:385-419. Conover, Pamela J. 1984. “The Influence of Group Identifications on Political Participation and Evaluation.” The Journal of Politics (46 #3): 760-785. Christenson, Dino and David Glick. 2014. “The Micro-Foundations of the Court’s Legitimacy.” Working Paper. Dworkin, Ronald. 1986. Law’s Empire. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Easton, David. 1965. A Systems Analysis of Political Life. New York: John Wiley & Son, Inc. Gibson, James L. 1989. “Understandings of Justice: Institutional Legitimacy, Procedural Justice, and Political Tolerance.” Law & Society Review 23 (#3):469-496. Gibson, James L. 2002. “Truth, Justice, and Reconciliation: Judging the Fairness of Amnesty in South Africa.” American Journal of Political Science 46: (#3, July): 540-556. Gibson, James L. 2012. Electing Judges: The Surprising Effects of Campaigning on Judicial Legitimacy. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Gibson, James L., and Gregory A. Caldeira. 1992. “Blacks and the United States Supreme Court: Models of Diffuse Support.” Journal of Politics 54 (#4, November): 1120-1145. Gibson, James L., and Gregory A. Caldeira. 2009. Citizens, Courts, and Confirmations: Positivity Theory and the Judgments of the American People. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. Gibson, James L., and Gregory A. Caldeira. 2011. “Has Legal Realism Damaged the Legitimacy of the U.S. Supreme Court?” Law and Society Review 45 (#1): 195-219 Gibson, James L., Gregory A. Caldeira, and Vanessa Baird. 1998. “On the Legitimacy of National High Courts.” American Political Science Review 92 (#2, June): 343-358. Gibson, James L., Gregory A. Caldeira, and Lester Kenyatta Spence. 2003. “Measuring Attitudes toward the United States Supreme Court.” American Journal of Political Science 47 (#2, April): 354-367. Gibson, James L., and Michael J. Nelson. 2014. “Is the U.S. Supreme Court’s Legitimacy Grounded in Performance Satisfaction and Ideology?” American Journal of Political Science. Heatherington, Marc and Joseph L. Smith 2007. “Issue Preferences and Evaluations of the Supreme Court.” Public Opinion Quarterly. 71: 40-66. Johnston, Christopher, Sunshine Hillygus, and Brandon Bartels. Forthcoming. “Ideology, the Affordable Care Act Ruling, and Supreme Court Legitimacy.” Public Opinion Quarterly. Kunda Ziva. 1990. “The Case for Motivated Reasoning.” Psychological Bulletin 108 (#3): 480-498. Munro, Geoffrey D., Peter H. Ditto, Lisa K. Lockhart, Angela Fagerlin, Mitchell Gready, and Elizabeth Peterson. 2002. “Biased Assimilation of Sociopolitical Arguments: Evaluating the 1996 Presidential Debate.” Basic and Applied Social Psychology. 24 (#1): 15-26. Murphy, Walter F., and Joseph Tanenhaus. 1968. “Public Opinion and the United States Supreme Court: A Preliminary Mapping of Some Prerequisites for Court Legitimation of Regime Changes.” Law and Society Review 2 (#3, May): 357-384. Murphy, Walter F., Joseph Tanenhaus and Daniel L. Kastner. 1973. Public Evaluations of Constitutional Courts, Alternate Explanations. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage Publications. Nicholson, Stephen P. and Thomas G. Hansford. 2014. “Partisans in Robes: Party Cues and Public Acceptance of Supreme Court Decisions.” American Journal of Political Science Forthcoming. Nelson, Michael J. and Steven S. Smith 2014. “Change and Continuity in Diffuse Support for the U.S. Supreme Court.” Working Paper. Sears, David O. 1975. “Political Socialization.” In F.I. Greenstein and N.W. Polsby (eds.), Handbook of Political Science, Vol. 2. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley: 93-153. Simon, Dan, and Nicholas Scurich. 2011. “Lay Judgments of Judicial Decision Making.” Journal of Empirical Legal Studies 8 (#4, December): 709-727. Taber, Charles S. and Milton Lodge. 2006. “Motivated Skepticism in the Evaluation of Political Beliefs.” American Journal of Political Science. 51 (#3): 755-769. Tajfel, Henri. 1981. Human Groups and Social Categories. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Turner, J. C. 1987. Rediscoving the Social Group: A Self-Categorization Theory. Oxford: Basil Blackwell. Tyler, Tom R. 1989. “The Psychology of Procedural Justice: A Test of the Group Value Model.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 57: 830-838. Tyler, Tom R. 1990. Why People Obey the Law: Procedural Justice, Legitimacy, and Compliance. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press. Tyler, Tom R. 2000. “Multiculturalism and the Willingness of Citizens to Defer to Law and to Legal Authorities.” Law and Social Inquiry 25 (#3): 9831019. Tyler, Tom R. and K. Rasinski. 1992. “Procedural Justice, Institutional Legitimacy, and the Acceptance of Unpopular Decisions Made by the United States Supreme Court: A Reply to Gibson.” Law & Society Review 25: 101-110. Tyler, Tom R. 2006. “Psychological Perspectives on Legitimacy and Legitimation.” Annual Review of Psychology 57 (January): 375-400. Table 1. TAPS Questions about Justices’ Decision Making Asked in May and July 2012. Normative Set (May 2012) A. When the Supreme Court rules on the case challenging the requirement that everyone buy health insurance or pay a fine, how big of a role do you think each of the following SHOULD play in their decision? [major role, minor role, no role, don’t know] 1. The personal views of the justices about the health insurance requirement. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. 7. The justices’ analysis and interpretation of the law. The views of average Americans about the requirement. The public’s likely reaction to the Court’s decision. The justices liberal or conservative views. Whether the justice was appointed by a Democratic or Republican president. President Obama’s views on the legal issues. Prospective (May 2012) B. When the Supreme Court rules on the case challenging the requirement that everyone buy health insurance or pay a fine, how big of a role do you think each of the following WILL play in their decision? [major role, minor role, no role, don’t know] 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. 7. The personal views of the justices about the health insurance requirement. The justices’ analysis and interpretation of the law. The views of average Americans about the requirement. The public’s likely reaction to the Court’s decision. The justices liberal or conservative views. Whether the justice was appointed by a Democratic or Republican president. President Obama’s views on the legal issues. Retrospective (July 2012) C. When the Supreme Court ruled on the case challenging the requirement that everyone buy health insurance or pay a fine, how big of a role do you think each of the following played in their decision? ? [major role, minor role, no role, don’t know] 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. The personal views of the justices about the health insurance requirement. The justices’ analysis and interpretation of the law. The views of average Americans about the requirement. The public’s likely reaction to the Court’s decision. The justices liberal or conservative views. Whether the justice was appointed by a Democratic or Republican president. 7. President Obama’s views on the legal issues. Table 2. Percentage saying that a factor plays “a major role” in Supreme Court decisionmaking in the PPACA case. All Panelists "Major Role" Norm. Pros. Retros. Health Care is Unconstitutional Norm. Pros. Retros. Health Care is Constitutional Norm. Pros. Retros. Personal Views 18.68 39.15 44.65 20.31 38.34 47.79 15.18 40.89 38.09 Analysis of Law 74.09 73.89 68.84 72.20 72.98 64.68 78.14 75.85 77.47 Avg. Americans Public's Response 48.92 20.81 15.39 54.24 21.84 16.17 37.49 18.60 13.75 32.57 19.84 18.82 35.90 20.76 19.98 25.39 17.89 16.40 Lib/Cons Views Appointing POTUS 12.77 36.48 48.16 13.87 35.27 46.68 10.41 39.08 51.29 9.84 28.04 30.83 10.14 27.52 32.68 9.20 29.16 26.98 Obama's Views 16.61 21.77 35.31 14.50 23.15 41.41 21.38 18.79 22.64 Table 3. Cross-sectional views of Supreme Court decisionmaking HC is Unconstitutional Political Knowledge African American Hispanic Constant R2 Adjusted R2 N (1) Normative 0.09 (0.06) -0.21* (0.02) 0.91* (0.10) -0.36* (0.10) 1.36* (0.17) 0.27 0.27 1417 (2) Prospective 0.06 (0.06) -0.09* (0.02) 0.34* (0.12) 0.07 (0.10) 0.38* (0.11) 0.07 0.06 1417 (3) Retrospective 0.18* (0.05) -0.06* (0.02) 0.13 (0.11) 0.02 (0.06) 0.21* (0.10) 0.04 0.04 1712 Table 4. Models of retrospective views of decisionmaking in the PPACA case, conditional on prospective views. (1) (2) Prospective Views 0.20* 0.20* (0.05) (0.05) HC is Unconstitutional 0.17* 0.04 (0.06) (0.07) * Political Knowledge -0.05 -0.05* (0.02) (0.02) African American 0.15 (0.11) Hispanic -0.04 (0.06) Party Identification 0.04 (0.02) Ideology 0.04* (0.02) Constant 0.16 -0.03 (0.12) (0.13) R2 0.08 0.11 Adjusted R2 0.08 0.10 N 1712 1712 Table 5. Change in Diffuse Support for the Court (1) Legitimacy at t1 0.69* (0.04) Change in Perceived Style -0.11* (0.03) Normative Evaluations Retrospective Evaluations HC is Unconstitutional Political Knowledge Constant R2 Adjusted R2 N -0.30* (0.06) 0.10* (0.02) 1.00* (0.15) 0.52 0.52 1712 (2) 0.62* (0.05) -0.09 (0.05) -0.19* (0.06) -0.30* (0.06) 0.07* (0.02) 1.36* (0.27) 0.54 0.54 1712 Figure 1. Density plots showing panelists’ normative, prospective, and retrospective views of judicial decisionmaking in the PPACA case. 0.4 0.2 0.0 Density 0.6 Normative Views -2 -1 0 1 2 3 2 3 Belief in Extralegal Influence 0.4 0.2 0.0 Density 0.6 Prospective Views -2 -1 0 1 Belief in Extralegal Influence 0.2 0.4 All Respondents Unconstitutional Constitutional 0.0 Density 0.6 Retrospective Views -2 -1 0 1 Belief in Extralegal Influence 2 3 Appendix A: Measurement In this appendix, we describe the measurement of a variety of variables used in the empirical analyses, both in the main text of the paper and throughout this appendix. Normative, Prospective, and Retrospective Index Information on the factor loadings for the normative, prospective, and retrospective indexes is shown in Table A1. Overall, the items seem to load well onto each factor, though the items load more weakly on the retrospective index. Notably, the item asking respondents about their opinions of the role that the “analysis of law” should/will/did play in judicial decisionmaking loads poorly on all three factors. Diffuse Support Some of the models in the paper and in the appendix utilize an index of diffuse support. Our measure is the same as the one used by Nelson and Smith (2014). The items used in the index, their factor loading, and the percentage of respondents giving a response supportive of the Court at t1 and t2 are: If the Supreme Court started making a lot of rulings that most Americans disagreed with, it might be better to do away with the Court altogether. [0.76, 70%, 72%] The right of the Supreme Court to decide certain types of controversial issues should be reduced. [0.77, 54%, 57%] The U.S. Supreme Court gets too mixed up in politics. [0.57, 26%, 23%] Judges on the U.S. Supreme Court who consistently make decisions at odds with what a majority of the people want should be removed from their position as judge. [0.78, 49%, 54%] The U.S. Supreme Court ought to be made less independent so that it listens a lot more to what the people want. [0.82, 50%, 54%] It is inevitable that the U.S. Supreme Court gets mixed up in politics; therefore, we ought to have stronger means of controlling the actions of the U.S. Supreme Court. [0.82, 45%, 49%] Common factor analysis indicates that the measure is unidimensional (1st eigenvalue=3.46; 2nd eigenvalue=0.08). Religiosity To measure the extent to which respondents practice a religion in their daily lives, we relied upon a four question battery. The scale was measured in January 2012. The four items in the scale are: How often do you say grace before meals? [loading=0.75] How often do you pray? [loading=0.81] How often do you attend religious services? [loading=0.73] Please indicate which statement comes closest to expressing what you believe about God [I don’t believe/I have no way to find out/I believe in some higher power/I believe sometimes/I believe but have doubts/I know that God exists] [loading=-.75]. Common factor analysis indicates that the measure is unidimensional (1st eigenvalue=2.32; 2nd eigenvalue=0.07). Support for Minority Political Liberty This scale, used by Gibson and Nelson (Forthcoming) measures the degree to which the respondent favors social order when it comes into conflict with the liberty of political minorities. The three items in the scale, measured in May 2012, are: Society should not have to put up with those who have political ideas that are extremely different from the majority. (15.44% adopt the order position [agree]; loading=.63) It is better to live in an orderly society than to allow people so much freedom that they can become disruptive. (36.16% adopt the order position [agree]; loading=.56) Free speech is just not worth it if it means that we have to put up with the danger to society of extremist political views. (13.83% adopt the order position [agree]; loading=.67) Common factor analysis indicates that the measure is unidimensional (1st eigenvalue=1.17; 2nd eigenvalue=-0.12). Support for the Rule of Law This scale, used by Gibson and Nelson (Forthcoming) measures the degree to which the respondent favors universalism rather than particularism. The five items in the scale, measured in May 2012, are: It is not necessary to obey a law you consider unjust. (75.04% adopt the rule of law position [disagree]; loading=.59) Sometimes it might be better to ignore the law and solve problems immediately rather than wait for a legal solution. (56.96% adopt the rule of law position [disagree]; loading=.62) The government should have some ability to bend the law in order to solve pressing social and political problems. (45.41% adopt the rule of law position [disagree]; loading=.57) It is not necessary to obey the laws of a government I did not vote for. (83.65% adopt the rule of law position [disagree]; loading=.61) When it comes right down to it, law is not all that important; what's important is that our government solve society's problems and make us all better off. (66.54% adopt the rule of law position [disagree]; loading=.68) Common factor analysis indicates that the measure is unidimensional (1st eigenvalue=1.89; 2nd eigenvalue=0.14). Political Intolerance The final core democratic value we measured was respondents’ tolerance for the ability of minority groups to advocate their views. To this end, we queried respondents in January 2012 about their willingness to allow unpopular groups to hold a public demonstration. The question stem read: Next, let's suppose that [THE GROUP] wanted to hold public rallies and demonstrations in your community to advance their cause, but that the authorities decided to prohibit it. How would you react to such a ban by the authorities of a public demonstration by [THE GROUP]? Would you strongly support the ban, support the ban, oppose the ban, or strongly oppose the ban" The groups about which we asked are: Radical Muslims (48.13% support the ban; loading=.75) A group of people who are against all churches and religion (36.26% support the ban; loading=.72) U.S. Communists (41.12% support the ban; loading=.80) Religious Fundamentalists (27.39% support the ban; loading=.66) Common factor analysis indicates that the measure is unidimensional (1st eigenvalue=2.14; 2nd eigenvalue=-0.06). Political Knowledge The measure of political knowledge is an additive scale of the number of correct responses to the following eight questions: Do you happen to know whether the justices of the U.S. Supreme Court serve for a set number of years or whether they serve a life term? Do you happen to know who has the last say when there is a conflict over the meaning of the Constitution? Which one of the parties is more conservative than the other at the national level? Is it the democrats or the Republicans? Which party holds a majority of seats in the U.S. House of Representatives? Which party holds a majority of seats in the U.S. Senate? How many decisions with opinions does the Court issue each year? Would you say that it is less than one hundred decisions with opinions each year, around five hundred decisions with opinions, or a thousand decisions with opinions or more per year? How long is one term for a member of the U.S. Senate? On which of the following federal programs is the most money spent each year? Aid to foreign countries, Medicare, subsidies to farmers, or education? Appendix B: Robustness and Additional Analyses Additional Predictors of Diffuse Support One of the key points in the debate between Bartels and Johnston (2013) and Gibson and Nelson (2014) is the appropriate control variables to include when modeling the Supreme Court’s diffuse support. The models presented in Table 5 in the body of the paper omit many of the control variables, such as support for democratic values, that previous research has suggested are essential predictors of diffuse support for the Court. To this end, Table A2 replicates the models in Table 5 of the text, controlling for the battery of explanatory variables included in Nelson and Smith’s (2014) model of diffuse support. Note that the substantive conclusions from Table A2 are identical to those shown in Table 5, and many of the findings of previous research, such as the importance of democratic values and African American ethnicity in predicting diffuse support hold when including perceptions of decision-making style, as well. Additional Predictors of Cross-Sectional Perceived Styles The models presented in Table 3, which regress normative, prospective, and retrospective perceived styles on policy preferences, political knowledge, and ethnicity do not include a variety of demographic and political information about respondents that one might believe affects both policy preferences or political knowledge and perceptions of Supreme Court decisionmaking. To this end, Table A4 reestimates those three models, including respondents’ party identification, ideology, age, gender, income, and religiosity. The major findings from Table 3 still hold. There are no differences in respondents’ normative beliefs about judicial decisionmaking by party identification, policy preferences, or ideology, and respondents’ levels of political knowledge structure their perceptions of the Court’s decisionmaking. In Table 3, beliefs about the constitutionality of the individual mandate were a statistically significant predictor of retrospective beliefs; that is not the case in Table A3. Instead, we see that, once controlling for partisanship and ideology, this effect is seen through ideology. The one major difference between Table 3 and Table A3 is that the models in Table A3 suggest that Republicans and Democrats had different prospective beliefs about the extent to which the Court would be influenced by external forces when deciding the PPACA case. Differential Effects of Policy Preferences by Level of Knowledge Johnston, Hillygus, and Bartels (2014) have recently suggested that individuallevel policy preferences may have differential effects by level of political sophistication. To examine whether this is the case with regard to the public’s view of the Court’s decisionmaking practices, Table A4 reestimates the model in Table 3, interacting political knowledge and beliefs about health care constitutionality. There is no evidence of a statistically significant interactive effect. Alternate Measure of Support for Health Care Reform The measure of health care attitudes used throughout the paper is a dichotomous variable which indicates whether the respondent prospectively believed that the individual mandate of the PPACA. The advantage of this measure is that it is targeted and issue-specific; its disadvantage is that it does not measure overall attitudes toward health care reform. To this end, we replicate the analyses in Table 3 and Table 4 using an alternative measure of support for health care reform more broadly. Panelists were asked, “Do you support or oppose the federal health care plan that was enacted in 2010?” About 55% of respondents supported the federal health care plan. Since some individuals opposed the plan because it went too far while others opposed it because it did not go far enough to provide coverage to Americans. To this end, we asked opponents of the whether they opposed the health care plan because it does not go far enough to extend health care coverage (Liberal Opponents) or because it goes too far in creating a new plan (Conservative Opponents). 3.40 percent of respondents were liberal opponents to the law, while 42.1 percent of respondents were classified as conservative opponents of the health care plan. Table A5 reports the reestimated models. The first three columns of the table replicate the models in Table 3 and the final two columns replicate the models in Table 4. The results are robust. Policy preferences continue to demonstrably shape respondents’ retrospective views, both in the cross-section and while controlling for respondents’ prospective views. Likewise ethnicity shapes normative and prospective views in the cross-section, but not after controlling for prospective views. Finally, political knowledge exerts a statistically significant effect in all of the models, indicating that, as knowledge increases, attitudes toward legal realism decrease. Table A1. Factor Analysis Factor Loadings. Normative Prospective Retrospective Personal Views 0.63 0.63 0.58 Analysis of Law -0.32 -0.12 -0.25 Avg. Americans 0.66 0.44 0.33 Public's Response 0.74 0.57 0.49 Lib/Cons Views 0.70 0.65 0.56 Appointing POTUS 0.72 0.68 0.66 Obama's Views 0.70 0.62 0.58 Table A2. Expanded Legitimacy Model Legitimacy at t1 Change in Perceived Style (1) 0.62* (0.06) -0.11* (0.03) Normative Evaluations Retrospective Evaluations HC is Unconstitutional Political Knowledge Supreme Court Approval Party Identification Support for Minority Liberty Support for Rule of Law Political Tolerance Ideology Age African American Hispanic Female Income Religiosity Constant R2 Adjusted R2 N -0.22* (0.09) 0.08* (0.02) -0.01 (0.09) -0.02 (0.02) 0.20* (0.04) 0.05 (0.03) 0.10* (0.05) -0.01 (0.02) -0.00 (0.00) 0.22* (0.08) -0.07 (0.10) -0.01 (0.06) 0.01 (0.01) -0.04 (0.03) 1.44* (0.26) 0.57 0.56 1712 (2) 0.57* (0.07) -0.09 (0.05) -0.19* (0.05) -0.21* (0.09) 0.07* (0.02) -0.02 (0.09) -0.03 (0.02) 0.18* (0.04) 0.04 (0.03) 0.11* (0.05) -0.01 (0.02) -0.00 (0.00) 0.30* (0.10) -0.07 (0.10) -0.02 (0.06) 0.01 (0.01) -0.05 (0.03) 1.75* (0.27) 0.58 0.57 1712 Table A3. Cross-sectional models, controlling for partisanship and ideology HC is Unconstitutional Political Knowledge African American Hispanic Party Identification Ideology Age Female Income Religiosity Constant R2 Adjusted R2 N (1) Normative 0.09 (0.06) -0.18* (0.02) 0.75* (0.11) -0.26* (0.09) -0.05 (0.03) 0.05 (0.04) -0.00 (0.00) -0.11 (0.07) -0.04* (0.01) -0.03 (0.04) 1.54* (0.17) 0.30 0.30 1417 (2) Prospective 0.08 (0.07) -0.07* (0.02) 0.25* (0.12) 0.14 (0.12) -0.05* (0.02) 0.03 (0.03) -0.00 (0.00) 0.09 (0.05) -0.01 (0.01) -0.03 (0.04) 0.41* (0.15) 0.08 0.07 1417 (3) Retrospective 0.04 (0.08) -0.06* (0.02) 0.14 (0.14) 0.01 (0.07) 0.04 (0.02) 0.03* (0.02) -0.00 (0.00) -0.04 (0.06) -0.01 (0.01) -0.05 (0.05) 0.17 (0.16) 0.07 0.06 1712 Table A4. Cross-sectional models, interacting knowledge and health care attitudes (1) (2) (3) Normative Prospective Retrospective HC is Unconstitutional 0.00 0.22 -0.01 (0.17) (0.15) (0.12) Political Knowledge -0.22* -0.07* -0.09* (0.03) (0.02) (0.03) Knowledge X Health Care 0.02 -0.04 0.04 (0.04) (0.03) (0.03) African American 0.91* 0.34* 0.13 (0.10) (0.12) (0.11) Hispanic -0.36* 0.07 0.02 (0.09) (0.10) (0.06) Constant 1.42* 0.26* 0.34* (0.17) (0.12) (0.14) R2 0.27 0.06 0.04 Adjusted R2 0.27 0.06 0.04 N 1417 1417 1712 Table A5. Alternate measure of health care support. Liberal Opponents Conservative Opponents Political Knowledge African American Hispanic (1) Normative 0.07 (0.25) -0.09 (0.05) -0.21* (0.02) 0.86* (0.10) -0.32* (0.10) (2) Prospective -0.02 (0.18) -0.07 (0.10) -0.09* (0.02) 0.31* (0.12) 0.09 (0.12) (3) Retrospective 0.31 (0.17) 0.21* (0.07) -0.07* (0.02) 0.19 (0.10) -0.02 (0.07) Prospective Views (4) Retrospective 0.30 (0.16) 0.20* (0.06) -0.05* (0.02) 0.21* (0.05) Party Identification Ideology Constant R2 Adjusted R2 N 1.45* (0.17) 0.27 0.27 1417 0.45* (0.10) 0.06 0.06 1417 0.26* (0.10) 0.05 0.05 1712 0.19 (0.12) 0.09 0.09 1712 (5) Retrospective 0.23 (0.17) 0.10 (0.11) -0.05* (0.02) 0.18 (0.09) -0.05 (0.07) 0.20* (0.05) 0.04 (0.03) 0.03 (0.02) -0.01 (0.15) 0.11 0.11 1712