Day 4 Lecture notes – Reading The Nature of Cognitive Models Cognitive models delineate the stages involved in any mental activity. Take object naming. In order to do this we have to go through a number of stages. We have to see/recognise the object, we have to understand it, we have to access the spoken word form and then articulate the word. These stages can be expressed in terms of a simple model: The model is ‘true’ in as much as it accounts for observed patterns of behaviour. For example we all occasionally forget words (particularly the names of people). Often we know all about the meaning of the word (eg we can describe the person, what they do, where they come from etc), but cannot think of the phonology. This is consistent with the above model. It suggests that we have processed as far as semantics, but can’t get to phonology. People with language problems following stroke (aphasia) experience such word finding failures much more frequently than people with intact language. Nevertheless, we can still see their difficulties as a derailment of the normal stages of word retrieval. Thinking about where the chain of processing is breaking down should give us new insights into their problems and help us to select an appropriate therapy. When cognitive models were first formulated little attempt was made to relate them to brain structures. It was stressed that models were purely abstract representations of mental functioning. With the advent of more sophisticated imaging techniques, there has been a greater attempt to relate the processes delineated in the models to actual brain regions (eg see Hillis and Tuffiash 2002). Universality One important assumption about models is that they are basically the same from person to person. In other words, my language system works along the same principles as yours. This means that we can use evidence from individuals to draw conclusions about how humans generally process language. Despite this, there is a lot of controversy about the structure of models and a number of different architectures have been proposed. The model described in these lectures is just one of many options, although it is one that is widely applied in aphasia therapy. Modularity and the Effects of Brain Injury on Cognitive Models The model introduced here will consist of a number of processing components, or modules, each of which is responsible for a particular task. This is an important principle. If reading is accomplished by different modules brain injury may result in some modules being impaired and others still being intact. In other words, a deficit need not be all or nothing. This is good news for therapists, in that intact modules offer potential resources for improvement. It also means that one of our tasks, as therapists, is to find out which components of the system are impaired and which (relatively) intact. A Simple Reading Model What processes are involved in reading aloud a single word? Clearly the first stage is to analyse the individual letters and their order -the 'Visual Analysis System'. This system must be able to recognise different letters regardless of handwriting, font or case. This suggests that the system deals in abstract letter identities, rather than set visual templates. Having worked out the letter string, the reader has two options available. One is to read the word aloud using 'phonics', the other is to use a whole word approach. Let’s take 'phonics' first. This method converts individual letters or groups of letters into sounds. This has been termed 'Grapheme to Phoneme Conversion' and is shown on the model as one of the possible reading routes (The GPC route is also sometimes termed the sub-lexical route or Orthography to Phonology Conversion). This route has to do a number of things. First of all it has to break up the word into graphemes: ship = sh i p Then each grapheme has to be linked to a phoneme: sh /∫/ i // p /p/ Then these phonemes have to be blended: /∫p/. Supposing this was the only reading route available. You will immediately begin to see problems. Firstly, the route is dependent upon regular correspondences between print and sound, eg that 't' is always read aloud as /t/, ‘ch’ as /t∫/. Yet many English words flout these correspondences. Think about 'mortgage' and 'yacht', and compare 'hint' and 'pint'. Such irregular words cannot be handled by the GPC route. A second problem: if this were the only reading route available all words would have to be understood via their phonology. What about homophones? These are words which share the same pronunciations but have different meanings (week/weak, gait/gate, air/heir, pain/pane etc). If we only read via the GPC route we could not tell the difference between these words. The GPC route has one strength. This is the only route that can handle novel or non words. Thus we can use this route to assemble a probable pronunciation for 'brap', even though we have never encountered this word before. Reading via the lexical route Here the product of the Visual Analysis is fed to the Visual Input Lexicon. This might be imagined as a store of all known words. If the letter string corresponds to an entry in the store, the word is recognised. Thus this system differentiates between familiar words and unfamiliar ones (such as foreign words or non words). The Visual Input Lexicon does not store word meanings. These are processed at the next Semantic stage. There is huge controversy about the nature of the Semantic (or Cognitive) system. This will be revisited later in the course. For now, we will say that the system contains the features that make up word meanings. Thus the word 'cat' would access the following features: animal, domestic, fur, tail, whiskers, purr, miaow, etc. Once the word has been processed by the semantic system, it is understood. Of course, processing often ends here. However, if our aim is to read the word aloud we have to access its phonology from the next module: The Phonological Output Lexicon (or Speech Output Lexicon). This is another store, this time of whole word phonologies. A number of commentators suggest that this lexicon only contains the roots of words (morphological endings being added by another system). Our model has a 'Response Buffer' after the POL. This can be imagined as a store that holds onto the word until it is needed in the speech stream. It is also reasonable to assume that quite a lot of processing is involved in converting the phonological form of a word into a phonetic/motor programme. However, this remains unspecified in the model. Reading Aloud Via Meaning? - The Direct Lexical Route There is evidence that normal readers access meaning while reading text aloud. For example, Morton (cited in Ellis 1993) noted that skilled readers tended to make semantic errors when reading passages aloud, ie they might read 'Saturday' as 'Sunday'. These errors could only occur if reading was being mediated by the semantic system. There are also a number of words that can only be read aloud correctly via the semantic system. For example, in order to pronounce ‘bow’ correctly we need to know whether we are talking about a bow and arrow or a person taking a bow. Is it possible to read aloud without accessing meaning? Two sources of evidence suggests that it is. One comes from people with brain damage. Schwartz, Saffran and Marin (1980) describe a woman with dementia with a severe deficit in her semantic system. This person was profoundly confused and could not understand either spoken or written words. However, she could read aloud well. Crucially, this skill extended to both regular and irregular words, which confirmed that she was not simply converting written words to sound using the GPC route (see example below). The only way of explaining her performance was to argue for a third reading route directly connecting the Visual Input Lexicon with the Phonological Output Lexicon. Frequency/ Familiarity Effects Some words are used a lot (man, car), others more rarely (ram, elk). There are two main measures of this. One is frequency, which is a count of how often a word appears in a range of written texts or spoken corpora. The other is familiarity, which is derived by asking subjects to rate how familiar they feel a word is. Lexical frequency effects are very powerful and pervasive. For example, high frequency words are read aloud more rapidly than low frequency ones and stimulate faster responses in lexical decision. It seems that the lexicon has an intrinsic bias in favour of common words. This has been explained by arguing that word entries (eg in the VIL) have different resting levels of activation, according to their frequency. So a common word like ‘man’ has quite a high resting level of activation, while an uncommon one like ‘elk’ has a very low resting level. This would mean that ‘man’ only requires a little additional input before it fires (or becomes available), whereas 'elk', would need considerably more. Semantic Priming Several experiments show that reading is facilitated by a meaningful context. For example, people might be shown the beginning of a sentence followed by a very rapidly presented closing word. The exposure time needed to recognise the closing word was measured. Closure words might be consistent with the preceding context, neutral or unexpected. Unsurprisingly, subjects needed the least exposure time for the consistent words (see Ellis 1993). Semantic priming is an example of 'top down' processing. 'Top down', is confusing because, on our model it looks more like 'bottom up'. The term is used because a higher order of cognitive processing. Let’s imagine that related words are somehow linked together in the semantic system – possibly because they share semantic features. Thus the word 'dog' is linked to 'cat', 'lead', 'kennel' etc. Seeing the word 'dog' partially activates these related words. The feedback route between the semantic system and VIL primes the relevant entries in the input lexicon. As a result, recognition of one of these entries in a lexical decision task is facilitated. Imageability effects Imageable words can be pictured, while low imageability words cannot (as this implies, imageability correlates very highly with concreteness). Highly imageable/ concrete words (such as saucepan and tart) stimulate faster responses in lexical decision than low imageability/abstract words (such as democracy & idea). Synonym judgements, in which the person has to judge whether two words are similar in meaning show the same trend. These effects suggest that processing abstract meanings may be intrinsically more difficult than processing concrete meanings. Problems in Visual Analysis Reading difficulties may arise from damage to the system that analyses the letter string (Visual Analysis). If this is the case, the person may be unable to carry out even very basic tasks, such as cross case matching: Are these letters the same? R- r; B- d or matching letter strings: Are these the same? BDAQ – bdaq; HMYG - hmyg We would also expect visual errors in reading, eg LEND read as ‘land’. Difficulties in visual analysis may arise from a more general impairment, such as visual hemispatial neglect. Here the person seems unaware of information presented on one side of space. Neglect most commonly arises after RH damage, therefore neglect dyslexia usually affects the left side of words. (Cases of Neglect Dyslexia in the literature: Ellis, Flude and Young 1987; Hillis and Caramazza 1995). Here are some examples of reading errors made by people with left neglect dyslexia target word never elate willow read as lever plate pillow sad sadness sad badness n e v e r never Note that the person seems to be aware that there is a letter on the left side of the word, but is not sure of its identity. In the ‘sad/sadness’ example, the addition of a suffix has ‘pushed’ the beginning of the word further to the left, which brings about the reading error. In the final example the word is presented vertically. Now there are no left-most letters, so the word is read correctly. Problems in the Visual Input Lexicon (Example in the literature: Coltheart et al 1983) What would happen if the person could no longer access entries in the VIL? This would mean that they could not use the lexical reading routes (ie the semantic route &/or the direct lexical route). However, if this is their only problem, they could still use the GPC. How would this manifest? Regular words are read better than irregular (exception) words The GPC assembles a pronunciation for a word by using regular letter to sound correspondences. This works well for regular words, but much less well for irregular words (like ‘yacht’). A reader of English, therefore, will be quite disadvantaged if they can only use GPC. They may make regularisation errors with irregular words – eg reading ‘yacht’ as ‘yakt’. Transparent languages, like Spanish and Urdu, have few irregular words. So GPC readers of these languages will be better off. Problems with Lexical Decision The person is shown written words and non words and has to judge which are real words. The task requires access to the VIL, so will be impaired. If regular and irregular words are tested – the person may show an advantage for regular items. Why? They can read these items via GPC, then judge them by using auditory processing. The tester could be sneaky and include non words which, when pronounced, sound like real words (such as fone). These will be judged (incorrectly) as being real words. Words are understood by reading them aloud The person assembles a pronunciation (via GPC), listens to that pronunciation then comprehends the word. Therefore, if the person misreads the word (eg because it is irregular) they will also misunderstand it, eg: Target word bear listen Read aloud as beer Liston Definition a drink the boxer Homophones may be misunderstood English contains numerous homophones. These are pairs of words that are pronounced the same, but have different meanings. A GPC reader will have no means of telling the difference between these items, eg: Target word Read aloud as Definition pane pane something that hurts (pain) berry berry dig in the ground (bury) Non words can still be read The GPC is responsible for reading non-words. Therefore these will be read comparatively well. A word of caution Brain damage rarely results in absolute dissociations. Rather we are looking for trends. The following example is typical: reading regular words aloud reading irregular words aloud 30/39 18/39 This person clearly shows a regularity effect in their oral reading (ie regular >irregular). However, they are not perfect at reading regular words, and they have some ability to read irregular ones. The latter may indicate that some words can be accessed in the VIL – possibly because they are very frequent. Despite this, there is a clear over dependency on GPC reading. A Note about Terminology Dependency on the GPC for reading aloud is often termed Surface Dyslexia. Problems with Grapheme to Phoneme Conversion (Cases in the literature: Patterson 1982; Funnell 1983; Caccappolo van Vliet 2004) What would happen if GPC were impaired? If this is the only problem, many aspects of reading will still be intact, since the lexical reading routes are still available. As a result, the person will be able to recognise (VIL), understand (access Semantics) and read words aloud (via POL). However, they will not be able to read non words, as this can only be accomplished by GPC. They might try to read these items by accessing a similar word in the VIL. This will result in lexicalisation errors, eg: Target Soof Read aloud as soot This form of reading impairment is termed ‘Phonological Dyslexia’. A problem in semantics (Cases in the literature: Hillis et al 1990; Hillis and Caramazza 1991) What would happen if the semantic system were impaired? We would expect difficulties in any task which requires the person to access word meanings. Furthermore, in our model there is just one semantic system serving all modalities of language. Therefore we would expect both written and spoken words to be poorly understood and we would anticipate problems in production. Semantic damage is rarely total. Rather, the person often retains a partial knowledge of word meaning. For example, if they read the word ‘dog’ they may know that this is an animal, but be unable to access more specific semantic features. This person will make semantic errors in comprehension testing, eg where the word ‘dog’ is matched to a picture of a cat (but not to an unrelated item). Performance in reading aloud will depend upon what other reading mechanisms are available. Suppose neither the GPC nor the direct route is available, because they have also been damaged. This means that the person will have to read via their damaged semantic system. We would expect the following pattern of performance: the person will make semantic errors in reading aloud (ie reading ‘dog’ as ‘cat’) reading will be affected by semantic factors; in particular we would expect concrete words to be read better than abstract words reading will be affected by grammatical class: eg nouns>verbs (because these classes differ in abstractness); function words will be poorly read non word reading will be impossible (as the GPC is unavailable) This pattern of performance has been termed Deep Dyslexia. A Problem in the Phonological Output Lexicon (Cases in the literature: Kay and Patterson 1985; Caramazza and Hillis 1990) Reading aloud may be impaired by a deficit in the POL. How this manifests will, again, depend upon what other reading mechanisms are available. Take the case of EST (Kay and Patterson, 1985). Here are some of his test results: non words were read quite well (67/80) regular words were read aloud better than irregular words regularisation errors occurred lexical decision was good EST could often define irregular words, even though he could not read them aloud, eg: thyme it grows and smells nicely choir singing He could also often match an irregular word to the correct definition: gauge the distance between rails of a railway track a deep valley with steep sides (gorge) dig something out with your hands (gouge) What is the story? EST can access the VIL (hence the good lexical decision). He can also access meaning from written words (hence good performance on the definition tasks). His reading aloud, therefore, is impaired by access to the POL. GPC mechanisms are still available (hence good non word reading). If the word is regular EST can therefore use this to assemble a pronunciation (hence the regularity effect in reading aloud). EST shows that people can depend on GPC reading for different reasons. Some people may be unable to access the VIL, while others may have problems at the level of POL. Letter By Letter Reading (Pure Alexia, Alexia without Dysgraphia) This reading impairment is difficult to classify. The person can only read aloud by first naming the letters, eg: horse ‘aitch .. oh .. are .. ess ..ee … horse’ Unsurprisingly, letter by letter readers tend to be very slow at reading and show powerful effects of word length (short words > long words). Letter by letter readers can still write. This lead to the proposal that they are reading via the spelling mechanism. In other words, they name the letters as if they are spelling the word and this enables them to identify it. Can our model account for letter by letter reading? Clearly some visual analysis is taking place (given that the person can name the letters). However, access to the VIL seems blocked. We also have to assume that the GPC cannot be used. Some letter by letter readers seem able to carry out lexical decision with words if they are presented rapidly, ie they are flashed up briefly on a screen (Coslett and Saffran 1989). This suggests that there may be some covert skills which do not emerge in normal testing. These skills have also been exploited in therapy (Friedman and Lott 2000).