PAM A. MUELLER Psychology Department, Princeton University Princeton, NJ 08540 847-644-2466 pamuelle@princeton.edu http://lapa.princeton.edu/peopledetail.php?ID=667 EDUCATION Princeton University Ph.D., Social Psychology, expected 2015; M.A., 2012 Dissertation: Mind Perception and Morality in the Justice System Program in Law and Public Affairs, Graduate Associate Harvard Law School J.D., cum laude, 2008 Thesis: Legacies in an Ephemeral Art Harvard Law & Policy Review, Student Writing Editor Harvard Law School Democrats, Secretary Loyola University Chicago B.S., Psychology, summa cum laude, Honors Program Degree, 2002 Thesis: The Effect of Modality on Eyewitness Memory Phi Beta Kappa Minor in French language and literature PUBLICATIONS Mueller, P.A., & Oppenheimer, D.M. (in press). The pen is mightier than the keyboard: advantages of longhand over laptop note-taking. Psychological Science. Taking notes on laptops rather than by longhand is increasingly common. Many researchers have suggested that laptop note-taking is less effective for learning. Prior studies have primarily focused on laptops' capacity for multitasking and distraction. The current research suggests that even when distractions are eliminated, laptops still may be impairing learning. In three studies, we find that students who took notes on laptops performed worse on various assessment questions than students who took notes longhand. We show that while taking more notes can be beneficial, laptop note-takers' tendency to transcribe lectures verbatim rather than processing information and reframing it in their own words is detrimental. Chandler, J.J, Mueller, P.A., & Paolacci, G. (2013). Nonnaïveté among Amazon Mechanical Turk workers: consequences and solutions for behavioral researchers. Behavior Research Methods, 10.3758/s13428-013-0365-7 Crowdsourcing services – particularly Amazon Mechanical Turk – have made it easy for behavioral scientists to recruit research participants. However, researchers have overlooked crucial differences between crowdsourced and traditionally recruited participants that provide unique opportunities and challenges. We show that crowdsourced workers are likely to participate across multiple related experiments, and researchers are overzealous in the exclusion of research participants. We describe how both of these problems can be avoided using advanced interface features that also allow prescreening and longitudinal data collection. Using these techniques can minimize the effects of previously ignored drawbacks and expand the scope of crowdsourcing as a tool for psychological research. Chandler, J.J., Paolacci, G., & Mueller, P.A. (in press). Risks and rewards of crowdsourcing marketplaces. In Pietro Michelucci (Ed.), Handbook of Human Computation, New York: Springer. This chapter focuses on the strengths and limitations of crowdsourcing marketplaces, with an emphasis on the quality of crowdsourced data. We describe general considerations and provide specific recommendations for measuring and improving data quality, which are applicable across crowdsourcing markets. Shapiro, D.N., Chandler, J.J., & Mueller, P.A. (2013). Using Mechanical Turk to study clinical populations. Clinical Psychological Science, 1(2), 213-220. Participants with psychiatric symptoms, specific risk factors, or rare demographic characteristics are sometimes crucial for research in the social, behavioral, and clinical sciences. No research to date has examined the utility of crowdsourcing software for conducting research on psychopathology. We examine the prevalence of several psychiatric disorders and related problems, as well as the reliability and validity of participant reports on these domains, among users of Amazon’s Mechanical Turk. Findings suggest that crowdsourcing software offers several advantages for clinical research while providing insight into potential problems, such as misrepresentation, that researchers should address when collecting data online. Mueller, P.A., Solan, L.M., & Darley, J.M. (2012). When does knowledge become intent?: Perceiving the minds of wrongdoers. Journal of Empirical Legal Studies, 9, 859-892. In a series of experimental studies, we asked people to assign appropriate liability to individuals who cause harm with various culpable states of mind and kinds of knowledge. The studies are principally aimed at two related issues. First, do people actually separate the various states of mind conceptually? How much knowledge, and what kind of knowledge, regarding something that may go wrong (understanding risk) is sufficient to count as knowing that something will go wrong (having knowledge legally equivalent to intent)? Second, to the extent that people distinguish among the states of mind that help define normative behavior, how much do those distinctions contribute to people’s judgments of civil liability? Findings suggest that people are able to make explicit distinctions about the states of mind of others that more or less correspond to legally relevant categories. However, when asked to assign consequences, their “hot” moral judgments play a larger role than do their “cold” cognitive categorizations. Diamond, S.S. & Mueller, P.A. (2010). Empirical legal scholarship in law reviews. Annual Review of Law and Social Science, 6, 581-599. Despite persistent calls for more empirical legal scholarship, only recently have scholars provided evidence that empirical legal scholarship has indeed entered the mainstream of the legal academy. We analyzed the content of 60 law review volumes published in the years 1998 and 2008, finding that by 2008 nearly half of law review articles included some empirical content. Production of original empirical research, however, is less common. The highest-ranked law reviews published more original empirical articles and included more complex research designs. Analyzing the benefits and costs of publishing in law reviews, we predict that law reviews will see more original empirical scholarship in the future, despite the increased availability of peer-reviewed publication outlets. Diamond, S.S., & Mueller, P. (2008). Jury decisions versus judges' decisions. In B. Cutler (Ed.), Encyclopedia of psychology and law. (pp. 406-409). Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE Publications. In American trials, the verdict is reached by either a judge or a jury, raising questions as to how these two fact finders reach their decisions and whether their decisions systematically differ. Most research has focused on the jury, though some key studies have compared the decisions of judges and juries. The available archival studies, case-specific judicial surveys, and experimental research reveal substantial similarities and a few differences. WORK UNDER REVIEW AND WORKING PAPERS Mueller, P.A., & Fiske, S.T. Mind perception and morality in the justice system. One common view is that the role of morality in the justice system is to differentiate punishment from compensation, or more generally, criminal consequences from civil consequences. We find that morality plays a more nuanced role in laypeople’s legal judgments. The moral character of the harmdoer influences blame and responsibility judgments in criminal cases, while the moral character of the victim influences blame and responsibility judgments in civil cases. These differences in blame are partially accounted for by differences in perceived agency of harmdoers and victims. Mueller, P.A. Immorality and inchoate crimes. The majority standard for attempt crimes – “substantial step” – does not comport with laypeople’s intuitions about criminalization or immoral behavior. This empirical law review piece argues for the implementation of the minority “dangerous proximity” standard, based on original research that 1) replicates Robinson & Darley’s finding about lay intuitions, 2) shows that the substantial step standard is applied less consistently than the minority “dangerous proximity” standard, leading to capricious and unjust outcomes, and 3) demonstrates that the harmdoer’s moral character is more likely to be inappropriately considered when the substantial step standard is applied. Mueller, P.A., Fiske, S.T., Solan, L.M., & Darley, J.M. Intentionality and institutional choice. We investigate laypeople’s intuitions about the appropriateness of criminal and civil consequences for harms committed with different levels of mens rea, in order to determine whether criminalizing unintentional behavior comports with laypeople’s beliefs about what is just. In three studies, we find that civil consequences are seen as more just than criminal consequences for all harms other than those committed truly intentionally. mens rea. We then explore whether these lay beliefs about the appropriate relationship between criminal penalties and intentional harms lead people to perceive harm-doers who face criminal consequences as having acted more intentionally than those who face civil consequences. We find that people do perceive harm-doers facing criminal charges to have acted more intentionally, though this effect is qualified by their initial perception of the justness of the consequence. Mueller, P.A., & Pronin, E. Self-other asymmetries in blind evaluation. Blind evaluation (e.g. blind grading, auditions, etc.) removes the potential for bias in many situations, but it also may hide useful information. In the legal context, for instance, some information may be both probative and prejudicial. We find that people are most likely to think that blind evaluation will improve the judgments of individuals making other kinds of decisions. They are somewhat less likely to think that it will improve decisions for other people making the same decision that they are, and are least likely to think that it will improve their own decision. The difference in their perception of the utility of blind evaluation for themselves versus others making their same decision is driven by the bias blind spot; that is, they believe that blind evaluation will reduce bias more for others than for themselves. On the other hand, the difference in the perception of the effectiveness of blind evaluation for others making the same decision as they are and those making a different decision is driven by the fact that they believe blind evaluation is hiding useful information from their group, but not from the other group. Mueller, P.A., & Pronin, E. The bias blind spot in legal decision making. Individuals are more likely to see the existence and operation of both cognitive and motivational biases in others than in themselves. This tendency has ramifications for decisions made throughout the justice system. This article traces cases through the legal system, and explores situations in which the bias blind spot is likely to be an important consideration. We then suggest specific measures that can be taken to counteract the bias blind spot in these situations, beyond simply making people aware of their biases. Mueller, P.A. Legacies in an ephemeral art. Choreography is one of the most ephemeral arts. As such, choreography has faced significant challenges when issues of intellectual property have arisen. Because of these challenges, the dance community has, until recently, depended largely on internal good will and self-policing for their dispute resolution. Drawing on both psychology and law, I argue that it is important for choreographers to work within the legal system, and provide suggestions for how to protect these intensely personal artistic legacies. SELECTED CONFERENCE PRESENTATIONS AND INVITED TALKS Social Psychology and Law Pre-Conference, SPSP Annual Meeting, Austin, 2014 Mind Perception and Morality in the Justice System Association for Consumer Research Conference; Chicago, 2013 Incentivizing Mechanical Turk Workers’ Attention ACM Conference on Electronic Commerce, Crowdsourcing and Online Behavioral Experiments Workshop; University of Pennsylvania, 2013 Non-Naïveté Among Amazon Mechanical Turk Workers Four College Conference; Princeton University, 2013 The Pen Is Mightier Than the Keyboard: Laptop and Longhand Note-Taking Society for Personality and Social Psychology Annual Meeting and Social Psychology and Law Pre-Conference; New Orleans, 2013 (poster presentation) Is Strict Liability Too Strict?: Lay Intuitions about Intentionality and Institutional Choice Society for Personality and Social Psychology Annual Meeting; San Diego, 2012 (symposium speaker and co-chair) Advanced Uses of Mechanical Turk Crowdsourcing in Psychological Research Conference on Empirical Legal Studies; Northwestern University, 2011 When Does Knowledge Become Intent?: Perceiving the Minds of Wrongdoers Philosophy, Linguistics, and Law Series; Brooklyn Law School When Does Knowledge Become Intent? MERG Experimental Philosophy Conference; New York University From Risk to Knowledge, from Knowledge to Intent: How People Perceive the Minds of Wrongdoers AWARDS AND FELLOWSHIPS Fellowship of Woodrow Wilson Scholars, 2013 – present (awarded to selected doctoral students engaged in interdisciplinary research relevant to public policy) Society for Personality and Social Psychology Graduate Student Travel Award, 2012. Post-Graduate Research Fellowship, Harvard Law School, 2008 – 2011. Eugene B. Zechmeister Prize (awarded to each year’s outstanding psychology scholar), Loyola University Chicago, 2002. TEACHING EXPERIENCE Assistant in Instruction/Lab Instructor Psychology 101, Princeton University (Fall 2011 – Prof. Daniel Oppenheimer) Psychology 101, Princeton University (Fall 2012 – Prof. Yarrow Dunham) Psychology 101, Princeton University (Spring 2013 – Prof. Nicholas Turk-Browne) LEGAL EXPERIENCE Foley and Lardner, LLP (Chicago and Los Angeles, 2008-2009) Attorney – Copyright, Trademark, and Advertising Department Researched, wrote, and filed responses to trademark office actions Drafted and filed complaints, responses and various motions for trademark and copyright infringement cases Quinn, Emanuel, Urquhart, Oliver, and Hedges, LLP (Summer 2007) Summer Associate Attorney Researched and wrote memoranda on trademark, antitrust, and securities issues; drafted motion to exclude Drafted memorandum and wrote deposition questions on key issue in a significant trademark case Lar Lubovitch Dance Company (Summer 2006) Summer Associate – Copyright and Licensing Registered copyrights for choreography created under different copyright statutes over a 40-year period Investigated issues related to past licensing of choreography, as well as music licensing for dance performance videos OTHER EMPLOYMENT Northwestern University School of Law (2009-2010) Research Associate – Professors Kenworthey Bilz, Shari Diamond, & Janice Nadler Congressman Jerry Kleczka, D-WI (2004-2005) Communications Director; Appropriations Aide Dean for America, Burlington, VT/Falls Church, VA (2003-2004) Press Secretary, State of Virginia PROFESSIONAL MEMBERSHIPS Society for Empirical Legal Studies, Association for Psychological Science, Society for Personality and Social Psychology, American Psychology-Law Society Admitted to the Illinois Bar (2008)