From social cognition to social phenomena CogSci: core course Winter term, 2014 Course description What are the psychological bases of the rich social interactions and cultural life that characterise human societies? This course will review some of the answers provided by recent studies in cognitive psychology, evolutionary psychology and social anthropology. It will cover a wide range of topics related to social cognition and human sociality, including: Mind reading Naive sociology Communication, social learning, imitation The biological evolution of social cognitive capacities Models of Man in the social sciences The cultural diversity of human psychology How human psychology constrains culture Models of cultural evolution Co-operation and moral cognition (Note that some key themes will be omitted. Joint action, for instance, has been taught in N. Sebanz and G. Knoblich’s research course. Mind-reading is a key ability that ground most aspects of our social life, but it will be dealt more thoroughly in D. Samson’s elective course. I have also included no brain studies). The course is structured in three parts that focus on different aspects of social cognition and human sociality. The first parts is focused on the social cognitive skills that humans have. It will include sessions on mind-reading, social perception and naïve sociology, and the biological evolution of social cognition. The two last parts are focused on culture and cognition. The second part will review social scientists’ take on human psychology and how it influences their understanding of social phenomena. The third part will deal with specific themes in cognition and culture: morality, religion and science. Course convenor Christophe Heintz Office hours Meeting by appointment. Students are also welcome just to pop in the office. http://www.ceu.hu/profiles/faculty/christophe_heintz Goal of the course Students will be presented with up to date research on key issues in the study of social cognition. Even though the course could not consist of a comprehensive review of current research on social cognition, themes have been chosen so as cover the main issues and illuminate what are the stakes of the research field. 1 The second goal of the course is to open up psychology students to basic questions in the social science and reflect on the actual and potential contributions of social sciences and cognitive psychology to each other. Learning outcome Students will learn about the theories specifying the cognitive bases of human sociality. They will reflect on the empirical evidence--from comparative psychology, developmental psychology, neuroscience, experimental psychology and social anthropology--that ground these theories. Students will also become aware of the diversity of assumptions about human psychology in the social science and have a better grasp of what social scientists have said about human sociality. Requirements: All students must read the core reading before the seminars. Students are expected to contribute to class discussion and should have ready, each week, at least one question based on the texts and that could be fruitfully addressed during class discussion. Each student will present a set of papers to the class and will participate to a ‘debate’ (see week 2 session). For individual presentations, I encourage preparing a handout that summarizes the goals of the papers, their main arguments and the method and evidence they rely on. For participation to debates, relatively comprehensive reviews of the arguments and empirical evidence in favour of a given position will have to be presented and defended. Registered students must submit a short essay of no more than 2,000 words at the end of the term. Students will decide on the topic of the essay in agreement with me. First year PhD students will be encouraged to focus on the social and cultural aspects of their chosen PhD topics. This could mean questioning the potential cultural variability of the cognitive mechanisms to be investigated, questioning how the type of behavioural effects to be investigated participate to the shaping of some social phenomena, questioning societal implications of the student’s research project (political and organisational implications, potential applications in cognitive ergonomy). Grades will be awarded as follows: Final essay 40% Paper presentation and debates 40 % Participation 20% 1. Capacities for navigating our social world Week 1 Theories of mind-reading: - The intentional stance (Dennett) - Theory theory (Gopnik) - Simulation theory and the role mirror neurons (Goldman) - Core cognition (Leslie) 2 Core reading: Leslie, A. M., Friedman, O., & German, T. P. (2004). Core mechanisms in “theory of mind”. Trends in cognitive sciences, 8(12), 528–33. Other readings: Dennett, D. (2009). Intentional Systems Theory. In B. McLaughlin, A. Beckermann, & S. Walter (Eds.), Oxford Handbook of the Philosophy of Mind (pp. 339–50). Oxford University Press. Gopnik, A., & Wellman, H. (1992). Why the child’s theory of mind really is a theory. Mind & Language, 7(1-2), 145–171. Gallese, V., & Goldman, A. (1998). Mirror neurons and the mind-reading, 2(12), 493–501. Jacob, P. (2008). What Do Mirror Neurons Contribute to Human Social Cognition? Mind & Language, 23(2), 190–223. doi:10.1111/j.1468-0017.2007.00337.x Leslie, A.M. (2000) How to acquire a ‘representational theory of mind’ In D. Sperber (ed) Metarepresentations: A multidisciplinary perspective, OUP, 197—223. Week 2 Making sense of others: with or without representing intentions and beliefs? - Automatic vs. effortful mind reading - Enactive vs. ‘representational’ theories - Dealing with the developmental paradox During this week, a debate will be organised on early mind-reading abilities. Two groups of 3 students will be randomly assigned a position that they will have to defend: Position A: early mind-reading performances are not resulting from the capacity to represent others’ beliefs and intentions Position B: early mind-reading performances are resulting from the capacity to represent others’ beliefs and intentions With or without representing others’ thoughts Heyes, C. (2014). False belief in infancy : a fresh look. Developmental science, 1–13. doi:10.1111/desc.12148 Hutto, D. D., Herschbach, M., & Southgate, V. (2011). Editorial: Social Cognition: Mindreading and Alternatives. Review of Philosophy and Psychology, 2(3), 375–395. doi:10.1007/s13164-011-0073-0 Jacob, P. (2011). The Direct-Perception Model of Empathy: a Critique. Review of Philosophy and Psychology, 2(3), 519–540. doi:10.1007/s13164-011-0065-0 Two systems belief reasoning Apperly, I. a, & Butterfill, S. a. (2009). Do humans have two systems to track beliefs and belief-like states? Psychological review, 116(4), 953–70. doi:10.1037/a0016923 Early mind reading—attribution of goals Gergely, G., Nfidasdy, Z., Csibra, G., & Bfr, S. (1995). Taking the intentional stance at 12 months of age, 0277(95). Gergely, G., & Csibra, G. (2003). Teleological reasoning in infancy: the naïve theory of rational action, 7(7), 287–292. doi:10.1016/S1364-6613(03)00128-1 Woodward, a. (1998). Infants selectively encode the goal object of an actor’s reach. Cognition, 69(1), 1–34. Early mind reading—attribution of beliefs Onishi, K. H., & Baillargeon, R. (2005). Do 15-month-old infants understand false beliefs? Science, 308(5719), 255–8. doi:10.1126/science.1107621 Southgate, V., Chevallier, C., & Csibra, G. (2010). Seventeen-month-olds appeal to false beliefs to interpret others’ referential communication. Developmental science, 13(6), 907–12. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00946.x 3 Surian, L., Caldi, S., & Sperber, D. (2007). Attribution of beliefs by 13-month-old infants. Psychological science : a journal of the American Psychological Society / APS, 18(7), 580–6. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01943.x Southgate, V., Senju, A., & Csibra, G. (2007). Action anticipation through attribution of false belief by 2-year-olds. Psychological science, 18(7), 587–92. doi:10.1111/j.14679280.2007.01944.x Automaticity—perspective taking Samson, D., Apperly, I. a, Braithwaite, J. J., Andrews, B. J., & Bodley Scott, S. E. (2010). Seeing it their way: evidence for rapid and involuntary computation of what other people see. Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance, 36(5), 1255–66. doi:10.1037/a0018729 Automaticity—representing beliefs Apperly, I. a, Back, E., Samson, D., & France, L. (2008). The cost of thinking about false beliefs: evidence from adults’ performance on a non-inferential theory of mind task. Cognition, 106(3), 1093–108. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2007.05.005 Kovács, Á. M., Téglás, E., & Endress, A. D. (2010). The social sense: Susceptibility to others’ beliefs in human infants and adults. Science, 1830. doi:10.1126/science.1190792 Week 3 Comparative psychology and the evolution of social cognition - The Machiavellian intelligence hypothesis - Homologies and analogies for ToM. - The evolution of cooperative dispositions (a brief note only) Core reading: Moll, H., & Tomasello, M. (2007). Cooperation and human cognition: the Vygotskian intelligence hypothesis. Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences, 362(1480), 639–48. doi:10.1098/rstb.2006.2000 Others: Evolution of ToM Byrne, R., & Whiten, A. (1989). Machiavellian Intelligence : Social Expertise and the Evolution of Intellect in Monkeys, Apes, and Humans. Oxford Science Publications. Flinn, M. V., Geary, D. C., & Ward, C. V. (2005). Ecological dominance, social competition, and coalitionary arms races. Evolution and Human Behavior, 26(1), 10–46. doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2004.08.005 Homologies: primate’s ToM Povinelli, D. J., & Vonk, J. (2003). Chimpanzee minds : suspiciously human ?, 7(4), 157–160. doi:10.1016/S1364-6613(03)00053-6 Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2008). Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? 30 years later. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12(5), 187–92. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2008.02.010 Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., Call, J., Behne, T., & Moll, H. (2005). Understanding and sharing intentions: the origins of cultural cognition. The Behavioral and brain sciences, 28(5), 675–91; discussion 691–735. doi:10.1017/S0140525X05000129 Analogies: corvides, dogs, dolphins Emery, N. J., Seed, A. M., Bayern, A. M. P. Von, & Clayton, N. S. (2007). Cognitive adaptations of social bonding in birds. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B, 362, 489–505. doi:10.1098/rstb.2006.1991 Emery, N. J., & Clayton, N. S. (2004). The mentality of crows: convergent evolution of intelligence in corvids and apes. Science (New York, N.Y.), 306(5703), 1903–7. doi:10.1126/science.1098410 Cooperative dispositions West, S., Griffin, A., & Gardner, A. (2007). Evolutionary explanations for cooperation. Current Biology, 17, R661-R672. 4 Bshary, R., & Grutter, A. S. (2006). Image scoring and cooperation in a cleaner fish mutualism. Nature, 441(7096), 975–8. Retrieved from http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nature04755 Week 4 Cognizing social phenomena - Ingroup vs. outgroups, coalitional cognition and racism - Thinking about dominance - Social perception Core reading: Kurzban, Tooby & Cosmides, 2001. Can race be erased? PNAS, 98, 15387-15392. Others: Ingroup/outgroup Tajfel, H. (1970). Experiments in intergroup discrimination. Scientific American. Tajfel, H. (1982). Social psychology of intergroup relations. Annual Review of Psychology, 33, 1–39. Race and coalition Kinzler, Dupoux & Spelke, 2007. The native language of social cognition. PNAS, 104, 1257712580. Kinzler, K. D., & Dautel, J. B. (2012). Children’s essentialist reasoning about language and race. Developmental Science, 15(1), 131–8. Hirschfeld (1998). Race in the making. MIT Press. Dominance Thomsen, Frankenhuis, Ingold-Smith & Carey , 2011. Big and mighty: preverbal infants mentally represent social dominance, Science, 331, 477-480. Mascaro, O., & Csibra, G. (2012). Representation of stable social dominance relations by human infants. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109(18), 6862–6867 Social perception Nosek, B., et al. (2007). Pervasiveness and correlates of implicit attitudes and stereotypes. European Review of Social Psychology, 1-53. Fiske, S., et al. (2007). Universal dimensions of social cognition: warmth and competence. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11, 77-83. Todorov, A., et al. (2008). Understanding evaluation of faces on social dimensions. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12, 455-460. Week 5 Learning from others: communication and imitation Core reading Csibra, G., & Gergely, G. (2009). Natural pedagogy. Trends in cognitive sciences, 13(4), 148– 53. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2009.01.005 Other readings: Király, I., Csibra, G., & Gergely, G. (2013). Beyond rational imitation: Learning arbitrary means actions from communicative demonstrations. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2012.12.003 Wilson, D., & Sperber, D. (2002). Relevance theory. In L. Horn & G. Ward (Eds.), Handbook of Pragmatics. Blackwell Publishing. Marchand, T. H. J. (2010). Embodied cognition and communication : studies with British fine woodworkers. African Studies. 5 2. The psychological foundations of cultural diversity Week 6 Psychologizing culture, enculturating minds The course will consist of a short lecture introducing the theory of cultural models, doing a short review of key findings in cross-cultural psychology and contrasting it with Cosmides and Tooby’s account of cultural diversity. It will be followed by a debate about the plasticity of the mind, dealing with the following questions: - Is cultural diversity an evidence for the plasticity of the mind? - Reciprocally: can cognitive plasticity account for cultural diversity? Two groups of 3 students will be randomly assigned a position that they will have to defend: Position A: cognitive plasticity is evidenced by the cultural diversity and makes it possible. Position B: enculturation is mainly acquisition of knowledge. There is no need to presuppose cognitive plasticity for explaining cultural diversity. Cultural anthropology: culture and personality, and cultural models Mead, M. Sex and Temperament in Three Primitive Societies, Chap. 17: The standardization of sex temperament. Strauss, C., & Quinn, N. (1998). A cognitive theory of cultural meaning. Cambridge University Press. Chapter 6: Research on Shared Task Solution. Bloch, M. 'What goes without saying' In Anthropology & the cognitive challenge. R A Shweder, and M A Sullivan (1993), "Cultural Psychology: Who Needs It?", Annual Review of Psychology 44: 497–523. Cultural diversity of psychological traits and cognition Henrich, J., Heine, S. J., & Norenzayan, A. (2009). The Weirdest People in the World. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 5. Dehaene, S., & Cohen, L. (2007). Cultural recycling of cortical maps. Neuron, 56(2), 384398. Evolutionary psychologists’ critics of social scientists’ psychological assumptions: Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (1992). The psychological foundations of culture. In J. Barkow, L. Cosmides, & J. Tooby (Eds.), The Adapted Mind: Evolutionary Psychology and the Generation of Culture (pp. 163–229). Oxford University Press. Situated and distributed cognition: Hutchins, E. (1995). How a Cockpit Remembers Its Speeds, 288, 265–288. Week 7 The human mind is shaping cultural phenomena Core reading: Sperber, D., & Hirschfeld, L. (2006). Culture and modularity. In T. Simpson, P. Carruthers, S. Laurence, & S. Stich (Eds.), The innate mind: Culture and cognition. Others: Structuralist anthropology and componential analysis Lévi-Strauss, C. (1949). L'efficacité symbolique. Revue de l'Histoire des Religions, 135(1), 527. (English translation in Structural Anthropology, chap. 10). 6 Fessler, D. M., & Navarrete, C. (2003). Meat is good to taboo - dietary proscriptions as a product of the interaction of psychological mechanisms and social processes. Journal of cognition and culture , 3 (1), 1-40. Evolutionary psychology Shackelford, T. K. (2005). An evolutionary psychological perspective on cultures of honor. Evolutionary Psychology, 3, 381-391. Cultural epidemiology Sperber, D., & Hirschfeld, L. (2004). The cognitive foundations of cultural stability and diversity. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8(1), 40–46. . Week 8 Models of cultural evolution Models of diffusion of innovation: r- and s-shaped curves Information cascades; cascading behaviour in network The attractor vs. selectionist model of cultural evolution Core reading: Heintz, C., & Claidière, N. (2013). Current Darwinism in Social Science. In Handbook of Evolution Theory in the Sciences. Others: Henrich, J. (2001). Cultural Transmission and the Diffusion of Innovation: Adoption Dynamics Indicate That Biased Cultural Transmission Is the Predominate Force in Behavioral Change. American Anthropologist, 103(4), 992–1013. Henrich, J. (2004). Cultural group selection, coevolutionary processes and large-scale cooperation. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 53, 3–35. Claidière, N., Scott-Phillips, T. C. & Sperber, D. (2014). How Darwinian is cultural evolution? Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Sperber, D. (1996) Explaining culture: a naturalistic approach, Blackwell Publishing. Chapter 5. 3. Themes in cognition and culture Week 9 Morality Short debate: are moral judgement based on intentions or on outcomes? Two groups of 3 students will be randomly assigned a position that they will have to defend: Position A: Moral intuitions are first and foremost based on outcomes Position B: Moral intuitions are first and foremost based on intentions Core reading Baumard, N., André, J., & Sperber, D. (2012). A mutualistic approach to morality. Behavioral and Brain Sciences. Other readings: Moral intuitions and moral reasoning Haidt, J. (2001). The Emotional Dog and its Rational Tail : A Social Intuitionist Approach to Moral Judgment. Psychological Review; Psychological Review, 814–834. 7 Greene, J., & Haidt, J. (2002). How (and where) does moral judgment work? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6(12), 517–523. Greene, Joshua D. "The secret joke of Kant’s soul." Moral Psychology: Historical and Contemporary Readings (2007): 359-372. Dupoux, E., & Jacob, P. (2007). Universal moral grammar: a critical appraisal. Trends in cognitive sciences, 11(9), 373–8. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2007.07.001 Cultural diversity of morality Baumard, Nicolas, and Pascal Boyer. "Explaining moral religions." Trends in cognitive sciences 17.6 (2013): 272-280. Gurven, M., Zanolini, A., & Schniter, E. (2008). Culture sometimes matters: intra-cultural variation in pro-social behavior among Tsimane Amerindians. Journal of economic behavior & organization, 67(3-4), 587–607. doi:10.1016/j.jebo.2007.09.005 Developmental psychology of moral behaviour Sloane, S., Baillargeon, R., & Premack, D. (2012). Do infants have a sense of fairness? Psychological science. Hamlin, J., & Mahajan, N. (2013). Not like me= bad infants prefer those who harm dissimilar others. Psychological Science, 24(4), 589–594. Hamlin, J., Wynn, K., Bloom, P., & Mahajan, N. (2011). How infants and toddlers react to antisocial others. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108(50), 19931–19936. Oucome vs. intention-based moral judgement McCabe, K. (2003). Positive reciprocity and intentions in trust games. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 52(2), 267–275. Cushman, F. (2008). Crime and punishment: distinguishing the roles of causal and intentional analyses in moral judgment. Cognition, 108(2), 353–80. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2008.03.006 Week 10 Religion Guest lecturer: Vlad Naumescu Readings: TBA ----OR----Economic cognition and economic traditions Readings: The substantivist-formalist debate in economic anthropology Polanyi, K. (1958) "Trade and Markets in the Early Empires" edit par Karl Polanyi, Conrad Arensberg et Harry W. Pearson, pp. 243-270. The Free Press; 1958 Sahlins, M. D. (1972). Stone Age Economics. Transaction Publishers. Chapter 4: The spirit of the gift. Firth, R. Elements of social organization, PP. 122-154. London: Watts and Co, 1952 Cook, S. (1966). The Obsolete “Anti-Market” Mentality: A Critique of the Substantive Approach to Economic Anthropology. American Anthropologist, 68(2), 323–345. doi:10.1525/aa.1966.68.2.02a00010 The economic approach to human behaviour à la Becker Levitt, S. D., & Dubner, S. J. (2006). Freakonomics: A Rogue Economist Explores the Hidden Side of Everything. HarperCollins. Chapter 3: Why Do Drug Dealers Still Live with Their Moms? Week 11 Scientific cognition and science as a cultural phenomenon Core reading 8 Nersessian, N. J. (1995). Opening the Black Box: Cognitive Science and History of Science. Osiris, 2nd Series, 10, 194–211. Other reading Heintz, C. The epidemiology of the infinitesimals. Giere, R. N., & Moffatt, B. (2003). Distributed Cognition: Where the Cognitive and the Social Merge. Social Studies Of Science, 33. Dunbar, K. (1995). How scientists really reason: Scientific reasoning in real-world laboratories. In R. J. Sternberg & J. E. Davidson (Eds.), The nature of insight (pp. 365–395). The MIT Press. Mercier, H., & Heintz, C. (2014). Scientists’ Argumentative Reasoning. Topoi, 33, 513–524. Week 12 Catch up, wrap up, or a theme on demand ... 9