KINDERGARTEN READINESS 20 Kindergarten Readiness: Is Your Child Ready for School? Introduction Traditionally, Kindergarten was considered an opportunity for children to socialize and learn the skills necessary to begin a formal education. Over the last few decades, however, there has been an increasing concern by parents, teachers, researchers, and policymakers over the question of whether children are “ready” for school. Even though Goal 1 of the National Education Panel called for all children to be “ready for school” by the year 2000 (National Education Goals Panel, 2002), there is little consensus on how “ready” should be defined or how it is to be measured. One factor that is being considered in the readiness of kindergarten is preschool. Research clearly shows that children who arrive to kindergarten with stronger cognitive, language, social and behavioral skills have a smoother transition their first year of school (Bellisimo, 1995). Kindergarten readiness can affect a child’s self-esteem, motivation and future success in school. Some parents are choosing to delay their child’s entry into kindergarten for a year. This practice is believed to have increased in recent years due to rising academic expectations for kindergarten and the fear of high stakes testing since the passage of No Child Left Behind (NCLB) in 2001 (Frey, 2005). By offering another year in community preschool or in a public school Pre-K program, children are given “the gift” of maturity to develop the skills needed to do well. The practice of voluntary delay is much more common than in the past. The most current estimates available that are nationally, about 7%-9% of children begin kindergarten a year later than the age at which they are eligible (Entwisle, 2010). KINDERGARTEN READINESS 20 How do you know your child is ready for kindergarten? In the past, the only factor parents needed to consider was age and birth date. Many preschool programs use to fit the curriculum and expectations to the development of each individual child. This approach focused on stimulation appropriate to their learning needs and abilities which would help ensure your child will learn at his or her own pace and develop during the year and be ready for their first formal year of education. Unfortunately, today’s rigors of kindergarten have made it more difficult to prepare a child. Some of the issues we are facing are reduced funds and subsidies for Pre-K programs and high academic expectations with increased class sizes. Kindergarten preparedness goes beyond attending preschool and a child’s eligibility age but, now includes factors such as gender, early experience, developmental maturity and poverty. Should the emphasis of “ready” be placed on the child who gets assessed or the schools themselves to be ready to meet the needs of an increasingly diversity of young learners? Literature Review The following literature review will address four key areas of kindergarten readiness: chronological age and/or developmental age, prior preschool experience, the child’s sex, prepared schools and holding out. Many factors have been examined in the efforts to determine what affects kindergarten readiness. Research suggests that a child’s relative age of entry into formal schooling, prior experience and gender play an interactive part on the ability to construct and reconstruct knowledge in a way that is consistent with the expectations of formal curricula (Gullo & Burton, 1992). Children enter kindergarten with a varying abilities, comprehension, and degrees of preparedness. Furthermore, the same research indicates that more resources KINDERGARTEN READINESS 20 should be devoted to developing Kindergarten programs that better support the learning and development of children with diversified strengths and weaknesses. First, I will discuss the research the effects a child’s age makes upon entry to school. Second, I will examine the research communicating the importance of preschool. Third, I will evaluate the research done on the biological factors of gender. Fourth, I will explore the research that suggests the factors of readiness for children entering Kindergarten should be placed on “Ready Schools”. Finally, I will critique the research discussed in the literature and present my personal response to it. Chronological/Developmental Age Although children may meet a specific age requirement, there are two concerns regarding eligibility. First, there has been a major concern that not all five year olds are “ready” for today’s Kindergarten (Uphoff & Gilmore, 1986) based on their eligible age and that developmental age should be considered. This is due primarily to the academic demands being placed on children by the curriculum and rigors of testing. Academic readiness for Kindergarten includes a child’s ability to perform basic academic tasks, such as counting recognizing letters, and communicating effectively (Harnish, Martin, Fabes & Reiser, 2007). A child’s development is irregular and sporadic. Readiness gaps have been attributed to family life and parenting, home-school connectedness, community resources, and lack of social skills. For example, some children may have participated in various out-of-the home care experiences, had access to literature, can recognize letters, numbers, and shapes, while others have not been read to frequently in the home, have not had social interaction or participated in the kinds of activities that would seem to promote success in Kindergarten. KINDERGARTEN READINESS 20 Secondly, do older children benefit more from their Kindergarten experience than children who are younger? This question has developed into the common practice of “holding out”, “holding back” or “redshirting” in which parents delay their age eligible children’s entry into kindergarten for one more year (Ackerman & Barnett, 2005) in order to gain the skills needed to handle their first formal year of education. The assumption that some children are not mature enough to participate in kindergarten are mixed. Some studies suggest that the developmental levels of children who are closest to the cut-off dates lack maturity. For example, young kindergarten students are more likely to have low work related skills, such as listening to directions and complying with teacher demands (McClelland & Morrison, 2000). A small study in Chicago of children in kindergarten who did and did not meet the cut-off date showed that the younger children in the group had lower test scores and had difficulty processing the information needed to take the tests (Bisanz, Morrison & Dunn, 1995). Many of these children lacked the ability to communicate and pay attention, their ability to take turns and be sensitive to other’s feelings. In addition, chronologically young children continue to perform at a disadvantage in comparison to their older peers. For example, one of the earliest studies exploring the issue looked at the Metropolitan Achievement Test scores of 100 younger and older kindergarteners. The results of this study showed that 13% of the younger students equaled those of their older classmates, whereas the remaining 87% fell below standards (Carter, 1956). This trend has since continued. Math and reading skills are often challenging on a young child’s inability to process KINDERGARTEN READINESS 20 information. On the average, children with little or no amount of preschool or prekindergarten experience lacked mental arithmetic skills and ability to conserve numbers (Bisanz, et al., 1995). A case study by Gullo and Burton (1992) was conducted to determine whether older children in kindergarten benefit from the school experience than children who were younger. One hundred and four kindergarteners participated in the study (52 boys and 52 girls) in a Milwaukee school district to establish a connection between kindergarten age and readiness and were selected by lottery. In addition to the test, a survey was sent home asking if the child attended preschool. Kindergarten readiness was determined using the Metropolitan Readiness Test (MRT) during the school year. Age was examined in two ways. For the regression analyses, age was viewed as a continuous variable (in months). For analyses of variance, age was viewed as a categorical variable using the following criteria to divide the children: (a.) those who started kindergarten at 5 years, 6 months or below and (b.) those who started kindergarten when they were older than 5 years, 6 months. The MRT is a norm-referenced test designed to assess important knowledge and skills required for reading and math. The multi-skill battery included: auditory- beginning consonants and sound letter correspondence, visual- visual matching and finding patterns, language- school language and listening, quantitative- mathematical concepts and operations, and a composite readiness score- the sum of auditory, visual and language. For this investigation, only the MRT composite and quantitative scores were used in the analyses for reading and math. The research indicated that significant main effects were found for age of entry and length of preschool experiences combined versus age of entry with no experience. The findings indicate that there were no differences between the older and younger children by the middle of their kindergarten KINDERGARTEN READINESS 20 year. It was further suggested that children who entered kindergarten somewhat young, had a year of preschool to negate the age effect. Prior School Experience The most hopeful approach for supporting kindergarten readiness is preschool. Participation in a preschool or prekindergarten program can positively influence a child’s first year of academic schooling. Preschool can be defined as those programs serving children not old enough to attend elementary school (Gullo & Burton, 1992). Many preschool and state funded Head Start programs offer a variety of early childhood educational philosophies that include combine learning with play on a developmentally appropriate level. One large component of quality is teacher education/instruction. Preschool quality is contingent on a variety of structural factors, such as the number of children in a classroom, teacher-child ratio, and the physical environment of the center. Quality relies on the types of learning the children participate in and the interactions they have with their teachers and classmates. The importance of preschool quality is demonstrated in many studies. For example, the large scale Cost, Quality & Child Outcomes Study found that attending higher quality programs was correlated with better language scores and math skills for those children who attended a state funded prekindergarten program (Mugnuson, 2004). One assumption is likely related to the fact that teachers in state funded programs are required to obtain a Bachelor’s Degree whereas many teachers in private preschools need a minimum requirement of twelve college units in Early Childhood Education. Research on the effects of varying preschool experiences (public or private) has been somewhat limited to studying populations of children KINDERGARTEN READINESS 20 who are in need of intervention (Goffin, 1989) or examining the different effects of specific curriculum models (Powell, 1987). The defining of “ready” will always depend on who is defining- the school or parent. The demands that a given kindergarten program should be taken into account. There is no question that preschool can enhance a child’s social and cognitive development, but when is the right time to expose a child to preschool? Even though preschool can begin at any age prior to elementary school, many parents are delaying their children an extra year from kindergarten to maximize their learning. This practice is often referred to as “holding out” (holding back) an age-eligible child for kindergarten. One of the prevalent reasons parents give for delaying entry one year into formal education is they felt their child was simply not “ready” based on the assumption they were not mature enough to participate in or benefit from the rigors of an academic program (Bellisimo, Sacks & Mergendoller, 1995). An examination of data from the Early Childhood Longitudinal Study in 1999 showed that children who entered kindergarten a year older than their peers had higher math and reading achievement scores in both the fall and spring of the kindergarten year. Preschool experiences not only include social and cognitive developmental readiness, but also the relationship between the teacher and the child. Kindergartener’s with prior school experience also reflects their ability to play cooperatively and establish friendships. Research by the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development in 2007 indicates Preschoolers who develop successful relationships with their teachers and peers ultimately become more motivated towards engaging in learning activities (Birch & Ladd, 1997). Further research has also shown that teachers have a significant influence on young children’s school readiness and adjustments, such that children who experience warmer and closer teacher-child relationships in KINDERGARTEN READINESS 20 the early years tend to have fewer behavioral problems, like school more and perform better academically than children who do not encounter preschool (Birch & Ladd, 1997). For example, Birch & Ladd (1997) found that secure teacher-child relationships were positively associated with self-directedness and assertive social skills. This finding supports the idea that positive early school relationships encourage class room participation and engagement in school related tasks, which draws other peers to them and improves their social and emotional development needed for school readiness. A case study by Palermo (2007) was conducted to determine whether prior preschool experience influenced a child’s readiness for kindergarten. Participants were 95 kindergarten children from random schools in a major metropolitan area in the southwestern United States using a convenience sampling procedure. Written consent was obtained for 98% of the participants. Approximately half of participants (48%) were girls. Data were gathered in the fall of the kindergarten school year. Parents completed questionnaires regarding their child’s prior school experiences and teachers filled out questionnaires regarding academic readiness, behavior and social skills. The purpose of the study was to test the hypothesis that preschool was related to the readiness of kindergarten. The distributions of variables found that children who attended preschool displayed prosocial behavior and high levels of prior knowledge whereas children with no preschool experience exhibited aggressive behavior, peer exclusion and teacher-child dependency. The descriptive data (Palermo, 2007, p. 411) found that, on average, children in this sample (76%) tended to have a positive experience in kindergarten. The data correlated with the number of children who attended preschool. KINDERGARTEN READINESS 20 Gender/Sex Differences Along with age and prior preschool experiences, gender is a characteristic that can have an influence on a child’s kindergarten readiness. Even though a child’s sex is a biological attribute, being a boy or girl will lead to various social and intellectual experiences in a given environment. Because boys and girls construct knowledge differently (Gullo & Burton, 1992), differences in rates of maturation between boys and girls put boys at a disadvantage, both academically and socially. Lack of maturity can lead to classroom behavioral issues such as class disruption, uncooperative play and an inability to follow directions. Ultimately, boys who have entered kindergarten at a younger age have been found more likely to be retained in kindergarten compared to girls and are possibly due to both lower academic performance and greater behavior issues (Meisels & Liaw, 1993). Gender differences at the early childhood level can manifest themselves in many forms. For instance, previous research by the University of Wisconsin-Milwaukee (1990) suggested that because the majority of early childhood teachers are females, there may be a feminization of the curriculum during these years, possibly leading to detrimental effects for boys. The same study also revealed that behavioral and academic expectations of a given school environment favored girls over boys and in return many teachers tended to spend more time working with girls. Additional attentions in the classrooms give girls the advantage in reading achievement (Gullo, 1990). Cultural and societal expectations can also shape performance. Teachers and parents in the United States tend to expect girls to perform better academically, especially in reading (Lehr, 1982), whereas those in other cultures see holding out as an opportunity to give their sons an KINDERGARTEN READINESS 20 edge over other children in leadership and sports if they are older and more competent than the other children in their class. While boys, in general, may begin schooling at a disadvantage compared to girls due to differences in maturation, socialization, and prior experiences; these differences often diminish or reverse themselves in later years (Maccoby & Jacklin, 1984). A case study by Willson & Hughes (2006) was conducted to determine whether gender/sex influenced a child’s readiness for kindergarten. Participants included 1481 prekindergarten students (54% boys and 46% girls) in the Miami School Readiness Project. All students were eligible to start kindergarten the following school year according to their birth date. During the fall of their 4 year old pre-k year, children were individually assessed for school readiness by their pre-k teachers via a norm-referenced development assessment. Scores were used to measure three domains: cognitive (counting and matching), language (naming and comprehension), and fine motor (writing and manipulation). Teachers and parents completed the Devereux Early Childhood Assessment (DECA) prior to the initial assessment to determine the child’s socioemotional strengths or protective factors, which consisted of three sections: initiative, attachment and self-control. Overall, delayed kindergarten entry was very rare for children in the area attending pre-k programs. Only (0.5%) entered kindergarten a year later than suggested by their birth date. Of the 0.5%, 100% of the children were boys. It is clear that voluntary delayed entry into kindergarten is prevalent amongst boys. The results correlated with teacher and parent DECA assessments providing additional support to the data. In a separate study by Bellisimo (1995), it was suggested that gender was not a single factor in determining kindergarten readiness, but in combination with other aspects such as age and/or previous school experience. KINDERGARTEN READINESS 20 Ready Schools and Holding Out The question is not just “is a child ready for school?”, but also if the school is ready to meet the diverse needs of all age eligible kindergarten children. And if they are not, is holding back their child an extra year from school a result of the schools not being prepared? It has already been established that age, prior experience and gender can make a child’s adjustment to kindergarten difficult, but research from the National Center for Family and Community Connections with Schools has found that early care and education programs that have family components can boost children’s educational success (Ackerman & Barnett, 2005). For example, partnering families with the community. Libraries and museums can provide and extend out of school enrichment opportunities. Seventeen states are participating in the School Readiness Indicators Initiative. The goal is to develop transition initiatives and community partnerships not only with school districts, but on a state level. The initiative does not define readiness solely as a set of traits within an individual child, but instead, it identifies three characteristics of a “ready school” (Gonzalez, 2002). These characteristics help close the information gap between parents and schools. The first characteristic focuses on transitional issues such as cultural, linguistic and contextual restraints. This will require schools to have a variety of highly qualified professional staff, as well as environments that are conducive to learning (Gonzalez, 2002). Staff must also have positive expectations about children’s abilities to learn and succeed in school. The second characteristic concentrates on teaching and learning, and mirror the literature on effective schooling (Newman, 1996). Specifically, ready schools support the professional KINDERGARTEN READINESS 20 development of all those who interact with children. Some strategies include monitoring and adjusting curriculum according to the child’s needs. These educational approaches not only provide support to staff to accommodate diversified teaching, but also solicit parental involvement. The last characteristic addresses that ready schools recognize that children can benefit from support outside of the school, including nonacademic supports relating to healthcare, nutrition and social services (Ackerman & Barnett, 2005). These services can provide parents with the tools that they need to make informed decisions. Studies show that differences in children’s cognitive, language and social skills upon entry to kindergarten correlate with families’ poverty status, parents’ educational levels or ethnic backgrounds, and children’s health and living environments (Ackerman & Barnett, 2005). By giving families information, it may not guarantee there will not be risk factors, but it will provide the necessary support for healthy growth and development. A quantitative study was conducted by Bellisimo, Sacks and Mergendoller (1995) to research the practice of holding back (“holding out”) children who are eligible for kindergarten by examining the prevalent reasons parents give for delaying entry one year into formal education in a single Northern California County. 15.1% of all age-eligible children within 30 schools in the San Francisco Bay area were older than their classmates as a result of being held out of school in 1992 (Bellisimo, et al, 1995) and have fluctuated by 2% from 1988 to 1992. The most significant factor was that parents felt their child simply was not “ready” based on the assumption that they were not mature enough to participate in or benefit from the rigors of an academic program and that schools were simply not ready to modify or accommodate a diversity of learners. KINDERGARTEN READINESS 20 The variable of this study is to determine whether there was a change over time (between 1988 and 1992) in the proportion of parents holding children out of kindergarten and to explore school and family characteristics thought to be associated with this phenomenon (Bellisimo, et al, 1995). Is there a relationship between parents holding back their age eligible child from kindergarten to gain experience and being socially and cognitively ready for entry to kindergarten the following year? Therefore, the research design would be correlational. The study was conducted in one county in the San Francisco Bay area. The county was demographically diverse with the majority of the population being middle class or above. It included suburbs and local farms. Within the county, there were 12 public school districts. These districts had 35 elementary schools which contained 82 regular kindergarten classrooms and 14 prekindergarten classrooms. Of the initial 35 schools, 30 of those schools would be participating in the study (four of the schools contained four small one-room schools, one school did not offer kindergarten and one elementary school did not open up until after the study- these schools were omitted). The population of boys and girls attending kindergarten in September school year 1988-89 (N=1776) and September school year 1991-92 (N=1908) which initially produced sample sizes September 1988 (n=313) and September 1991 (n=320). Of those children who were 68 months of age or over, it was noted between those who were retained or repeating kindergarten (1988 N=72; 1991 N=39) and those who were older as a consequence of having been held out of school (1988 N=243; 1991 N=148). Ultimately, students who were retained were excluded from the study. The sample size used resulted in 1988 n=148 and 1991 n=108. The sample was obtained from school records using the birthdates of all children entering kindergarten in September, 1988 and September, 1991. Each child was then calculated an age based on months. KINDERGARTEN READINESS 20 The sample was obtained through simple random sampling from the various school districts within the county. Random sampling would give each school an equal probability of being selected for the study. The researchers conducted surveys and interviews. Surveys were given to kindergarten teachers within the 30 schools to assess attitudes which might be pertinent to holding out and interviews of parents of kindergarten children in 4 of the schools selected. Teacher surveys for the school year 1988-89 contained seven “school readiness” questions which indicated the common basis why age eligible children should not enter kindergarten. The teachers’ responses were based on a 5-point scale ranging from definitely to definitely not. The same survey was given to teachers for the 1991-92 school year to evaluate if teacher attitude had changed over time. The 1991-92 survey included an additional seven questions which addressed academic expectations regarding math and reading. These questions were also based on a 5point scale. The results indicated that the number of children being held out declined in 1988 and 1991. Boys in 1988 averaged 166 out of 861 (19.3%) and the girls averaged 77 out of 843 (9.1%) compared to 1991 where boys averaged 111 out of 975 (11.4%) and girls 37 out of 894 (3.7%). The figures report that girls were held out 53% less than boys in 1988 and 69% less often than boys in 1991 (Bellisimo, et al., 1995). Holding out patterns were also examined between the initial 30 participating schools. The majority of the decline between 1988 and 1991 came from 7 of the 30 schools. Table 3 shows significant declines, the percentage of held out students in 1988 and 1991, and the accompanying chi-square statistics (Bellisimo, et al., 1995) on page 215. The findings suggest that teacher expectations and responses on the questionnaires did not predict which schools decreased holding out patterns between 1988 and 1991 and did KINDERGARTEN READINESS 20 correlate with the data, but to some extent did influence a parents’ decision to hold their child back. Parental perceptions and their answers during interviews did appear to have played a role in holding out decisions. The results accurately reflected the data and showed a connection of the same results in a previous study by Shephard (1988) which confirms expectations and ideology with holding out patterns. The study revealed an internal validity threat of experimental mortality in which 14 of the schools being studied lost state funded prekindergarten classes between 1989 and 1991. The researchers did consider the possibility that any loss might explain a decline of holding out patterns, but this interaction was not significant enough to affect the data or its’ results. No other threats appeared to be present. In conclusion, it is revealed that between 1988 and 1991, the practice of holding out children of kindergarten when they are age eligible declined progressively each school year by 2%. The finding is in direct contrast with what might have been expected if kindergartener’s “escalating academic demand” (Shephard & Smith, 1988) was continuing. Although, the gender difference confirms the findings of previous research demonstrating a higher holding out rate for boys, and socioeconomic status was only confirmed for boys and not girls. It is further believed that students who were retained only benefited from retention socially and emotionally (Doyle, 1989). Academically, students who were held out did not achieve significantly better than students who were not retained. Conclusion This literature review explored research in four key areas of kindergarten readiness: chronological age and/or developmental age, prior preschool experience, gender/sex, and KINDERGARTEN READINESS 20 prepared schools and holding out. Overall, the goal of determining whether a child is ready for school is based on several factors. It is acknowledged that all children enter school at various stages of development based on their experiences. Preparing a child for entry into formal school begins with exposure to early childhood education programs such as prekindergarten or Head Start. The quality of these programs should include well educated and trained teachers. Finally, after reviewing the literature, it is evident that each child’s abilities are different. I agree with the research that proposes that children who arrive to kindergarten from a transition from preschool have stronger cognitive, language, social, and behavioral skills than those with no prior school experiences. Although the research discussed specific factors contributing to readiness, I felt one of the most promising feature was the forming of a relationship between the schools and parents by providing resources based on a families specific needs. It was encouraging to know that many policies have been instituted beyond the district level and being established state wide. As with any initiative effort, merely creating a guideline or strategy does not necessarily mean its goals will be accomplished. Good policymaking on readiness begins with an assessment of the resources in place and the capacity of organizations to collaborate in supporting children’s readiness for school. As a current prekindergarten teacher, I will continue to research my topic and educate myself as I will face a variety of challenges. As a future elementary school teacher, I will take into consideration the diversity of my students and give each individual student what they need. KINDERGARTEN READINESS 20 References Ackerman, D. J. & Barnett, W. S. (2005). Prepared for kindergarten: What does “readiness” mean? New Brunswick, NJ: NIEER. Bellisimo, Y. (1995). Predicting kindergarten academic skills. Early childhood Quarterly,13 501-521. Bellisimo, Y., Sacks, C. H., & Mergendoller, J. R. (1995). Changes over time in kindergarten holding out: Parent and school contexts. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 10, 205222. Birch, S. H., & Ladd, G. W. (1997). Children’s interpersonal behaviors and the teacher-child relationship. Developmental Psychology, 34, 934-946. Bisanz, J., Morrison, F.J., & Dunn, M., (1995). Effects of age and schooling on the acquisition of elementary quantitative skills. Developmental Psychology, 31, 221-236. Carter, L. B., (1956). The effect of early entrance on the scholastic achievement of elementary School children in the Austin public schools. Journal of Educational Research, 50, 91103. Doyle, R., (1989). The resistance of conventional wisdom to research evidence: The case of retention in grade. Phi Delta Kappan, 71, 215-220. KINDERGARTEN READINESS 20 Entwisle, D.R. (2010). The transition to kindergarten. Early Childhood Quarterly, 33(3), 254 -262. Frey, E. B. (2005). Issues in kindergarten policy and practice. Young Children,45(4), 29-34. Gonzalez, R., (2002). Ready schools: Practices to support the development and educational success of young children. Los Angeles: UCLA Center for Healthier Children, Families, and Communities. Goffin, S. G. (1989). Developing a research agenda for early childhood education: What can be learned from the research of teaching? Early Childhood Quarterly, 4, 187-204. Gullo, D. F. (1990). The changing family context: Implications for the development of all-day kindergarten. Young Children,45(4), 35-39. Gullo, D. F. & Burton, C. B. (1992). Age of entry, preschool experience, and sex as antecedents of academic readiness in kindergarten. Milwaukee, WI: University of WisconsinMilwaukee Press. Harnish, M.C., Martin, L.D., Fabes, R.A., & Reiser, D. A. (2007). Early school competence: The roles of sex-segregated play and effortful control. Developmental Psychology,39, 848-858. Lehr, F. (1982). Cultural influences and sex differences in reading. The Reading Teacher,35, KINDERGARTEN READINESS 20 744-746. Maccoby, E. E. & Jacklin, C. N. (1984). The psychology of sex differences. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press. Meisels, S. & Liaw, R. (1993). Remaking classroom assessment with the Work Sampling System. Young Children,48, 34-40. Mugnuson, K. A. (2004). Inequality in preschool education and school readiness. American Educational Research Journal, 41, 115-157. McClelland, M. M. & Morrison, F. J. (2000). Children at risk for early academic problems: The role of learning-related social skills. Early Childhood Quarterly, 15, 307-329. National Education Goals Panel. (2002). The national educational goals report. Washington, DC: Author. Newman, F. M., (1996). Authentic achievement: Restructuring schools for intellectual quality. San Francisco: Jossy-Bass Publishers. Palermo, F. (2007). Preschoolers’ academic readiness: What role does the teacher-child relationship play? Early Child research Quarterly,22(4), 407-422. Powell, D. R. (1987). Early schooling: The national debate. New haven, CT: Yale University Press. KINDERGARTEN READINESS 20 Shephard, L. A. (1988). Synthesis of research on school readiness and kindergarten retention. Educational Leadership,44(3), 78-86. Shephard, L. A. & Smith, M. L. (1988). Escalating academic demand in kindergarten: counterproductive policies. The Elementary School Journal,89, 135-145. Uphoff, J. K., & Gilmore, J. (1986). Pupil age at entrance – How many are ready for success? Educational Leadership, 43, 86-90. Willson, T. & Hughes, J.N. (2006). Influence of the teacher-child relationship on child conduct problems: A prospective study. Journal of Clinical Psychology,28, 173-184.