Ethnic Change and Polarization over Immigration In the American Public ABSTRACT This article explores the interplay between ethnic change and individual psychology in shaping mass opinion on immigration. Recent research highlights a “personality cleavage” underlying the left-right divide in American politics. Extending this into the domain of immigration, we argue this cleavage should be an important factor shaping citizens’ reactions to ethnic change. Using national survey data and a national survey experiment, we demonstrate that uncertainty aversion moderates the effect of perceived and actual ethnic change on citizens’ immigration attitudes. Our analysis reveals that ethnic change polarizes citizens by personality as those averse to uncertainty feel heightened cultural threat from ethnic change, while those open to uncertainty and novelty feel less threatened. As such traits are associated with left-right identifications, our results suggest that polarization of the American public over immigration, rather than being a mere product of top-down elite influence, is significantly driven by the interaction of citizen and context. Word Count: 8,460 Keywords: immigration, public opinion, ethnic change, polarization, personality, ideology INTRODUCTION The United States is growing more ethnically diverse each year, with steady immigration and a rapidly growing Hispanic population altering the sociocultural landscape surrounding many American citizens. What are the consequences of ethnic change for the politics of immigration in the United States? More specifically, what are the effects of ethnic change on citizens’ opinions on immigration? At present, the opinion literature on immigration comes up surprisingly short in providing a satisfactory answer to these questions. The lion’s share of studies in the opinion research analyzing Americans’ immigration policy preferences focus on the effects of the size of, rather than the change in, the immigrant populations surrounding citizens. Beyond the evident limitation of not directly addressing ethnic change, the results from studies addressing the effects of the size of immigrant populations are notoriously inconclusive. While lay intuition may suggest that immigration-driven ethnic change across the nation may explain observable patterns of nativist resentment and anti-immigrant sentiment among the American public, the empirical research has yet to establish a strong connection between growing immigrant populations and individual opposition to immigration. In this article, we engage the question of the impact of ethnic change on mass opinion on immigration and offer two key innovations over the standard approach taken in past research. First, we explore the effect of over-time growth in, rather than the size of, immigrant populations as the key feature of these populations responsible for driving public opinion. Second, in contrast to previous research which largely assumes that the effects of ethnic context are uniform across all citizens, we offer a novel framework that explores heterogeneity in the effects of ethnic change on citizens’ attitudes toward immigration. Recent research demonstrates that a significant portion of the variance in ideological orientations in the contemporary American 2 public can be explained by differences on a small set of core personality traits (e.g. Hetherington and Weiler 2009; Jost et al. 2003; Mondak 2010). Extending this perspective to immigration, we argue that this “personality cleavage” should be highly influential in shaping how citizens respond to ethnic change with important consequences for polarization of opinion across the leftright divide in American politics. This article develops the differential adaptation hypothesis, which argues that substantial ethnic change should serve as an environmental determinant of opinion on immigration, but that citizens should react differently to ethnic change conditional on their relative aversion or attraction to epistemic uncertainty and novelty. The rapid influx of members of racial and ethnic out-groups can be viewed as threatening to existing cultural institutions, and implies uncertainty in one’s environment and one’s interaction with fellow citizens. As Jost and Hunyady (2005) note, “There is a good match between needs to reduce uncertainty” and the experience of institutional change as threatening, because “preserving the status quo allows one to maintain what is familiar while rejecting the uncertain prospect of social change” (p. 262). Such change could, however, also be viewed in a positive light, as diversity and opportunity enhancing. As Gerber et al. (2010) argue with respect to institutional change generally, “It follows that this attraction to novelty and tolerance for complexity encourage not only overall liberalism, but also support for liberal social and economic policies, which typically involve new programs or interventions that overturn existing practices” (p. 116). According to the differential adaptation hypothesis, there should be differential responsiveness to rapid demographic change across personality types, such that the uncertainty averse should see ethnic change as threatening, while those comfortable with uncertainty and attracted to novelty should find such changes desirable. 3 We test the differential adaptation hypothesis in two steps. In Study 1, using the 2005 CID national survey and data from the U.S. Census Bureau, we demonstrate that traits related to uncertainty aversion moderate the effect of local ethnic change on the perception that immigrants pose a cultural threat. In Study 2, we report the results of an internet-based national survey experiment that manipulated perceptions of ethnic change. The data from this study strongly reinforce the cross-sectional findings from Study 1—namely, that citizens with a strong aversion to uncertainty are significantly more culturally threatened than their uncertainty-tolerant counterparts by induced perceptions of ethnic change. In addition to this core finding, we also demonstrate the political relevance of the interplay between personality and ethnic change through the estimation of a structural equation model linking perceptions of cultural threat to support for restrictive immigration policies. The results from our analyses demonstrate that the differential experience of cultural threat, across personality, in response to perceived or actual local ethnic change ultimately results in a substantial deepening of polarization on immigration policy within the American public. In total, this article makes several important contributions. First, it contributes to the opinion literature on immigration. We move beyond group-size-based measures of ethnic context that tend to dominate the contextual research and instead focus on ethnic change as the principle feature of citizens’ ethnic context driving their opinions on immigration. Further, we reconcile tension between intergroup threat and contact theories by demonstrating heterogeneity in the effects of ethnic change, with change showing opposite effects across citizen types. And last, we demonstrate that the effects of ethnic change on citizens’ policy preferences are mediated by cultural threat perceptions, thus offering the opinion literature a mediated-moderated effects model of opinion formation on immigration. Beyond these specific contributions, we believe our 4 paper also makes a more general contribution to the study of public opinion and political behavior. By assessing the interplay of large-scale, objective contextual processes and individual psychological factors, we fuse the macro with the micro into an integrated approach for understanding political behavior. IMMIGRATION, ETHNIC CONTEXT, AND PUBLIC OPINION The ethnic composition of citizens’ residential environment has long stood as a primary factor hypothesized to account for public opinion on immigration. Underlying the research on the contextual sources of opinion on immigration rests the issue of identifying which aspect of immigrant populations is responsible for driving public opinion. A substantial body of opinion research exists that explores the effect of the size of the immigrant population surrounding citizens on their immigration policy preferences. The racial or power threat hypothesis (Blalock 1967; Key 1949), when translated from White-Black relations to the case of immigration, argues that anti-immigrant sentiment and policy support will be greater among citizens residing in more immigrant heavy areas (Hopkins 2010). This line of opinion research, however, has generated notoriously mixed results, with some studies finding limited evidence in support of the power threat hypothesis (Campbell, Wong, and Citrin 2006; Tolbert and Grummel 2003), other studies finding that residing near large immigrant populations reduces anti-immigrant sentiment and policy support (Fetzer 2000; Hood and Morris 1997), and the bulk of the research finding that the size of local immigrant populations exerts no significant effect on citizens’ immigration policy preferences (Cain, Citrin, and Wong 2000; Citrin et al. 1990; Citrin, Reingold, Walters, and Green 1990; Dixon and Rosenbaum 2004; Taylor 1998). While several factors have been proposed to explain the inconsistency of results for group-size based measures of ethnic context, and the relative empirical weakness of the power 5 threat hypothesis (e.g. degree of contact, Hood and Morris 2000; residential segregation, Rocha and Espino 2008), they ignore perhaps its most defining aspect, which is its focus on the size, rather than the growth, of immigrant populations. Hopkins (2010) argues that American citizens are surprisingly unaware of their demographic surroundings, and that occupational and residential segregation limit the visibility of immigrants to American citizens. Citing a principal axiom of prospect theory (Kahneman and Tversky 1979), Hopkins reasons that while the average citizen may filter out vast quantities of information streaming in from their environment, significant changes in their environment are more likely to capture attention. Given these considerations, Hopkins concludes that while contemporary levels of ethnic diversity may elude citizens’ attention, significant changes in the level of ethnic diversity are less likely to evade notice. Hopkins (2010) argument for focusing on immigrant growth, rather than population size, is strongly supported by the results from his analysis of time series cross sectional survey data, as well as by other recent research on immigration policy and opinion (Alexseev 2006; Citrin et al. 1990; Newman 2012; Newman et al. 2012; Newman and Johnson 2012). Beyond the limitations of the group size framework, extant research implicitly assumes that context exerts a uniform effect across all citizens. While the literature has seen the emergence of a new line of contextual studies exploring the conditional effects of ethnic context (Branton and Jones 2005; Hood and Morris 2000; Hopkins 2010; Oliver and Mendelberg 2000; Rocha and Espino 2008), this work restricts its focus to how the effects of local minority populations may be conditional upon other contextual-level or broader environmental factors. What is missing from the opinion research is an attempt to move the literature forward by engaging the issue of whether important differences across citizens shape how they react to factors operative within their ethnic context. 6 PERSONALITY AND DIFFERENTIAL ADAPTATION We pursue individual-level heterogeneity in reactions to ethnic change with a practical purpose, namely, with respect to its implications for the politics of immigration. The extent to which individual differences in response matter for American politics is conditional on the degree to which such differences align with politically relevant divisions. More specifically, they will matter if they align with the left-right dimension of American politics, and thus map onto elite-level political conflict. We begin by considering how citizens who identify with the left and right in American politics differ psychologically with respect to their stable traits, and how these personality differences should shape interpretation and responses to ethnic change. Personality Differences in American Politics Recent research converges on the proposition that citizens identifying with the left and right in American politics differ with respect to their relative aversion or attraction to epistemic uncertainty and novelty. The idea is that conservatism, in its emphasis on institutional stability, is palliative for individuals who find uncertainty and change aversive, as it lends stability and predictability to one’s social environment. In contrast, liberalism’s emphasis on institutional change and diversity is appealing to citizens who are comfortable with uncertainty, and who seek out novelty and new experiences (Jost et al. 2003; Jost, Federico and Napier 2009). In political science, supportive evidence comes from studies of the “Big Five” personality traits (Carney et al. 2008; Gerber et al. 2010; Mondak 2010; Mondak and Halperin 2008). Such work suggests that, of these five traits, “openness to experience” and “conscientiousness” most reliably distinguish the political right from the left. As Mondak (2010) explains, “the openness and conscientiousness hypotheses are best understood in terms of traditional views in which liberalism corresponds with a willingness to see government tackle new and varied problems, 7 while conservatism implies a more cautious approach in which presumption favors the status quo” (p. 127). As Gerber et al. (2010) explain, openness to experience corresponds with a general attraction to novelty, while conscientiousness is associated with rule and normfollowing, and “socially prescribed impulse control’ (p. 115; see also Carney et al. 2008). This work largely converges with other recent research in psychology proper. Jost et al. (2003) report the results of a meta-analysis of over eighty studies, and find that conservative political orientations are strongly associated with several indicators related to epistemic needs for certainty, such as “intolerance of ambiguity” (Frenkel-Brunswik 1949). Federico and Goren (2009) find that an aversion to uncertainty is associated with conservative self-identifications. Hetherington and Weiler (2009) find that needs for order and certainty constitute a highly influential basis for party identification in American politics since at least 2004. Overall, this body of work suggests that left and right orientations in contemporary American mass politics can be distinguished in terms of relative attraction or aversion to epistemic uncertainty, and thus a general dislike or preference for novelty and change. Implications for Responses to Ethnic Change The psychological divide between the left and right in the American mass public is important as it implies the potential for differential responses to immigration-driven ethnic change. At its most basic level, immigration engenders intercultural contact. Such contact can lead to a process of large-scale cultural change labeled acculturation (Redfield, Linton, and Herskovits 1936; Castro 2003), where the original cultural patterns of either or both groups become permanently altered by the transmission and fusion of culture. When triggered by immigration, the process of acculturation can be characterized by the displacement of the ethno- 8 cultural status quo of the host community and the emergence of a more ethnically and culturally diverse sociocultural landscape. Psychological research on acculturation focuses on how individuals residing within environments undergoing cultural change adapt to the over-time dislocation and replacement of their habituated ethnic context. According to this literature, adaptation to one’s environment involves both psychological and sociocultural components, with the former pertaining to feelings of belonging to one’s community, social trust, and satisfaction with life (Berry and Sam 1997; LaFromboise, Coleman, and Gerton 1993), and the later pertaining to the ability to effectively interact and communicate with cultural outgroups (i.e., sociocultural competence) (Castro 2003; La Fromboise et al. 1993; Ward and Rana-Deuba 1999). The acculturation literature contends that individuals are susceptible to the experience of “culture shock” (Furnham and Bochner 1986; Oberg 1960) or “acculturative stress” (Berry 1970; Berry 1997) as their habituated environment changes and they (potentially) fail to adapt to heightened levels of ethnic and cultural diversity. When brought together, we believe that the research on personality and politics and the psychological research on acculturation hold an important implication for the politics of immigration and ethnic change. Namely, these works suggest that the politically relevant personality traits underlying the left-right divide will be relevant for the politics of immigration because they will shape how individuals adapt to residing in environments undergoing acculturation. The reactions of primary theoretical concern revolve around the feelings and perceptions of cultural threat in response to immigration, which should be highly politically consequential given that cultural threat is a pre-potent source of citizens’ immigration policy 9 preferences (Citrin et al. 1997; Espenshade and Calhoun 1993; Ha 2008; Hood and Morris 1997; Sides and Citrin 2007; Sniderman et al. 2004). The personality and acculturation frameworks suggest openness to, and indeed affinity for, novelty and change, should lead citizens to find immigration-driven sociocultural changes less culturally threatening and should thus engender positive adaptation to ethnic change and heightened diversity. In contrast, dispositions associated with aversion to ambiguity and uncertainty and a corresponding desire for stability and predictability in one’s social environment should lead citizens to find ethnic changes aversive, thus fixating on the displacement of the sociocultural status quo and negative adaptation to ethnic change. Indeed, extant research on ethnic change argues that one principle consequence of the influx of ethnic minorities for white residents, especially those who reside in previously white dominated areas, is that it creates uncertainty about the identity and future of one’s community (Green, Strolovitch, and Wong 1998). In sum, we offer the differential adaptation hypothesis: (H1) Ethnic change and citizen personality will interact to predict perceived cultural threat from immigrants such that citizens low in needs for certainty will become less culturally threatened by immigration as a function of ethnic change, while those high in such needs will become more culturally threatened. Furthermore, we argue that differential adaptation by personality should impact policy preferences indirectly through cultural threat perceptions. To be sure, while our theoretical approach departs from theories of racial or power threat by focusing on changes in the ethnic composition of citizens’ environment as the principle feature of ethnic context driving opinion, we take a cue from these theories by focusing on threat as a perceptual intermediary between citizens’ context and their policy preferences. Applying this standing theoretical logic to our 10 theory, we offer the following second prediction concerning policy preferences over immigration: (H2) Ethnic change induced polarization of perceived cultural threat by personality will mediate polarization of support for restrictive immigration policies, such that citizens low in needs for certainty will become less supportive of restrictive policies, while those high in such needs will become more supportive. Finally, we consider theoretically and empirically the broader implications of these dynamics, and argue that personality-driven partisan and ideological sorting in contemporary U.S. politics, combined with hypotheses one and two, entails exacerbated partisan and ideological polarization over immigration as a function of continuing ethnic change. If correct, then the influx of immigrant populations into the United States, rather than leading to convergence of opinion over the correct approaches to dealing with this issue, will further divide the public along already existing lines of political conflict. Such polarization incentivizes politicians to move to the extremes to score political points with core constituencies, forcing out pragmatic approaches and solutions, and thus lowering the probability of genuine bipartisanship and compromise across party lines, even as these issues increase in importance as a function of such change. We represent our model and its potential political implications graphically in Figure 1. To test our hypotheses regarding differential adaptation and polarization we rely upon secondary analysis of nationally representative survey data and analysis of an original national survey experiment. We address these studies in turn. 11 Figure 1. Connecting Personality Processes to Polarization Epistemic Needs + + + RightWing Affiliation Ethnic Change Perceived Threat + + (Induced Polarization) AntiImmigrant Policy Prefs Notes: The dashed, two-headed arrow is intended to represent an induced bivariate association between political orientations and immigration policy preferences (i.e. “partisan polarization”), not the residual covariance of these constructs within the context of the structural model. 12 STUDY 1 Data Our first study relies upon the 2006 United States Citizenship, Involvement, Democracy Survey, conducted for the Center for Democracy and Civil Society at Georgetown University by International Communications Research (Howard, Gibson and Stolle 2005). The overall survey consists of approximately 1,000 interviews collected door-to-door using a cluster sample design intended to represent the adult population residing in occupied residential housing units. Respondents were matched to county-level context data (e.g. change in the Hispanic population) which we describe further below. In keeping with prior opinion research on immigration (e.g. Brader, Valentino, and Suhay 2008; Campbell et al. 2006; Citrin et al. 1990), the present analysis restricts its focus to N=905 non-Hispanic respondents in the survey. Measures Perceived Cultural Threat. In line with our theory, we expect ethnic change to influence perceptions of the cultural threat posed by immigrants. To measure individual perceptions of cultural threat, this analysis relied upon an item in the CID tapping whether respondents believe that “America’s cultural life is undermined or enriched by people coming to live here from other countries.” This item is comparable to measures of cultural threat in leading opinion research (e.g., Citrin et al. 1997; Sniderman et al. 2004). This item has 11 response options, ranging from 0 (“cultural life undermined”) to 10 (“cultural life enriched”), and was recoded to range from 0 to 1 with higher values indicating greater perceived threat. Objective change in local immigrant population. We measure the objective change in the immigrant population of the respondent’s environment as the percentage change in the Hispanic 13 population in the respondent’s county of residence.1 Drawing upon data from the U.S. Census Bureau, we obtained the percent Hispanic in each respondent’s county of residence in 1990 and 2000. To obtain the measure of change in this population in each county, we subtracted the percentage of the county population which was Hispanic in 1990 from the percentage in 2000.2 The usage of a 10 year time frame is consistent with extant research analyzing the effects of ethnic change on opinion and behavior (Alexseev 2006; Citrin et al. 1990; Green et al. 1998; Hopkins 2010). Personality. To measure uncertainty aversion, we rely on two items measuring respondents’ levels of authoritarianism (r=.48). Both items asked respondents how much they agree or disagree with a specific statement (5-points, from “Strongly agree” to “Strongly disagree”). The first statement read, “What young people need most of all is strict discipline by their parents.” The second statement read, “In preparing children for life, it is extremely important that they learn to be obedient.” In recent work, scholars have theorized that authoritarian attitudes and behaviors are derivative of more basic psychological needs for a wellordered and predictable environment (e.g. Duckitt 2001; Feldman 2003; Hetherington and Weiler 2009; Jost et al. 2003; Stenner 2005). In a meta-analysis of over 80 previous studies, Jost et al. (2003) find strong associations between epistemic needs and authoritarianism (see also Hetherington and Weiler 2009; Van Hiel, Pandelaere and Duriez 2004).In addition to their similarity to recent operationalizations of the construct (see, e.g., Hetherington and Suhay 2011; 1 We use the Hispanic population as the selected immigrant group to test our theory because Hispanics are the largest (Passel, Cohn, Lopez 2011) and arguably most salient immigrant group (Domke et al. 1999). Further, this decision is consistent with extant opinion research on immigration (Burns and Gimpel 2000; Citrin, Reingold, and Green 1990; Hood and Morris 1997; Rocha and Espino 2009; Stein, Post, and Rinden 2000), 2 One limitation of this measure is that the time period under which change is being observed goes from 1990 to 2000 rather than to 2006, which is the year in which our survey data was collected. This operationalization of county-level change is due to limitations in data availability, as the annual American Community Surveys (ACS) taken in between the Decennial Censuses, such as the 2006 ACS, do not have foreign born data available for many counties across the nation with smaller population sizes. In order to obtain data for each county in our data, we were restricted to usage of the 1990 and 2000 Decennial Censuses. 14 Hetherington and Weiler 2009), these items should distinguish respondents on the basis of epistemic needs. Each item considers potential solutions to dealing with a dangerous or uncertain world, more specifically, by adhering to established norms, rules and institutions, and respecting legitimate authorities. As Duckitt (2001) argues, “A view of the world as dangerous, unpredictable, and threatening…would activate the motivational goal of social control and security. This motivational goal would be expressed in the collectivist sociocultural values of conformity and traditionalism and in…authoritarian social attitudes” (p. 50). Controls. We control for three additional indicators measured at the county-level. To account for the predictions of power and economic threat hypotheses, we control for levels of immigrants as the percent Hispanic of the county population in 2000, as well as data from the U.S. Department of Labor’s Bureau of Labor Statistics to obtain the unemployment rate within each county in 2005. Finally, we control for the political culture of citizens’ counties of residence with a variable measuring the proportion of the vote won by George W. Bush in each county in the 2004 Presidential Election. This latter variable has been identified as a relevant factor shaping opinion on policies concerning racial and ethnic minorities (Campbell et al. 2006). At the individual level, we control for several additional factors, including age, gender, black self-identification, educational attainment, income, nationalism, personal economic retrospections, whether the respondent has friends and/or family members who are immigrants, birth in the U.S., employment status, partisanship (higher values = more Republican), and ideology (higher values = more conservative). All variables were recoded to range from zero to one prior to analysis. 15 Analysis We estimate the following regression for perceived cultural threat (Y) via restricted maximum likelihood: 13 π¦ππ = π½0π + π½1π π΄πππ»ππ + ∑ π½π πΆππππ ππΏπππ + πππ ; πππ ~π(0, π12 ) π=2 π½0π = πΎ00 + πΎ01 βπ»πΌπππ + πΎ02 %π»πΌπππ + πΎ03 π΅πππ»π + πΎ04 %πππΈπππ + πΏ0π ; πΏ0π ~π(0, π22 ) π½1π = πΎ10 + πΎ11 βπ»πΌπππ + πΎ12 %π»πΌπππ + πΏ1π ; πΏ1π ~π(0, π32 ) In this model both the intercept of the individual-level equation and the marginal effect of authoritarianism are modeled as a function of county-level predictors and normally distributed, random disturbances. With respect to the marginal effect of authoritarianism, we can think of this specification in terms of two cross-level interactions, one with the change in the county-level Hispanic population from 1990 to 2000, and one with the percent Hispanic at the county level in 2000. We include the latter as a control to ensure that the dynamic we observe is due to change per se, and not simply the overall percentage of Hispanics in a given county. We expect a negative and significant coefficient on the Hispanic change variable (πΎ01 ), indicating that at low levels of authoritarianism change in the ethnic composition of one’s social environment decreases perceptions of cultural threat. Conversely, we expect a large and positive interaction term between change and authoritarianism (πΎ11) such that |πΎ11 | > |πΎ01 |, indicating that at high levels of authoritarianism, ethnic change entails an increase in perceptions of cultural threat. These dynamics are conceptually equivalent to polarization of threat by personality as a function of ethnic change. 16 Figure 2A. Perceived Cultural Threat and Ethnic Change High Authoritarianism .25 .3 .35 .4 .45 .5 Low Authoritarianism 0 .2 .4 .6 .8 Change in % Hispanic, 1990 to 2000 Notes: Estimates are restricted to values of Hispanic change ranging from its 5th to its 95th percentile Figure 2B. Regression Results for Perceived Cultural Threat Change Hispanic Change X Auth Hispanic Levels Levels X Auth Authoritarianism Bush Vote % Unemployed Education Income Male Age Black Nationalism Pocketbook Immigrants F&F Born in U.S. Unemployed Partisanship Ideology Intercept Ni=827, Nj=106, s_1^2=.04, s_2^2=.00, s_3^2=.01 -.5 -.25 0 .25 .5 .75 Beta Notes: Data from 2006 CID. Dots are restricted ML estimates. Extended lines are 95% confidence bounds 17 Results The estimates for this model are shown in the first column of Panel B of Figure 2 with their associated 95% confidence bounds. They show strong support for theoretical expectations. With respect to significance tests of our key directional hypotheses, we utilize one-tailed tests. First, the coefficient for change in the county-level Hispanic population from 1990-2000 is in the expected negative direction and statistically significant (πΎΜ01=-.28, p<.05). In addition, as expected, the coefficient for the interaction of change with authoritarianism is positive, larger in absolute value than the coefficient for change, and statistically significant (πΎΜ11=.49, p<.05), indicating a reversal in direction of the effect of Hispanic change from negative to positive as authoritarianism increases from low to high. To better interpret the substance of these results, we generated predicted values of perceived cultural threat as a function of Hispanic change and authoritarianism in Panel A of Figure 2. The black line represents the predicted values of threat, moving from the 5th to the 95th percentile of Hispanic change, for citizens at the 5th percentile of authoritarianism. The grey line represents the predicted values for citizens at the 95th percentile of authoritarianism. This graph illustrates nicely the theorized conditional relationship between ethnic change and perceived threat. For citizens low in authoritarianism, and thus open to change, uncertainty and novelty, changes in the ethnic composition of one’s county entail a decrease in the perception that immigrants pose a cultural threat to the United States. Conversely, at high levels of authoritarianism, and thus for citizens averse to uncertainty and novelty, changes in ethnic composition entail an increase in the perception that immigrants pose a cultural threat. We will additionally comment briefly on the estimates for the model’s control variables. With respect to Hispanic population levels in 2000, we find no evidence of an interaction, but 18 some suggestion that levels may have an influence on the attitudes of low authoritarians. At the lowest levels of authoritarianism, increases in the percentage Hispanic at the county-level in 2000 are associated with decreased perceptions of cultural threat (p<.10, two-tailed). For every additional 10% of the county-level population that is Hispanic, we predict a decline in perceived cultural threat among low authoritarians of about four percentage points. The political context, operationalized as the percent voting for Bush in 2004, had no influence on threat, while the unemployment rate had a positive effect, but did not attain conventional levels of statistical significance. With respect to individual-level controls, education matters a great deal. Moving from the lowest to the highest levels of educational attainment entails a decrease in perceived cultural threat of about 17 percentage points. Males were about 3 points more threatened than females on average, Black citizens were about 5 points more threatened than other groups, and conservatives were about 7 points more threatened. Finally, citizens who perceive their own financial situation to be tenuous were more likely to feel that immigrants threaten American culture. As seen in Figure 1, however, the strongest determinant of perceptions of threat are real changes in the ethnic composition of the local environment, but in distinct ways conditional on the personality traits of citizens themselves. The Potential for Bias Due to Selection We turn now to a brief consideration of the well-known problem of selection bias, or in other words, the possibility that respondents select into geographical areas on the basis of the presumed causal variable as a function of their attitudes or political dispositions. In our case, this would entail citizens selecting into counties whose demographic profile with respect to ethnicity fits with their dispositional levels of perceived cultural threat. While we address the issue of 19 causality rigorously through a survey experiment (described further below), we believe there are reasons to reject the selection bias story in the present case. While we do not deny that citizens may select into environments on the basis of attitudes and traits which may be relevant to the immigration attitudes examined herein, we believe that our hypothesized dynamic of interest, namely, the interactive effects of ethnic change with personality, should be less subject to claims of selection-induced spuriousness. To see why, consider the logic of the selection bias argument. This argument would claim that citizens select into geographic regions as an interactive function of both the region’s level of expected and/or ongoing ethnic change and the citizen’s own traits and attitudes. Thus, citizens who are most negative toward immigrants would select into relatively homogenous regions, and those most positive would select into regions with greater extant or expected diversity. But our hypothesis is not a simple direct effect of change on attitudes, but rather a conditional one. Specifically, we expect the impact of ethnic change to be exactly opposite across personality orientations, with those low in needs for epistemic certainty becoming more positive and vice versa. While the selection bias argument could potentially hold water for the former hypothesis, it simply fails for the latter. Specifically, for the selection argument to work, one would have to posit that those with personality traits and attitudes most averse to such social uncertainty would be most likely to move to regions undergoing substantial change. This makes little sense. Thus, while we cannot rule out the possibility of these selection biases in one of the two cases, confirmation of our hypothesis for citizens with high needs for certainty in their social and cultural environs speaks strongly in favor of our causal model. 20 STUDY 2 While Study 1 provided evidence strongly consistent with theoretical expectations, we cannot entirely rule out the possibility that these effects are due to selection. In Study 2, we replicate our findings above with a survey experiment which manipulates perceptions of ethnic change, and thus rules out the possibility of selection through random assignment. In addition, we utilized Study 2 as an opportunity to include a more direct measure of aversion to uncertainty. Data The data for our second study consist of 441 valid responses collected through Amazon.com’s Mechanical Turk interface. The procedure for obtaining these data are outlined in Appendix A. Political and demographic summary statistics are also reported in Appendix A. Recent research suggests that, while imperfect, samples obtained through Mechanical Turk are similar in their demographic and political characteristics to those of nationally representative samples, and are superior to more common methods of collecting convenience samples while remaining economical (Berinsky, Huber and Lenz 2012). Given that the influence of personality on information processing and judgment should be a general process, we expect this sample to be generalizable to the broader population. Experimental Design This survey experiment was intended to provide additional support for the causal mechanism advanced in our theory by directly manipulating perceptions of ethnic change, as opposed to relying on the assumption of experienced ethnic change within a given geographic region, thus eliminating the possibility of selection effects. To this end, respondents were randomly assigned to one of three experimental conditions. In all three conditions, respondents 21 first completed a survey of their demographic profile, political affiliations, and relevant personality traits. In the control condition, respondents then completed a survey of preferences over several public policy issues, within which were embedded two questions concerning the respondents’ perception of the threat to American culture posed by immigrants. In the two treatment conditions (moderate and extreme ethnic change, respectively), before moving on to an identical policy survey, respondents were asked to read a mock newspaper article3 concerning the recent increases in the Hispanic population in “most parts of the country in recent years.” In the moderate change condition, the article informed respondents that several “typical” American cities had experienced changes in percent Hispanic from about 5% to about 10% from 2000 to 2010. The article also stated that the percentage of restaurants owned by Hispanics had increased from about 4% to about 9% over this same time period. In the extreme change condition, these values were 5% to 30%, and 4% to 19%, respectively. In both conditions the article included a graph displaying the change in the Hispanic population from 2000 to 2010. Both articles also discussed how these changes have implications for local culture. Finally, both conditions contained identical statements at the end of the articles from “residents” of one of the typical cities highlighted in the article. The first resident expressed the opinion that ethnic change has had positive effects on the local culture, while the other resident expressed the opinion that change has had negative effects. These two statements were intended to represent two potential interpretations of the implications of ethnic change with respect to local culture, and for uncertainty in one’s social environment. As we will show empirically below, this allows for a more direct test of the “resonance” of different interpretations of the same levels of change with different personality profiles. The full text of the moderate change article is presented in 3 Respondents were told this was a recent article from a major national newspaper, and were debriefed with respect to the true purpose and design of the experiment at the end. 22 Appendix B. Following the article, respondents in both conditions responded to three questions concerning their agreement with the two speakers, and then continued on to the same survey as the control condition. All survey items in the post-treatment segment of the study were randomized with respect to order. Measures Personality. We measure aversion to epistemic uncertainty via ten items from the Need for Nonspecific Cognitive Closure scale (Kruglanski and Webster 1996; Kruglanski, Webster and Klem 1993). In addition to its use as a key indicator of the more general personality dimension underlying current ideological conflict in the United States (see Jost et al. 2003;Federico and Goren 2009), the need for closure is highly relevant to our theoretical expectations concerning citizens’ relative comfort with, or aversion to, uncertainty generally, and thus to substantial changes in their social environment. As Jost et al. (2003) argue, “contents that promise or support epistemic stability, clarity, order, and uniformity should be preferred by highneed-for-closure persons over contents that promise their epistemic opposites (i.e. instability, ambiguity, chaos, and diversity)” (p. 348). The need for closure (hereafter NFC) was measured with ten items which are presented in Appendix C (e.g. “I dislike unpredictable situations), and formed a highly reliable scale (α=.82). Perceived Cultural Threat. We measured respondents’ perceptions of cultural threat from immigrants with two items. The first read, “Would you say that America’s cultural life is generally undermined or enriched by people coming to live here from other countries?” Responses were recorded on a six-point scale from “Undermined a great deal” to “Enriched a great deal.” The second item read, “To what extent do you agree or disagree with the following statement: “These days, I am afraid the American culture is undermined by immigration.” 23 Responses were again recorded on a six-point scale (“Agree strongly” to “Disagree strongly”).4The two items were highly correlated (r=-.68) and were thus combined into a single scale. Controls. We control for several additional variables in our analysis, including personal and sociotropic economic retrospections, age, gender, Black self-identification, educational attainment, income, employment status, religiosity (average of attendance and importance), partisanship (higher values = more Republican), and ideology (higher values = more conservative). Agreement with Residents. Finally, in the two treatment conditions, respondents were asked three questions regarding their agreement and disagreement with the diverging interpretations of the two “residents” in the mock article. The first two were six-point agreedisagree scales (as above), one for each of the two residents. The third asked respondents which interpretation they preferred if they “had to choose.” We examine responses to these items after our initial analysis below. All variables were recoded from zero to one prior to analysis. Analysis We test our key hypothesis with an OLS regression of perceived cultural threat on NFC, two dummy variables representing the two ethnic change treatment conditions (with control group excluded), the interaction of the dummies with personality, and all controls. In line with theory, and consistent with the results from Study 1, we expect negative and significant constituent terms on the treatment dummies, indicating that induced perceptions of ethnic change decrease perceived cultural threat for citizens low in epistemic needs for certainty. Conversely, 4 The first item was adapted from the 2006 CID, and the second was adapted from a similar item used in Sniderman et al. (2004). 24 we expect positive and significant interaction terms, indicating that perceived ethnic change increases cultural threat for citizens high in these needs (with the absolute magnitude of the interaction term larger than that of the respective constituent term). Our analysis again excludes self-identified Hispanic respondents. Results The estimates for this model are shown in Figure 3A. We again utilize one-tailed significance tests for our key directional hypotheses. First, as expected, both the moderate and extreme treatment constituent terms are in the negative direction, although only the moderate change term achieves statistical significance (B=-.19, p<.05). We will discuss this asymmetry further below. Second, the interactions of the two treatment conditions with NFC are also in the expected, positive direction, and of a substantial magnitude, again indicating a reversal of the influence of perceived ethnic change conditional on citizen personality. As with the treatment constituent terms, the interaction of moderate change with NFC is statistically significant (B=.32, p<.05), while the interaction with extreme change is smaller and not significantly different from either zero or the moderate interaction term. Overall, the pattern of estimates is supportive of theoretical expectations, and converges with the patterns observed in the cross-sectional data above. As ethnic change in the current context is manipulated, we can also be confident that our causal interpretation of the patterns is warranted. With respect to relative inefficiency of the estimates for the extreme condition, we can only speculate. A potential explanation for this result is that the extent of the change specified in the extreme condition was too extreme, and some respondents realized the deception. This would explain the fact that the extreme manipulation induced the expected dynamics, but the 25 effects were of a smaller magnitude, and contained greater uncertainty. Essentially, a portion of the respondents simply ignored this information as unreliable. Figure 3A. Regression Results, Mechanical Turk Experiment Moderate Change Condition Extreme Change Condition NFC x Moderate NFC x Extreme Need for Closure Age Male Black Born in U.S. Education Income Unemployed Religiosity Personal Retros Sociotropic Retros Partisanship Ideology Intercept N=441, R2=.23 -.4 -.2 0 Beta .2 .4 .6 Notes: Dots are OLS estimates. Extended lines are 95% confidence bounds Figure 3B. Conditional Effects of Manipulated Ethnic Change High NFC .2 .3 .4 .5 Low NFC Control Moderate Ethnic Change Experimental Condition Notes: All control variables held at central tendecies. Low NFC=5%, High NFC=95% 26 To better interpret the substance of these results, we generated predicted values of perceived cultural threat, across the control and moderate treatment conditions, varying NFC from its 5th to its 95th percentile, and holding all controls at their central tendencies. These estimates are shown in Figure 3B. The pattern of estimates is strikingly similar to the crosssectional results of Study 1. At low levels of NFC, respondents in the moderate and merged treatment conditions are about 12 percentage points less threatened culturally than their counterparts in the control condition. Conversely, at high levels of NFC, respondents in the two treatment groups are about 5 percentage points more threatened than similar control respondents. Looking at the interaction in terms of polarization by personality, in the control condition there is only a 5 percentage point gap in perceived cultural threat across levels of NFC. In the moderate ethnic change condition, by contrast, this gap increases to a substantial 22 percentage points. Finally, as discussed several times above, we understand the influence of personality as emerging from differential interpretations of the implications of ethnic change for local culture. We thus expect those high in needs for epistemic certainty to interpret change as a threat in its capacity to alter the socio-cultural landscape. Conversely, we expect those low in such needs, who tend to be attracted to novelty, to be attentive to the ways in which ethnic change enhances local culture through increases in diversity, experience and choice. Consistent with these ideas, we examined the influence of NFC on relative and absolute agreement with the opinions of the two “speakers” at the end of the article viewed in both treatment conditions. The first two items following the article asked about agreement with the position of each speaker. We combined these into a single scale (r=-.77), recoded from zero to one. The third item asked respondents which position they most agreed with “if they had to choose.” 27 We estimated an OLS regression of relative preference for the speaker who expressed apprehension about the influence of ethnic change on personality and all controls. The influence of NFC was strong and statistically significant. A change from the 5th to the 95th percentile of NFC was associated with an increase in agreement with the concerned speaker of 14 percentage points (SE=.04). We then estimated a probit regression of the choice variable on the same set of independent variables. Again, the influence of NFC on agreement is quite strong. A change from the 5th to 95th percentile entails a 28 point increase in the probability of agreement with the concerned speaker (SE=.09). We again interpret these results as consistent with the notion that different ways of interpreting ethnic change differentially “resonate” with personality in predictable, and politically consequential, ways. IMPLICATIONS FOR MASS POLARIZATION AND ELITE CONFLICT As argued in the introduction, our model implies that the interaction of personality and ethnic change generates polarization of support for restrictive immigration policy through changes in perceived cultural threat, and that these dynamics ultimately suggest increased partisan conflict over immigration (see Figure 1).In this final section, we address these dynamics empirically. Implications for Polarization of Preferences on Immigration Policy In addition to perceived cultural threat, we included three additional items in our survey experiment to measure immigration-related policy preferences. The first item asked, “Do you think the number of immigrants from foreign countries who are permitted to come to the United States to live should be increased a lot, increased a little, left the same as it is now, decreased a little, or decreased a lot?” The second asked, “Thinking about immigrants who are currently living in the U.S. illegally, should the U.S. Government require these immigrants to go home, or 28 should the Government grant these immigrants some kind of legal status that allows them to stay here?” The third asked, “How likely would you be to support a state or local law declaring English as the Official Language?” Responses ranged from 1 (“Extremely likely”) to 6 (“Extremely unlikely”). The three formed a relatively reliable scale (α=.68) and were averaged such that higher values indicate greater opposition to immigration (the scale ranges from zero to one). We estimated a structural equation model linking epistemic needs to immigration opposition, through perceived cultural threat, conditional on treatment condition. In other words, we regressed (1) threat on NFC and all controls, and (2) policy preferences on threat, NFC, and all controls, simultaneously. We estimated a multiple groups model to allow these estimates to vary across experimental conditions (the equivalent of interactions). Given the uncertainty regarding the extreme change condition discussed above, we focus here on moderate change only. This model was estimated via maximum likelihood, and the key results are displayed graphically in Figure 5. All variables are again coded on a zero to one scale. The first two rows show the direct effects of perceived cultural threat and the need for closure on policy preferences for the control and ethnic change conditions, respectively. With respect to the influence of cultural threat, our work strongly converges with past work. A change in threat from low to high is associated with a change in policy conservatism of about 70 percentage points. Conversely, there is no direct effect of epistemic needs on policy preferences in either condition. Instead, as theorized and represented in Figure 1, the influence of needs on preferences works indirectly through perceived cultural threat, conditional on the presence of ethnic change. As above, the gap between citizens high and low in the need for closure is small and insignificant (.11) in the 29 control condition, but very large and statistically significant in the ethnic change condition (.40, p<.05). This dynamic translated into conditional polarization of policy preferences by epistemic needs through threat. The last row of Figure 5 shows the estimated indirect effects of the need for closure on policy preferences across conditions. In the control condition this effect is minimal and insignificant (.07), while in the ethnic change condition it is strong and significant (.28, p<.05). Again, the implication of these dynamics is exacerbated polarization of political groups on the issue of immigration, and thus the potential for increased polarization at the elitelevel as a function of these “bottom-up” processes. Figure 5. SEM Results for Ethnic Change and Policy Preferences Control Moderate Ethnic Change Threat on Policy NFC on Policy (Direct) NFC on Threat NFC on Policy (Indirect) -.5 0 .5 1 -.5 0 .5 1 Beta Notes: Data from MTurk experiment. Dots are ML estimates. Extended lines are 95% confidence bounds 30 Personality and Partisan and Ideological Sorting As we stated in the introduction, we pursue individual-level heterogeneity in responses to ethnic change with a practical purpose, namely, to understand the potential implications for the politics of immigration in the United States. While one could, perhaps, posit a large number of individual differences which structure responses to contextual factors, these will be more or less politically influential to the extent that they map onto critical divisions at the elite level. Perhaps the most important question concerns the extent to which these differences map onto partisan and ideological divisions, and thus have the potential to affect the behavior of partisan elites. In this final empirical section, we provide evidence that these personality traits strongly structure partisan and ideological sorting in contemporary American politics, results which converge with much recent research in political psychology. First, in the 2006 CID data, we estimated ordered probit models of partisan and ideological affiliation (trichotomized into “left,” “center,” and “right”) as a function of authoritarianism and all controls.5 For both partisanship and ideology, authoritarianism exerts a strong and substantively significant influence on the probability of identifying with the right wing in American politics (.95 and 1.22, ps<.05, respectively). At the 5th percentile of authoritarianism, the probability of identifying as a Democrat is estimated to be .64, while the probability of identifying as a Republican is only .23, all else equal. Conversely, at the 95th percentile of this personality variable, the probability of identifying as a Democrat is .41, while the probability of identifying as a Republican is .44. The effects on ideological orientations are even larger. The estimated probabilities of liberal and conservative orientations at low 5 We utilize the non-Hispanic sample for these analyses. 31 authoritarianism are .41 and .29, respectively. At high levels of this trait, these probabilities are .16 and .58. Our own data largely converges with the results from the 2006 CID. For both partisanship and ideology, again estimating ordered probits with controls, NFC exerts a strong and significant influence on the probability of right-wing identification (.61 and .88, ps<.05, respectively).A change from low to high NFC corresponds with a change in the probability of Democratic identification from .64 to .51, and a change in the probability of Republican identification from .21to .32. For ideology, the effects are substantially larger. At low levels of NFC, the probabilities of Liberal and Conservative identification, respectively, are .64 and .18. At high levels of NFC, these probabilities are .45 and .33.6 Thus, both datasets converge with other recent work which identifies traits related to epistemic needs for certainty as critical to understanding contemporary partisan and ideological orientations. Given this underlying psychological structure to political divisions, ethnic change should be expected to exacerbate both mass and elite level conflict over immigration. CONCLUSION Summary of Findings The present research has considered the interactive role of personality and context on citizens’ perceptions of the cultural threat posed by immigrants. Utilizing a multi-method approach (both observational and experimental), we have provided strong evidence that the influence of ethnic change on perceived threat is strong, but conditional. More specifically, ethnic change increases perceived threat for citizens with dispositional needs for epistemic certainty, and decreases perceived threat for citizens open to novelty and uncertainty. We find 6 Recall, in the context of both the partisanship and ideology analyses, that Mechanical Turk respondents show higher average probabilities of identifying with Democrats and Liberals than a random sample of U.S. citizens (see Appendix). 32 an identical dynamic across two studies, the first examining nationally representative data and real changes in the ethnic composition of respondents’ environments, and the second manipulating perceptions of ethnic change via random assignment. The convergence of the two studies speaks strongly in favor of our theorized, interactive approach to immigration opinion. The present paper thus offers a novel perspective on how the public forms its preferences over immigration policy. While past research has focused either on “dispositional” variables (e.g. affective orientations toward immigrant groups) or contextual factors (e.g. out-group levels in one’s environment), no work to our knowledge has attempted to understand how the stable traits of citizens shape their attention and reaction to real changes in the ethnic composition of their environments, and how the interaction of these dispositional and contextual factors shape politically relevant attitudes. In this sense, we offer a framework which is truly “political-psychological” in its orientation, viewing citizen and context as inextricable. This view contrasts both with recent work on contextual influences on citizen preferences, as well as work in political psychology on the influence of personality. The former has largely ignored individual differences in how context influences political attitudes, while the latter has interpreted the influence of personality on preferences as causally “direct.” Our framework is distinct. We understand personality through a social cognitive lens (see, e.g., Cervone and Shoda 1999; Mischel and Shoda 1995), or in other words, as a stable force which shapes citizens’ judgments by influencing their selection and interpretation of information in the political environment. In the present context, we have argued that traits related to psychological needs for certainty influence how different aspects of ethnic change will “resonate” with citizens. While those comfortable with uncertainty may actually seek out the cultural diversity implied by ethnic change, those averse to uncertainty will 33 interpret their changing cultural milieu as threatening to the stable environment which they cherish. In this sense, we should view personality’s role for politics as a mediating one, translating the political environment into political opinions by shaping perceptions, feelings and inferences (Mischel and Shoda 1995). The political world impinging on the average citizen is a dynamic one, and our theoretical vision of how dispositional factors interact with this environment must be able to accommodate such dynamism. Given the rapidly changing ethnic environment in many parts of the United States, this is of particular importance to the literature on immigration politics. The present paper is one step in this direction. 34 REFERENCES Alexseev, Mikhail H. 2006. “Ballot-Box Vigilantism? Ethnic Population Shifts and Xenophobic Voting in Post-Soviet Russia.” Political Behavior 28(3): 211-240. Berinsky, Adam J., Gregory A. Huber, and Gabriel S. Lenz. 2012. “Evaluating Online Labor Markets for Experimental Research: Amazon.com’s Mechanical Turk.” Political Analysis (published online 3/2/2012). Berry, John W. 1970. Marginality, stress, and ethnic identification in an acculturated aboriginal community. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 1, 239-252. Berry, John W. 1997. Immigration, acculturation, and adaptation. Applied Psychology: An International Review, 46, 5-68. Berry, John W., and David L. Sam. 1997. Acculturation and adaptation. In Handbook of Cross Cultural Psychology, (ed) John W. Berry, Marshall H. Segall, and Cigdem Kagitcibasi. Boston: Allyn and Bacon. Blalock, Hubert M. 1967. Toward a Theory of Minority-Group Relations. New York: John Wiley and Sons. Brader, Ted, Nicholas A. Valentino, and Elizabeth Suhay. 2008. “What Triggers Public Opposition to Immigration? Anxiety, Group Cues, and Immigration Threat.” American Journal of Political Science 52(4): 959-978. Branton, Regina P., and Bradford S. Jones. 2005. “Reexamining Racial Attitudes: The Conditional Relationship between Diversity and Socioeconomic Environment.” American Journal of Political Science 49(2): 359-372. Burns, P., & Gimpel, J. G. 2000. “Economic insecurity, prejudicial stereotypes, and public opinion on immigration policy.”Political Science Quarterly,1152, 201-225. Cain, Bruce, Jack Citrin, and Cara Wong. 2000. Ethnic Context, Race Relations, and California Politics. San Francisco: Public Policy Institute of California. Campbell, Andrea L., Cara Wong, and Jack Citrin. 2006. Racial threat, partisan climate, and direct democracy: Contextual effects in three California initiatives.PoliticalBehavior28(2): 129-150. Carney, Dana R., John T. Jost, Samuel D. Gosling, and Jeff Potter. 2008. “The Secret Lives of Liberals and Conservatives: Personality Profiles, Interaction Styles, and the Things They Leave Behind.” Political Psychology 29 (6): 807-840. Castro, Vanessa S. 2003. Acculturation and psychological adaptation. Westport, CT: Greenwood Press. 35 Cervone, Daniel, and Yuichi Shoda. 1999. The Coherence of Personality. New York: The Guilford Press. Citrin, Jack, and Donald P. Green. 1990. “The self-interest motive in American public opinion.” S. Long (Ed.), Research in micropolitics (Vol. 3, pp. 1-28). Greenwhich, CT: JAI Press. Citrin, Jack, Donald P. Green, Christopher Muste, and Cara Wong. 1997. “Public Opinion toward Immigration Reform: The Role of Economic Motivations.”Journal of Politics 59(3): 858-881. Citrin, Jack, Beth Reingold, and Donald P. Green. 1990. American Identity and the Politics of Ethnic Change. Journal of Politics52(4): 1124-1154. Citrin, J., Reingold, B., Walters, E., &Green, D. P. 1990. “The ‘Official English’ Movement and the Symbolic Politics of Language in the United States.”Western Political Quarterly 43(3): 535-559. Dixon, Jeffrey C., and Michael S. Rosenbaum. 2004. “Nice to Know You? Testing Contact, Cultural, and Group Threat Theories of Anti-Black and Anti-Hispanic Stereotypes.” Social Science Quarterly 85(2): 57-280. Domke, David, Kelley McCoy, and Marcos Torres. 1999. “News Media, Racial Perceptions, and Political Cognition.” Communication Research 26(5): 570-607. Duckitt, John. 2001. “A Dual-Process Cognitive-Motivational Theory of Ideology and Prejudice.” In Advances in Experimental Social Psychology (vol. 33), ed. Mark P. Zanna. San Diego: Academic Press, pp. 41-112. Espenshade, Thomas J., and Charles A. Calhoun. 1993. “An Analysis of Public Opinion Toward Undocumented Immigration.” Population Research and Policy Review 12: 189-224. Federico, Christopher M., and Paul Goren. 2009. “Motivated Social Cognition and Ideology: Is Attention to Elite Discourse a Prerequisite for Epistemically Motivated Political Affinities?” In Social and Psychological Bases of Ideology and System Justification, eds. John T. Jost, Aaron C. Kay, and Hulda Thorisdottir. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Feldman, Stanley. 2003. “Enforcing Social Conformity: A Theory of Authoritarianism.” Political Psychology 24 (1): 41-74. Fetzer, Joel S. 2000. Public Attitudes toward Immigration in the United States, France, and Germany. New York: Cambridge University Press. Frenkel-Brunswik, Else. 1949. “Intolerance of Ambiguity as an Emotional and Perceptual Personality Variable.” Journal of Personality 18 (1): 108-143. 36 Furnham, Adrian, and Stephen Bochner. 1986. Culture Shock: Psychological Reactions to Unfamiliar Environments. London: Methuen. Gerber, Alan S., Gregory A. Huber, David Doherty, Conor M. Dowling, and Shang E. Ha. 2010. “Personality and Political Attitudes: Relationships across Issue Domains and Political Contexts.” American Political Science Review 104 (1): 111-133. Green, Donald P., Dara Z. Strolovitch, and Janelle S. Wong. 1998. “Defended Neighborhoods, Integration, and Racially Motivated Crime.” American Journal of Sociology 104(2): 372403. Ha, Shang E. 2010. “The Consequences of Multiracial Contexts on Public Attitudes toward Immigration.” Political Research Quarterly 63(1): 29-42. Hetherington, Marc J., and Elizabeth Suhay. 2011. “Authoritarianism, Threat, and Americans’ Support for the War on Terror.” American Journal of Political Science 55 (3): 546-560. Hetherington, Marc J., and Jonathan D. Weiler. 2009. Authoritarianism and Polarization in American Politics. New York: Cambridge University Press. Hood, M. V., and Irwin L. Morris. 1997. “Amigo o enemigo? Context, attitudes, and Anglo public opinion toward immigration.” Social Science Quarterly78(2): 7813-7819. Hood, M.V., and Irwin L. Morris. 2000. “Brother, Can You Spare a Dime? Racial/Ethnic Context and the Anglo Vote on Proposition 187.” Social Science Quarterly 81(1): 194206. Hopkins, Daniel J. 2010. “Politicized Places: Explaining Where and When Immigrants Provoke Local Opposition. American Political Science Review 104(1): 40-60. Jost, John T., Christopher M. Federico, and Jaime L. Napier. 2009. “Political Ideology: Its Structure, Functions, and Elective Affinities.” Annual Review of Psychology 60: 307-337. Jost, John T., Jack Glaser, Arie W. Kruglanski, and Frank J. Sulloway. 2003a. “Political Conservatism as Motivated Social Cognition.” Psychological Bulletin 129 (3): 339-375. Jost, John T., and Orsolya Hunyady. 2005. “Antecedents and Consequences of System-Justifying Ideologies.” Current Directions in Psychological Science 14 (5): 260-265. Kahneman, Daniel, and Amos Tversky. 1979. “Prospect Theory: An Analysis of Decision Under Risk.” Econometrica 47(2): 263-292. Key, V.O. 1949. Southern Politics in State and Nation. New York: A.A. Knopf. Kruglanski, Arie W., and Donna M. Webster. 1996. “Motivated Closing of the Mind: ‘Seizing’ and ‘Freezing’.” Psychological Review 103 (2): 263-283. 37 Kruglanski, Arie W., Donna M. Webster, and Adena Klem. 1993. “Motivated Resistance and Openness to Persuasion in the Presence or Absence of Prior Information.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 65 (5): 861-876. LaFromboise, Teresa, Hardin L. K. Coleman, and Jennifer Gerton. 1993. “Psychological Impact of Biculturalism: Evidence and Theory.”Psychological Bulletin 144(3): 95-412. Mischel, Walter, and Yuichi Shoda. 1995. “A Cognitive-Affective System Theory of Personality: Reconceptualizing Situations, Dispositions, Dynamics, and Invariance in Personality Structure.” Psychological Review 102 (2): 246-268. Mondak, Jeffery J. 2010. Personality and the Foundations of Political Behavior. New York: Cambridge University Press. Mondak, Jeffery J., and Karen D. Halperin. 2008. “A Framework for the Study of Personality and Political Behaviour.” British Journal of Political Science 38: 335-362. Newman, Benjamin J. 2012. “Acculturating Contexts and Opposition to Immigration in the U.S.” Unpublished Manuscript. Newman, Benjamin J., and Joshua Johnson. 2012. “Immigration, Ethnic Change, and Approval of State Government.” Unpublished Manuscript. Newman, Benjamin J., Christopher D. Johnston, April A. Strickland, and Jack Citrin. 2012. “Immigration Crackdown in the American Workplace: Explaining Variation in E-Verify Policy Adoption across the U.S. States.” Forthcoming. State Politics and Policy Quarterly. Oberg, Kalervo. 1960. “Culture shock: Adjustment to new cultural environments.”Practical Anthropology, 7, 177-182. Oliver, J. Eric, and Tali Mendelberg. 2000. “Reconsidering the Environmental Determinants of White Racial Attitudes.” American Journal of Political Science 44(3): 574-589. Passel, Jeffrey S., D’Vera Cohn, and Mark Hugo Lopez. 2011. “Census 2010: 50 Million Latinos, Hispanics Account for More than Half of Nations’ Growth in Past Decade.” Pew Hispanic Center Report. Redfield, Robert, Ralph Linton, and Melville J. Herskovitz. 1936. “Memorandum for the Study of Acculturation.” American Anthropologist 38(1): 149-152. Rocha, Rene R., and Rudolfo Espino. 2009. “Racial Threat, Residential Segregation, and the Policy Attitudes of Anglos.” Political Research Quarterly 62(2): 415-426. Sides, John, and Jack Citrin. 2007. “European Opinion About Immigration: The Role of Identities, Interests, and Information.” British Journal of Political Science 37: 477-504. 38 Sniderman, Paul M., Louk Hagendoorn, and Markus Prior. 2004. “Predisposing Factors and Situational Triggers: Exclusionary Reactions to Immigrant Minorities.”American Political Science Review98(1): 35-49. Stein, Robert M., Stephanie Shirley Post, and Allison L. Rinden. 2000. “Reconciling Context and Contact Effects on Racial Attitudes.” Political Research Quarterly 53(2): 285-303. Stenner, Karen. 2005. The Authoritarian Dynamic. New York: Cambridge University Press. Taylor, Marylee C. 1998. “How White Attitudes Vary with the Racial Composition of Local Populations: Numbers Count.” American Sociological Review 63: 512-535. Tolbert, Caroline J., and John A. Grumme. 2003. “Revisiting the Racial Threat Hypothesis: White Voter Support for California’s Proposition 209.” State Politics and Policy Quarterly 3(2): 183-192. Van Hiel, Alain, Mario Pandelaere, and Bart Duriez. 2004. “The Impact of Need for Closure on Conservative Beliefs and Racism: Differential Mediation by Authoritarian Submission and Authoritarian Dominance.” Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 30: 824-837. Ward, Collen, and Arzu Rana-Deuba. 1999. “Acculturation and Adaptation Revisited.” Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 30(4): 422-442. 39 APPENDIX A. DETAILS FOR MECHANICAL TURK EXPERIMENT Mechanical Turk is a service that Amazon.com provides to those who are interested in the performance of a large number of small tasks that require human judgment. Individuals performing the tasks create a user profile on the site and are paid a fee for their services that varies depending on the time, effort, and expertise involved in the task. Researchers post a human intelligence task (HIT) to the Amazon site and offer payment rate per HIT. A link to our survey was posted on this site, and advertised under the keywords “politics,” “survey,” and “public policy.” The description of the task read, “You will be asked to answer several questions about yourself and respond to a short newspaper article.” Valid completions of the survey were compensated $.50. The effective hourly rate was estimated to be $3.66. The data were culled to remove all respondents completing the survey multiple times, leaving a total of 441 valid completions. Summary statistics for these respondents are shown below. Median Age Male Black White Mean Political Interest (1-5) Mean Subjective Political Knowledge (1-5) Mean Partisanship (1-7, higher=Rep) Mean Ideology (1-7, higher=Con) Mean Importance of Religion to Self (1-5) Median Income Mean Income Median Education Unemployed or Temporarily Laid Off Student Working Now Central Tendencies and %s 28 55% 5% 83% 3.32 3.17 3.40 3.34 2.27 20K-30K 30K-40K Post-HS Degree 19% 18% 53% 40 APPENDIX B. TEXT OF TREATMENT CONDITIONS (LOW/HIGH) Hispanic Immigration and Culture in the United States BYLINE: By CAMPBELL JOHNSON SECTION: Section D; Column 0; Pg. 23 LENGTH: [512/518] words The 2010 Decennial Census is out, and, as most are aware, there has been a significant increase in Hispanic immigrants in most parts of the country in recent years. While immigration is often a topic of contemporary discussion in economics circles, less often considered are the cultural implications of immigration. Recent immigrants bring their culture with them, and this has led to an increased presence of Hispanic culture in cities across the United States, a trend expected to continue for at least the near future. Consider, for example, the typical American cities of Fairfield, NJ, Topeka, KS, Eugene, OR, or Newton, Mississippi. According to the Decennial Census, the Hispanic population in each of these cities grew [ /drastically] from about 5% of the population in 2000 to roughly [10%/30%] in 2010. With this increased presence of Hispanic immigrants has come a changing local culture. According to the National Restaurant Association, in 2000 4% of these cities’ restaurants were owned and operated by Hispanic immigrants. By 2010, this number had more than [doubled/tripled] to [9%/18%]. More generally, one sees a [rising/surging] number of Hispanic businesses within the strip malls of these areas. Obviously, culture is also intimately intertwined with language, and with increasing Hispanic immigration comes a [ /drastic] growth in Spanish-speaking citizens across U.S. cities. Driving down the street in cities like Fairfield, for example, such changes are apparent in the signage outside strip malls and places of business. Whereas English used to be the sole medium of communication for such displays, [one can sometimes see/one more often than not sees] signs with both English and Spanish translations, and even Spanish-only communications. Many see these developments as a % Hispanic positive change to their local culture, providing new opportunities and 30% additional experiences. John W., for example, a resident of Fairfield, thinks such changes have made his city a 20% more interesting and exciting place to live: “Five years ago you had basically two choices for eating out in Fairfield, 10% fast food or the local burger place. With the increase in Hispanic 0% immigrants, we now have a wealth of 2000 2010 new choices, not only Mexican, but Spanish, Brazilian, even a new Argentinean restaurant!” John also thinks the increased presence of the Spanish language is a positive development: “It has encouraged my family and me to try to learn a new language, which is great thing I think.” Many do not see these developments as a positive change to their local culture, but as a threat to their long-standing way of life. Mark S., for example, another resident of Fairfield, thinks that these changes have made his city a completely different place to live: “The changes over the last five years have been so large that I barely recognize Fairfield anymore. Sure there are a bunch of new restaurants, but my family and I were perfectly happy with the old ones; it seems that every new opening comes from a different country.” Mark is also unhappy with the increased presence of the Spanish language: “When I walk into a business now, I always wonder whether the clerk will understand me or the signs will be in English.” 41 APPENDIX C. NEED FOR NONSPECIFIC COGNITIVE CLOSURE ITEMS For each item, please indicate how much you agree or disagree with the statement. Do you Strongly agree, somewhat agree, slightly agree, slightly disagree, somewhat disagree, or strongly disagree? 1. “I enjoy the uncertainty of going into a new situation without knowing what might happen.” 2. “I dislike unpredictable situations.” 3. “I don’t like to be with people who are capable of unexpected actions.” 4. “I prefer to socialize with familiar friends because I know what to expect from them.” 5. “I think it is fun to change my plans at the last moment.” 6. “I dislike it when a person’s statement could mean many different things.” 7. “I feel uncomfortable when someone’s meaning or intention is unclear to me.” 8. “I feel uncomfortable when I don’t understand the reason why an event occurred in my life.” 9. “When I am confused about an important issue, I feel very upset.” 10. “It’s annoying to listen to someone who cannot seem to make up his or her mind.” 42