An Analysis of the Rally ‘round the Flag Effect: International Conflicts, Presidential Approval Ratings, and Multilateralism Fowler Paper Competition Roanoke College April 22, 2014 1 Table of Contents Abstract p. 2 Introduction p. 3 Literature Review p. 7 Argument and Hypothesis p. 13 Research Design p. 19 Findings p. 25 Conclusion p. 28 2 Abstract The rally ‘round the flag effect is an expression that has been used to describe the noticeable spikes in U.S. presidential approval ratings during instances of international conflict. Although the president can generally expect an increase in public support during crises, the size of the effect varies widely across different conflicts. Several studies have examined this phenomenon and yet the literature lacks consensus on why in certain instances of conflict rallies barely register, while in others they can reach upward of 30 percentage points. This paper contributes to this discussion by proposing a conditional explanation of the effect of conflict initiation on presidential approval ratings. Specifically, I argue that instances of militarized interstate disputes that gain multilateral support from the United Nations Security Council or North Atlantic Treaty Organization tend to lead to larger increases in approval ratings, which can explain the variations in spikes. To evaluate my argument empirically, I compiled a monthly data set of presidential approval ratings, conflict, and displays of multilateral support from 1949 to 2010. This study furthers our understanding of the complex connections between the domestic and international policy spheres. 3 Introduction: Following the September 11, 2001 terrorist attacks on the United States, George W. Bush’s job approval ratings surged to a record 90 percent on September 21st, 2001. Such a drastic increase in presidential approval ratings coincides with the idea that during instances of international crisis, the President will experience a period of increased public support (Mueller 1970). For example, following the September 11th terrorist attacks on the United States, not only did President George W. Bush’s approval ratings swell to 90 percent but they also lingered in the high 80’s in the subsequent months. This demonstrates a quintessential example of the rally ‘round the flag effect, yet this phenomenon is not consistently seen in all instances of international conflict involving the United States. In June of 1950, 75,000 North Korean soldiers advanced over the border into Western backed South Korea and by July, President Truman had sent American troops to fight on South Korea’s behalf (Stueck 2010, 168). In contrast to the 2001 example, President Truman’s job approval ratings did not show a massive inflation following the involvement in an international crisis. In the months following the military intervention, President Truman’s approval ratings hovered between the same 35 to 45 percent range, the same as prior to the initiation of the conflict. The tendency of the public to bolster support for the current President of the United States during times of international crises, the rally ‘round the flag effect, has produced an unexplainable range of variations of spikes in approval ratings. In some instances, this effect is barely seen, as within the Korean War under President Truman, while yet in others the increases in the ratings are massive, such as the 33% point increase as demonstrated after the 9/11 attacks in 2001 (Gallup). This discrepancy in the public’s reaction to conflicts raises the question of 4 what other factors might be responsible for this variation in presidential approval ratings during different instances of militarized interstate disputes. Given the unexplained variations in spikes of approval ratings during different instances of conflict, it becomes necessary to determine the underlying factors. This paper will analyze support from the UNSC and NATO as the principal factor intervening in the relationship between conflict engagement and presidential approval ratings. By examining the UNSC and NATO’s influences, this project can further the understanding of international institution’s impact on foreign policy in the domestic sphere. The more specific research question becomes: what is the effect of support from the UNSC and NATO during instances of foreign conflict on presidential approval ratings in the United States? International politics can be seen as having a significant impact on domestic policies, as demonstrated through the relationship between foreign militarized disputes and presidential approval ratings. Domestically, Abramson, Aldrich and Rohde (1987) and Fiorina (1981) illustrated that voting behavior in both presidential and congressional elections are effected by the current Presidents approval ratings. Presidents who have a higher job approval rating are symbolically given a general vote of confidence by the American public. This is then translated into voting patterns in congressional elections and how the presidential incumbent will most likely fair in the coming elections. Literature on the subject has established that voters with low opinions of the current president are less likely to vote for representatives of the same political party (Cover 1986; Kernell 1977; Marra and Ostrom 1989; Gronke, Koch and Wilson 1998). “The policies, programs, and outcomes associated with the current administration serve as important voting cues in these races, and citizens cast the congressional ballot at least in part as an expression of their attitudes towards the president’s agenda” (Gronke, Koch and Wilson 2013, 5 785). This allows for the more popular presidents to gain leverage in influencing other political actors to support administrative policies, and provides more options without the fear of congressional resistance. On the other hand, a president with a low job approval rating can also influence the next generation of elected congressional and presidential leaders. The effect that approval ratings has over elections at both the congressional and the presidential level can serve as a determinant for a president’s use of force on an international level. John Mueller, the first to operationalize the rally ‘round the flag effect, theorized that during a militarized interstate dispute, public opinion will generally favor the president due to the fear that opposing him would hinder a successful outcome to the crisis. During crises, job approval ratings can then become a major factor for the president when deciding how to respond. Wittkopf and DeHaven (1987) found that direct and forceful action by the current president in response to an international crisis have a positive relationship with public approval. Therefore, the presidents who hold lower approval ratings may be more likely to “rattle the saber” in an attempt to create a favorable surge in public opinion. Ostrom and Simon (1987) indicate that as a president can influence the environmental stimuli and therefore public opinion during instances of international conflict that they may choose a course of action specifically to influence popular support. Further, Morgan and Bickers (1992) suggest that “aggressive foreign behavior is a useful tool for dealing with domestic political problems” (p. 26). Through this relationship, job approval ratings can become significant in determining not only the future in American domestic politics, but it can be used to influence international affairs as well. The UNSC and NATO then allow for the public to more efficiently judge the presidents the actions as they are outside, legitimate, and independent organizations with varying members that hold a myriad of interests, goals and values. By providing support for the U.S. president, 6 resolutions of support from these multilateral organizations may signal to the American public that the president is acting appropriately, and therefore may explain for the variations in the rallying effect. For the purposes of this paper, the definition of the UNSC support is an issued resolution from the Security Council while NATO support includes special declarations of approval and missions and interventions directed by the organization. I collected conflict information from the Militarized Interstate Dispute (MID) data set and used primary source documents from the UN and NATO to determine if support was given or not. Within the paper, a means comparison and linear regression analysis were used. This paper continues with a literature review and goes on to explain the methodology, research design, and findings. This paper contributes to the literature that attempts to explain the underlying factor affecting the variations in increases in presidential approval ratings during instances of militarized interstate disputes. Up until this point, little has been written connecting the ways in which international institutions, presidential approval ratings, and the rally ‘round the flag effect are correlated, making this paper a significant addition to international relations scholarship. In understanding the ways in which an international event affects an American phenomenon, it becomes necessary to look outside of purely domestic influences. Although much of the literature is concerned with domestic aspects will affect this phenomenon, it is important to instead examine how international influences impact the rally ‘round the flag effect. For this reason, this paper ties together an interesting effect of and relationship between international and domestic politics. Further, as Chapman and Reiter (2004) produced a different set of findings from their examination of the relationship between the United Nations and presidential approval ratings, this study becomes important in the furthering of the ways in which international affairs influence domestic policies. 7 Literature Review: During instances of foreign conflict in the United States, the logical expectation would be that approval ratings for the president would increase due to a surge in public patriotism (Mueller 1973, Bradley and Oneal 1993, Oneal and Baker 2001). Mueller labeled this concept the rally ‘round the flag effect and it has since been analyzed by a number of scholars through different conditional factors that serve to expose the variations in presidential approval ratings stemming from this phenomenon (Mueller 1970). Different studies have proposed that the inconsistencies in the rallying effect are based on the impact of a specific factor such as the media, the president’s political party, or the temporal span of the conflict. Despite the fact that these studies have proposed a number of explanations for the variations in spikes, there is still little consensus on the underlying causes for this phenomenon. Lian and Oneal (1998) argued that the rallying effect of an instance of conflict is greater when it is heavily reported by the media, as the American public cannot support something of which they are unaware. Although they found a slight increase in mean change in the president’s popularity when prominently reported by the New York Times, the study still does not explain why in some instances the rallying effect generates a spike of 30 percent or more while in others the event barely registers. Baum (2002) believes that the phenomenon is more complex than just recording if there is a rallying effect or not. Instead, he argues that different types of Americans respond differently to instances of conflict based on various environmental factors. In his study he finds that different constituent groups do rally according to varying circumstances yet it is due to a number of possible economic and political environmental factors. Further, he found that individual political party identification, if there is a divided government, the economic status, and the timing of the use of force all affect the likelihood and strength of a rally. While this study 8 is useful in explaining the patterns and trends of the rallying effect, it does not provide a specific and definitive answer to why the phenomenon is inconsistent. Baum’s study instead indicates the possibility that these factors may play a part in determining the level of the spike, but based on the broad nature of his findings, it cannot point to the specific underlying cause for the variation in the rallying effect. Lee (1977) argued that the type of foreign event plays a more significant impact on the size and duration of the rally. He specifically examines important events that include “1) outbreak of wars or military crises that involved actual or potential use of American troops; 2) end of war or reconciliation of major international conflict that involved American resources; 3) Summit conferences between the U.S. and other major powers; 4) New initiatives in foreign policy which received a wide public attention; 5) international setbacks (or achievements), including break off of negotiations and other embarrassing events of failures; 6) events involving the personality of the president such as an assassination attempt” (p. 252). He argues that this is due to the tendency of the public to support the president directly after an international event due to the lack of information and therefore the public is more likely to grant the president the benefit of the doubt, but that the approval ratings will digress after a brief period of time. Further, his study serves to explain that during crises, the president becomes the focus of national attention and instigates a feeling of patriotism that does not exist without the crisis. Lee argues that the type of major event serves as the reason for the variations. Further, international setbacks were found to reduce approval ratings and he concluded that wars and military crises have the largest and most durable rallying effect while policy initiatives tend to be associated with the largest average change in job approval ratings. Although this study is significant in analyzing the ways in which the public perceives certain international events, Lee’s study is more focused on the 9 duration aspect of the rally ‘round the flag effect and therefore further examination is necessary in creating a complete analysis that describes the causes of the phenomenon. Although there is a general consensus among the different studies that war does tend to increase presidential approval ratings, they fail to explain for the variations in the level of the rallying effect. The empirical findings from these studies suggest that factors other than solely conflict initiation account for the differences in public support as demonstrated in the discrepancies in spikes in approval ratings. Although other scholars have attempted to explain the differences through an analysis of several specific factors, no study has been able to definitively answer why the phenomenon occurs. Each study has only been able to conclude that a certain variable may be able to influence the differences in the magnitude of the rallying effect, but it does not explain if one specific factor conditions the larger increase in some instances over others. This may be due to the fact that there is no generally agreed upon definition of foreign conflict and therefore each study may be analyzing different examples, which would explain the differences in findings. The most widely used definition comes from Mueller (1970): “In general, a rally point must be associated with an event which 1) is international and 2) involves the United States and particularly the President directly; and it must be 3) specific, dramatic, and sharply focused” (21). Although this is the most popular definition found in the rally ‘round the flag literature, I believe that it is too broad to allow for a definitive solution to the affect as it does not explain the type of foreign event. The definition could be referring to military intervention, a declared war, a direct attack, or a negotiation or summit meeting. Due to the generic nature of this definition of a rallying point, it may explain for the literatures inability to produce a conclusive answer. It is unclear how other scholars have 10 interpreted this definition and therefore analyzed the rallying points. While some studies may believe that certain instances fit each of the criteria for Mueller’s definition of a rallying point, this may not be true across all of the studies. Mueller’s generic definition does not further explain what specific, dramatic, or sharply focused includes and therefore scholars may be using different instances of militarized interstate disputes based on what they believe to be specific, dramatic, and sharply focused. Each aspect of this portion of the definition is generalizable in a number of different ways and based on the varying nature of militarized interstate disputes in that no one is an exact replica of another this may differ from study to study. Therefore, since it is unclear how other scholars have interpreted Mueller’s general definition or chose to identify foreign conflict through a different set of terms, this lack of a uniform conceptualization can be used to explain the inconsistencies in the literature. Without this commonality, it cannot be possible for the current literature to create a substantial and definitive understanding of the rally ‘round the flag effect if the authors are not defining the same category of events. The differences in findings could also be due to the fact that they are not all studying the same time periods. In some studies, the authors only examine specific instances of foreign conflict. Callaghan and Virtanen (1993) analyze only the Iran hostage Crisis under President Carter, arguing that the rallying effect is not unconditional and must be explained along party lines, and find that partisan groups must be understood as individual and not uniform in a comprehensive analysis of the rallying effect. Eichenberg, Stoll and Lebo (2006) examined President George W. Bush’s approval ratings in terms of the 2001 terrorist attack and the subsequent Iraq War. They include five variables: “a ‘honeymoon effect, an autoregressive function that tracks a decline in approval, measures of economic performance, measures of important ‘rally events,’ and a measure of the costs of war – in this case, the U.S. death toll in the 11 Iraq War” (p 783). They find that the terrorist attacks instigated a steep rallying effect but it was soon deflated by the Iraq War, which cost Bush a large percentage of his support, and that only truly dramatic events can determine a significant rallying effect. Schubert, Stewart, and Curran (2002) analyzed public approval ratings of George W. Bush immediately following the 2001 terrorist attacks through five factors: 1) the terrorist attack; 2) Bush’s speech following the attacks; 3) media exposure; 4) partisan support; and 5) gender effects. This study found that Bush’s September 11 speech was the most critical factor influencing this rally effect while none of the other factors were significant. Another approach has been to examine periods of 10 to 59 years. Oneal and Bryan 1995. Baker and Oneal (2001) examined the influence of how the crises are presented to the public from 1933 to 1992 in media coverage, bipartisan support, and from White House statements, and concluded that rallies are associated due to a number of conditional factors: higher levels of economic confidence, escalation of the hostility level of a dispute, and media coverage. Chapman and Reiter (2004) analyzed the effect of the UNSC on the rallying effect from 1945 to 2001 and their findings suggest that the UN Security Council can have a positive effect on approval ratings. Groeling and Baum (2012) attempted to explain the phenomenon using the idea that the persuasiveness, credibility, and influence of the messages from the political elite will determine the rallying effect from 1979 to 2003. They found that the relationship between the content of the message, public opinion, and rallies is not solely based on elite debate but instead the party affiliation of the elite. Without a consistent period of study, the scholars are examining different environmental factors that may only be influential at that time. This does not account for the phenomenon as a whole and therefore cannot result in a sole explanation that defines the underlying cause for the rallying effect 12 The lack of a consistent period of study can also be the cause of the inconsistent findings in the literature as without a consistent scope of research, the findings will not be able to represent and explain the phenomenon as a whole. As each investigates a different time period, the studies are analyzing different types of events and different environmental factors. The shortperiod studies draw conclusions on a very specific set of circumstances while they do not consider the long term effects of other stimuli such as if there is a divided government, the current political party of the White House, or how the economy may affect the ways in which the rallying effect is interpreted. Therefore, it becomes necessary to use a consistent temporal scope in order to account for those influences on job approval ratings. While this phenomenon has been studied with Chapman and Reiter in 2004, it did not include NATO and interpreted the variables of UNSC support and presidential approval ratings differently in contrast with this paper. Therefore this examination into the relationship between presidential approval ratings and multilateral support may be able to more fully explain the phenomenon. Our studies differ in our definition of the dependent and explanatory variables and the sample used to describe the phenomenon. Chapman and Reiter use a temporal scope of 1945 to 2001 while I instead analyze conflicts from 1949 to 2010. The dependent variable used in the Chapman and Reiter study is based on the change in public approval of the president immediately prior to and following a militarized interstate dispute and I instead use a monthly data set of presidential approval ratings which allows me to observe more adequately the true nature of the relationship between presidential approval ratings, conflict initiation, and multilateral support. Further, they measure support from the UN on a scale of -1 to 3 as -1 indicates a condemnation of force, 0 states no action, 1 is a proposed resolution that did not pass or a noncommittal resolution, 2 is a supportive resolution and 3 indicates only a resolution 13 calling for a cease-fire or establishment of peacekeeping force. In contrast, I only examine official and public declarations of support from the UNSC and NATO. I chose to use only a supportive resolution as my indicator as it is unlikely that the U.S. public would be notified of proposed but not passed resolutions and it is doubtful that if it was publicized that the public would significantly respond within the approval ratings. This paper will attempt to fill the gap in the literature by analyzing how support from the UNSC and NATO influence presidential approval ratings when the United States is involved in conflict. It contends that approval from the institutions serves as a public way to legitimize the acts of aggression and therefore may demonstrate why in some instances the spikes are larger than in others. Prior studies have determined that the rallying effect may not be presented in every instance, but are unable to propose a definitive solution to the puzzle of occasional and inconsistent spikes. In this paper, I propose that the spikes in approval ratings are due to the declarations of support from a legitimate international institution, such as the UNSC or NATO. Argument and Hypothesis: Conflict Initiation and the Rally ‘round the Flag Effect Beginning with Mueller (1970, 1973), there has been substantial literature that demonstrates that presidential approval ratings will increase following an event or crises that is international, involves the United States and the president directly, and is specific, dramatic, and sharply focused. Mueller theorized that this surge in public support stems from the ability of the president to evoke and manipulate more patriotic responses due to a number of reasons. First, only international events were capable of uniting the U.S., as domestic crises would instead exacerbate previously existing political divisions. In instances of foreign policy crises, the 14 political and social integrity of the U.S. is at stake, which then influences the public to refrain from criticism of the president, and results in a larger public backing of the president as seen in the job approval ratings. The effects of the international crises are furthered through the lack of media criticism as those who are typically more inclined to criticize the policies of the president either say nothing or support the president for the fear of appearing unpatriotic (Baker and Oneal 2001, p. 668). Subsequently, without media criticism that would otherwise influence the public to refrain from such an immediate patriotic response, the effects illustrated within presidential approval ratings are larger and more significant. This results in the rally ‘round the flag effect in which following a conflict, the president can therefore expect that there will be an increase in their job approval ratings. Much of the relevant literature supports Mueller’s findings (Lee 1977;Wittkopf and DeHaven 1987; Ostrom and Simon 1985; etc) although there is much debate on what influences the magnitude of the rallying effect. Therefore, I argue that the differentiations seen in the level of the spikes in presidential approval ratings following a conflict is due to support from such multilateral organizations the UNSC and NATO. The Effect of Multilateralism on the Rally Effect Resolutions of support from these multilateral institutions during instances of foreign conflict signal to the public that the way in which the United States government is acting is the correct form of response. Approval from these international institutions for military actions undertaken by the U.S. legitimizes and validates acts of aggression. Specifically, it can be argued that receiving approval from NATO and the UNSC is the underlying factor that accounts for variations in the magnitude of the spikes in presidential approval ratings as seen in some instances than in others. During times of interstate militarized disputes, the Americans tend to 15 monitor the president as to ensure that their actions are representative of the preferences of the public. When citizens are able to feel confident in that the president is demonstrating the best method of response to the international conflict, the approval ratings will then tend to be higher than in instances without that same conviction. The UNSC and NATO allow for the public to monitor the president’s actions, and therefore by legitimizing that the president’s actions are necessary and correct can lead to a higher spike in the approval ratings, as it would not be seen otherwise. Therefore, as a result, the rallying effect will be larger when the UNSC or NATO issues a statement of support for the president’s method of response during an instance of international dispute. Declarations from NATO or the UNSC can further account for the varying spikes in approval ratings as the president cannot always depend on support from these organizations as they are given inconsistently and can explain for the variations in the spikes seen following a conflict. Despite the fact that the government is supposed to operate in a way which coincides with the public opinion of its citizens, the support for the governments decisions on how to react during times of international disturbances are not always in the public’s best wishes. This can be explicitly seen in the current example of Syria. According to a Gallup survey, the public support for the United States taking military action against the Syrian government following its suspected use of chemical weapon on its constituents is the lowest it has been in the last 20 years. In May 2013, 68% opposed using military action in Syria while 24% supported the intervention. Following this trend, in September 2013, only 36% of Americans favored military intervention, while 51% opposed and 13% had no opinion. In contrast, in February of 2003 immediately prior to the invasion of Iraq, 59% supported that intervention, 37% opposed and 4% had no opinion (Gallup Politics). Despite the trends in the polling data which indicate that the 16 majority of the public does not support military action in Syria, President Barack Obama has publicly announced his support for the intervention and has called for a congressional vote. It is due to this possible difference in opinion that the rallying effect may not work in every instance but to explain the occasional larger spikes in approval ratings, it can be proposed that it is due to the support from an international institution that claims that the way in which the United States government is acting is the best course of action. Multilateral organizations signal that the militarized interaction is the best situation from a global standpoint. Since war is typically a societies last resort, multilateral support indicates to the U.S. public that the president’s mode of response is truly the best option, allowing for an increase in job approval ratings. Thomspon (2006) theorizes that such institutions are actors within the international community that “serve to constrain and assess the policies of potential coercing states, thereby generating political important information that can be used to screen desirable from undesirable actions” (3). This becomes especially important when the organizations are politically neutral as the institutions are less likely to share the beliefs and values of the involved states. The UNSC and NATO are not required to issue a resolution of support and there is no incentive to do so. Due to this, such a resolution of support then may allow for an increase in public support illustrated within presidential approval ratings. Domestically, this assures the American public that the President’s actions are truly legitimate, as it had been voted on by an unprejudiced group of member states that hold individual and varying interests and goals for either offering or denying support in the interstate dispute. Although the U.S. serves as a major influence in both institutions, the variety included in each of the voting procedures ensures that one state cannot overrule the opinions of other members. The UNSC has five permanent member states with veto power: China, France, Russia, 17 Great Britain and the U.S. Further, ten non-permanent member states, give of which are elected each year by the General Assembly to serve a two-year term comprise a majority of the Security Council. When voting, nine members of the council, including supporting votes from all of the five permanent member states, must be present in order to adopt a resolution. Based on the myriad of goals, interests, and values demonstrated within the UNSC that must be aligned in order to pass a resolution of support for a militarized action issued by the U.S., it demonstrates that such a resolution will be significant in terms of both global and domestic politics (United Nations Security Council 2013). The inability of the U.S. to dominate its agenda within international institutions is further demonstrated within NATO’s voting procedures: “Decisions are agreed upon on the basis of unanimity and common accord…This means that policies decided upon by the North Atlantic Council (NAC) are supported by and are the expression of the collective will of all the sovereign states that are members of the Alliance are accepted by all of them” (North Atlantic Council 2013). As both the UNSC and NATO require a general consensus from a number of different states, each holding to its own interests in the global arena, it further legitimizes the idea the resolutions of support from these institutions is significant. Such a resolution therefore indicates to the American public that the threat is real, the militarized response to the threat is the best response and that the militarized response to the threat is globally legitimate. In terms of job approval ratings for the president, support from international institutions allows for the American public to assess the danger differently. Based on the resolutions of support from these multilateral institutions, the threat is real, impending and the action taken by the president is necessary and befitting to the situation. This then translates into the public’s perception of the 18 president by increasing patriotism and decreasing criticism, thereby illustrating the rallying effect. Further on a domestic level, multilateralism and support allow for the United States to initiate the idea of burden-sharing. As Tago (2005) explains, “although acting with allies may not, in reality, substantially reduce costs for the United States, it may provide domestic audiences with the image that burdens of the operations are being shared with other nations” (588). By implementing this idea that the entire human and economic cost will not be placed solely on the American public, it defeats some of the common criticisms that wars are expensive and therefore not always in the best interest of the country. As burden-sharing inhibits economic criticisms that would elicit further cause for uncertainty for Americans when interpreting the conflict, multilateral support further allows for the public to interpret the president in a more positive light as demonstrated through an increase in presidential approval ratings. Despite the fact that some believe that the United Nations and NATO serve as an example of a hegemonic institution in the global arena, U.S. public opinion values the organizations. A recent study shows that 83% of voters state that it is important for the United States to maintain their influential role within these institutions (Mcinturff, Harrington and Garin 2012). In addition, 70% of Americans believe that the United Nations is still a necessary institution within the current global arena (Mcinturff, Harrington and Garin 2012). These statistics reject the idea that the majority of the public views the United Nations and NATO in a negative light, and justify the consideration of UNSC and NATO resolutions of support as an explanation for the variations in presidential approval rating spikes during militarized interstate disputes. Although some are critical of the UN and NATO, public opinion overall supports such 19 institutions, therefore making the organizations a viable factor to help explain the rally ‘round the flag effect. Therefore, this paper will test two hypotheses: H1: Presidential approval ratings tend to increase in years when the U.S. is involved in an interstate militarized dispute. H2: The positive effect of conflict initiation on US presidential approval ratings depends on support from the United Nations Security Council and NATO on the conflict. Holding all else constant, the presidential approval ratings will then be higher when the conflict initiated by the U.S. receives support in resolutions of approval by the UN and/or NATO. Research Design: Sample: Within this study, 12 presidents have been examined over a period of 62 years with monthly data that includes most major modern conflicts following the creation of the UNSC and NATO and was selected based on the availability of the data. This then allows for the rally ‘round the flag effect to be analyzed across multiple presidencies and conflicts, which will ensure that the results are representative of the actual phenomenon. This sample includes a total of 744 observations. Dependent Variable 20 The dependent variable, presidential approval ratings, is measured at the interval level, using data from the Gallup survey. The survey is records the percentage of respondents who approve of the current president. To obtain the approval ratings, Gallup asks the question “Do you approve or disapprove of the way that the current president is handling his job?” (Gallup). Based on the collected responses, Gallup reports the percentage of people in the sample who responded with “approve.” The usual sample size is around 1,000 adults, with a margin of error of plus or minus 4% (Gallup). The household is used as the polling unit, and is contacted through its landline phone. The caller asks to speak to the adult (18 or over) who has had the most recent birthday. The procedure used by Gallup ensures that the survey data are fully representative of the population of U.S. voters. Within the 744 observations collected, the average presidential approval rating was 54.17, with the lowest rating at 22% under Truman in February 1952 and the highest at 90% under George W. Bush in September 2001. The mean approval rating of 54% in this sample (e.g. George H.W. Bush in October of 1990 or Johnson in April of 1966). The distribution of the variable is not significantly skewed (skewness/std.error is less than 2 (-1.39). Out of 744 total observations, 40 are missing for this variable between 1949 and 1989. The majority of the data points that are not available for the purposes of this study were under the Truman administration, but some missing observations are also scattered throughout Eisenhower’s, Johnson’s, Nixon’s, Ford’s and Regan’s terms. Main Explanatory Variables 21 My argument focuses on two concepts that explore the variations in presidential approval ratings during instances of militarized interstate disputes: conflict initiation by a U.S. president, and multilateral support from the UNSC or NATO for that action. Conflict Initiation Conflict initiation was coded with a 1 indicating an initiation of conflict and a 0 otherwise. The data showed that conflict initiation was not an entirely common event, yet it is not extremely rare either. Using data that from the Correlates of War Index, which outlines each military conflict that the United States was involved in, as well as information collected from the Correlates of War Index, the mode and variation ratio were then used to interpret whether or not conflict initiation had impacted presidential approval ratings. This variable does not include or explain ongoing conflicts as that is analyzed further by the variable of conflict involvement. Therefore, among the 62 presidential years that were observed, the modal value for conflict initiation was 0 with a variation ratio of 13.7. In this time period, there were 122 conflicts initiated yet in 83.6% of this sample, no conflicts were initiated. Given the relatively small variation ratio, it can be concluded that the mode is representative of the sample. Further, it can be said that conflict initiation is not an explicitly common event in the recent history of the United States, yet on the other hand, it is obvious that it is not entirely rare either. Any Support Any support, as coded with a 0 or 1, indicated that only 2.4% of the time did the multilateral organizations offer their support in an instance of a militarized interstate dispute. With the next explanatory variable labeled any support, which observes if there was any sort of multilateral support garnered by the UNSC or NATO, it was measured nominally based off 22 information collected through the Correlates of War table and data recorded by analyzing the UN resolutions during instances when conflict was initiated by the United States. From this, the variable is defined only by if there was any support from the UNSC or NATO regarding the conflict, as it was coded with a 1 indicating endorsement or a 0 demonstrating no support. Within the 62 presidential years that were observed in this study, the mode was 0 and the variation ratio was 2.4, making the mode representative of the sample. Control Variables UNSC Support Within support from the UNSC, the measurement is based on a nominal level scale that indicates if the UNSC offered its support or not during times of conflict in which the United States was involved between the years of 1949 to 2010. The data was collected from the Correlates of War Index, which listed summaries of conflicts. From there, I examined summaries of documents taken from the UN website of each of the resolutions passed in that year in order to determine whether support was given or not. When there was a conflict, it was coded with a 1 and otherwise with a 0 to recognize no conflict, which was then measured and indicated that it is incredibly rare for the UNSC to offer its support. Since it is a nominal variable, the central tendency was measured through the mode and the variation ratio was used as to calculate dispersion. When evaluated, the data showed that the mode is 0, with a variation ratio of 2%. This indicates that as seen within the 744 observations within the sample, only 2% of the time did the U.N. offer its support for a conflict. Both the mode of 0 and the variation ratio of 2 are very representative of the sample and 23 demonstrate that within the data, it is incredibly rare for the UNSC to offer its support during an instance of foreign conflict which involves the United States. NATO Support For NATO support, I used information from the Correlates of War Index that described instances when the U.S. was involved in NATO missions or when NATO endorsed U.S. actions in a foreign conflict. NATO support was coded in the same manner as the UNSC and it also showed that NATO support is incredibly rare. It was measured nominally as 0 indicated that there was no support and 1 reflected an instance of endorsement. Within 744 observations, there were only 6 instances when NATO issued their support. The mode in this study is 0 and the variation ratio is .8%. The data then indicates that only .8% of the time did NATO offer its support for the U.S. From this very small variation ratio, it is clear that the mode is therefore representative of the sample. Conflict Involvement Conflict involvement was coded with either a 0 or a 1 and demonstrated that 82.5% of the time, the U.S. was involved in a conflict. Within the variable of conflict involvement, I nominally measured the data based off of the information retrieved from the PBS database and the Correlates of War Index that detailed the military conflicts in which the United States was involved. This variable is defined only by if the United States is involved in a conflict in that month and year. This includes ongoing conflicts, as well as militarized interstate disputes that are initiated in that month and year as well. Among the 62 presidential years that were observed, the modal value for conflict in that particular year was 1 with a variation ratio of 14.4. From this, it indicates that 82.5% of the time, the U.S. was involved in a conflict through the context of this 24 study. This variable was included for the fact that if the United States is already involved in another conflict, public fatigue with the war may reduce the rallying effect. Dispute Severity Dispute severity was measured as it was taken from the Correlates of War Index with a 3, 4, or 5 as to indicate a range from display of force to declared war. The results show that the quartile range and median of 0 were representative of the sample. Under the variable dispute data, which analyzes the severity of a conflict, the median and the quartile range were 0. When coded as a 5, this indicates that the dispute was a declared war. A 4 illustrates a use of force and a 3 describes a display of force. Out of the 744 observations, there were 69 cases of a dispute categorized as a 3, or labeled as a display of force, .47 cases demonstrated a use of force under the classification of a 4 and only 4 were marked as a declared war, or classified as a 5. Both the quartile range and median of 0 are representative of the sample. Unemployment Unemployment was used as a control variable as it allows for the effects of the economy, which can influence job approval ratings, to be separated from the effects of multilateralism. There was a high level of skewness in the data, making the median more representative of the sample. Unemployment has been collected and inputted using the data from the Bureau of Labor Statistics, is measured at the interval level for every month from January 1949 to January 2010. This control variable will allow for me to separate the effects of conflict and multilateralism from the effects of the economy, which has been identified in previous studies as an important determinant of presidential approval ratings in the U.S. The rate of unemployment is a good indicator of the state of the economy and other studies have proven that there is a negative 25 relationship between unemployment and presidential approval ratings (Norpoth and Yantek, 1983). Over the 62 presidential years and 744 observations examined in this study, the average unemployment rate is 5.74%, with a standard deviation of 1.62. The lowest rating was 2.5% in June 1953 under Eisenhower and the highest was 10.8% under Reagan in December 1983. When the skewness (.658) is divided by the standard error (.090), the results equaled 7.31, indicating that there is a high level of skewness within the data. Since there is a positive and significant skew in this variables distribution the median of 5.6% is a better representation of the central tendency. Method To analyze the data, a simple and controlled comparison of means and linear regression analysis was used. First, the comparison of means will be used to summarize the relationship between the explanatory and the dependent variables. A linear regression analysis will be used to estimate size and direction of the relationships and to make inferences from the sample about the population. Findings: Bivariate Exploratory Statistics: Comparison of Means The means comparison showed that presidential approval ratings tend to be higher when the U.S. is involved in a conflict. In Table 1, the mean of approval for years with conflict initiation is 58.75, as compared to presidential approval in years without a conflict initiation, which is 53.24. These findings provide support for my first hypothesis, as it illustrates that when conflict is initiated, the mean for the presidential approval ratings are higher. 26 When support from the UNSC and NATO were incorporated, the means of presidential approval ratings combined with multilateral support were lower than if multilateral support was given. When conflict is initiated and there is no multilateral support from the UNSC or NATO, the mean is 58.75, yet when these institutions do offer support during instances of militarized interstate disputes, the mean is only 46.00. As this indicates that the mean approval ratings for the president are lower during foreign conflict when there is support from the UNSC or NATO, this does not support my hypothesis. This finding contradicts my second hypothesis as the effect of UNSC and NATO support on presidential approval ratings when involved in a foreign conflict is in fact negative. When analyzing the severity of the dispute as compared against multilateral support, these means also do not support my second hypothesis. Dispute severity also shows that the means are higher than if there was no support from the international institutions. When the United States is involved in a level 3 conflict with no multilateral support, the mean is 57.77 while when there is support, the mean is 51. With no multilateral support and the United States is involved a conflict categorized as a 4 in terms of its hostility level, the mean is 59.17 while with support, the mean is 45.75. Based on disputes labeled as a 5, the mean is 73.00 with no support, and 44 with support. The second means comparison which includes multilateral support as it correlates to dispute severity and presidential approval ratings, seen in Table 2, demonstrates that during instances of militarized interstate disputes, presidential approval ratings will actually be lower if the UNSC or NATO displays support and therefore the means comparison also does not support my second hypothesis. Unemployment and conflict involvement have a positive relationship with presidential approval ratings. When the control variable of unemployment is factored in, it then becomes 27 clear that if there is high unemployment, only 52.95% of the American public would show support through presidential approval ratings yet if there is a time of low unemployment, 60.30% would approve, as seen in Table 3. For unemployment, as seen in Table 4, when the United States is already involved in conflict and multilateral support is given, the mean approval rating is 56.11 while when support is not granted, the mean is 54.73. Due to the fact that the mean of conflict initiation is higher than the mean of no conflict, one can see that the positive effect of foreign conflict on presidential approval ratings is consistently a variable that will continue to bolster the same results and therefore supports my first hypothesis. Even separated through periods of high and low unemployment, the means of conflict initiation are consistently higher than without conflict initiation as seen at 54.04% for low unemployment and 50.02% for high unemployment. Although this supports my first hypothesis, these results still do not support my second hypothesis. With high unemployment and conflict initiation, presidential approval ratings when combined with multilateral support is 45 while with low unemployment is 46.16, as compared with 60.30 and 52.95 for conflict initiation without multilateral support. Therefore, although support from the UNSC and NATO causes presidential approval ratings to decrease, conflict initiation regardless of high or low unemployment has a positive relationship with approval ratings. Inferential Statistics: Linear Regression The linear regression analysis demonstrates the same results as from the means comparison, therefore contradicting my second hypothesis. When conflict is initiated in that year, presidential approval ratings will increase 4.577 points and proves to be statistically significant with a p value of .001, as seen in Table 4. In contrast, the other variables analyzed, 28 unemployment, dispute severity, and conflict involvement, are not statistically significant according to the linear regression analysis. Based on the observations from the means comparison and the linear regression analysis, I cannot reject the null hypothesis and therefore cannot prove that support from international institutions serves as the underlying factor in the variations in approval rating spikes. Instead of indicating that UNSC or NATO support during instances of foreign conflict does increase presidential approval ratings, the results indicate the opposite, that there in fact is a negative correlation. The means comparison showed that unemployment and conflict initiation were related in terms of presidential approval ratings and therefore should be analyzed at a different time to create a definitive explanation for the rally ‘round the flag effect. Conclusion: In an attempt to explain for the varying levels of spikes in presidential approval ratings following a militarized interstate dispute, this paper analyzed the effect from the UNSC and NATO on such ratings. Such international institutions serve as a legitimate and outside source that could validate to the American public that the president is acting appropriately. Further, resolutions from multilateral organizations signal that the threat is real and impending, the militarized response is legitimate on a domestic and international level, and that the United States may not be forced to pay the human and economic cost of foreign conflicts alone. I therefore argued that the differences seen in the magnitude of spikes in presidential approval ratings following an international conflict were due to resolutions of support from the UNSC and NATO. 29 I proposed and tested two hypotheses. The first one simply establishes the rally effect, the second explores the effect of multilateral support on rally spikes H1: Presidential approval ratings tend to increase in years when the U.S. is involved in an interstate militarized dispute. H2: The positive effect of conflict initiation on US presidential approval ratings depends on support from the United Nations Security Council and NATO on the conflict. Holding all else constant, the presidential approval ratings will then be higher when the conflict initiated by the U.S. receives support in resolutions of approval by the UN and/or NATO. Using a sample of 744 observations, which examined 12 presidents over a period of 62 years from 1949 to 2010, I tested these hypotheses through a means comparison and linear regression analysis. Both the means comparison and the linear regression analysis demonstrated that conflict initiation has both a positive and statistically significant relationship on presidential approval ratings, thus supporting the rally effect hypothesis. In contrast, the means comparison and linear regression analysis did not allow me to reject the null for my second hypothesis. The means comparison instead illustrated that support from the UNSC and NATO has a small negative effect on presidential approval ratings following conflict initiation; the linear regression analysis also estimated a negative coefficient for multilateral support, but showed no statistical significance for this effect. The inability to reject the null concerning my second hypothesis may be due to the fact that creating consensus among sovereign nations, each of whom is focused on differing interests, goals and values, is a slower process. Therefore, if the UNSC or NATO does not respond 30 immediately by issuing a resolution of support, then it cannot be included in analyzing changes in presidential approval ratings directly following the initiation of a militarized interstate dispute. As most of my sample includes resolutions that were issued while the United States was already involved in conflict and not at the direct initiation, the lack of data corresponding to only conflict initiation makes it difficult to efficiently assess the relationship between presidential approval ratings and multilateral support. Therefore, as the UNSC and NATO do not always issue supportive resolutions directly following conflict initiation, further research may prove that instead support from multilateral organizations may have a larger impact when the United States is already involved in a conflict. By analyzing the relationship between multilateral organizations and presidential approval ratings when the conflict is already ongoing, this may be a more accurate analysis due to the often lagging nature of these resolutions and could still explain the relationships between presidential approval ratings and multilateral support. Another interesting avenue for future research is to explore if and how presidents use multilateral support in their public discourse on the conflict. 31 Appendix Table 1 – Comparison of approval ratings across instances of no conflict and conflict initiation Report presidential approval ratings conflict initiation Mean N Std. Deviation no conflict initiation 53.4201 588 13.23753 conflict initiation 57.9828 116 12.70740 Total 54.1719 704 13.25149 Table 2 - Comparison of approval ratings across instances of no conflict and conflict initiation controlling for multilateral support Report presidential approval ratings conflict initiation no conflict initiation conflict initiation Total any support Mean N Std. Deviation no multilateral support 53.2461 577 13.07438 multilateral support form UN 62.5455 11 18.61915 Total 53.4201 588 13.23753 no multilateral support 58.7523 109 12.58391 multilateral support form UN 46.0000 7 8.14453 Total 57.9828 116 12.70740 no multilateral support 54.1210 686 13.14395 multilateral support form UN 56.1111 18 17.21111 54.1719 704 13.25149 or NATO or NATO or NATO Total 32 Table 3 – Comparison of approval ratings across instances of no conflict and conflict initiation controlling for unemployment Report presidential approval ratings conflict initiation unemployment rate by year Mean N Std. Deviation (Binned) no conflict initiation conflict initiation Total Low 54.3213 470 13.88411 High 49.8305 118 9.49611 Total 53.4201 588 13.23753 Low 59.3804 92 12.59761 High 52.6250 24 11.90474 Total 57.9828 116 12.70740 Low 55.1495 562 13.79888 High 50.3028 142 9.95236 Total 54.1719 704 13.25149 Table 4 – Comparison of approval ratings across instances of no conflict or conflict involvement controlling for multilateral support Report presidential approval ratings conflict involvement any support Mean N Std. Deviation no multilateral support 51.1207 116 12.29574 Total 51.1207 116 12.29574 no multilateral support 54.7316 570 13.23705 multilateral support form UN 56.1111 18 17.21111 Total 54.7738 588 13.35971 no multilateral support 54.1210 686 13.14395 multilateral support form UN 56.1111 18 17.21111 54.1719 704 13.25149 no conflict involvement conflict involvement Total or NATO or NATO Total 33 Table 6 – Effect of conflict initiation and multilateral support on presidential approval ratings Explanatory Variable Model 1 Model 2 Conflict Initiation 4.805*** 5.651 Sig .000 .358 3.236 2.882 Sig .332 .398 -.591* -.502 .054 .105 -17.719* -16.671 .074 .104 Multilateral Support Unemployment Conflictin*anysupp Conflict Involvement 2.447* .076 Dispute Severity -.396 .824 Constant 56.748 54.283 N 744 744 R2 0.026 0.03 Note: *p,<0.1, **p<0.05, ***p<0.01 34 References Abramson, P.R., John H. Aldrich, and David. W. Rohde. 1987. Change and Continuity in the 1984 elections. Washington, DC: Congressional Quarterly. Baker, William and John R. Oneal. 2001. “The Nature and Origins of the Rally Round the Flag Effect.” The Journal of Conflict Resolution (October) : 661-687. Baum, Matthew A. 2002. “The Constituent Foundations of the Rally-Round-the-Flag Phenomenon.” International Studies Quarterly 46 (2): 263-98. "Bureau of Labor Statistics Data." United States Department of Labor. N.p.. Web. 23 Apr 2013. <http://data.bls.gov/pdq/SurveyOutputServlet>. Callaghan, Karen and Simo Virtanen. 1993. “Revised Models of the Rally Phenomenon: The Case of the Carter Presidency.” The Journal of Politics (August) : 756-764 Chapman, Terrence, and Dan Reiter. 2004. “The United Nations Security Council and the Rally ‘Round the Flag Effect.” The Journal of Conflict Resolution (December) : 886-909 Cover, Albert D. 1986. “Presidential Evaluations and Voting for Congress.” American Journal of Political Science 30 (4): 786-801. Eichenberg, Richard C., Richard J. Stoll and Matthew Lebo. 2006. “War President: The Approval Ratings of George W. Bush.” The Journal of Conflict Resolution (December) : 783-808. Fiorinia, Morris. 1981. Retrospective voting in American national elections. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press. 35 Gallup Politics. 2013. “U.S. Support for Action in Syria is Low vs. Past Conflicts.” September 6. http://www.gallup.com/poll/164282/support-syria-action-lower-past-conflicts.aspx. (September 22, 2013). Gartner, Scott and Gary M. Segura. 1998. “Opening Up the Black Box of War: Politics and the Conduct of War.” The Journal of Conflict Resolution (June) : 278-300. Groeling, Tim and Matthew A. Baum. 2012. “Crossing the Water’s Edge: Elite Rhetoric, Media Coverage, and the Rally-Round-The-Flag Phenomenon.” Journal of Politics (September) : 1065 – 1085. Gronke, Paul, Jeffery Koch and J. Matthew Wilson. 1998. “Follow the Leader? Presidential Approval, Perceived Presidential Support, and the Representatives’ Electoral Fortunes.” Journal of Politics. (October) 104-37. Kernell, Samuel. 1978. Explaining presidential popularity. American Political Science Review 72:506-22. Lee, John R. 1977. Rallying ‘round the flag. Presidential Studies Quarterly 7:252-56. Lian, Bradley and John Oneal. 1993. “Presidents, the Use of Military Force and Public Opinion.” The Journal of Conflict Resolution. (June) : 277-300. Marra, Robin F., Charles W. Ostrom Jr. 1989. Explaining seat change in the U.S. House of Representatives, 1950-86. American Journal of Political Science 27: 165-92. 36 Mcinturff, Bill, Elizabeth Harrington and Geoff Garin. 2012. “Index of Public Opinion on International Issues and the United Nations.” Public Opinion Strategies. (April) "Militarized Interstate Disputes (v3.10)." Correlates of War. N.p.. Web. 23 Apr 2013. <http://www.correlatesofwar.org>. Morgan T. Clifton, and Kenneth N. Bickers. 1992. Domestic discontent and the external use of force. Journal of Conflict Resoltuon. 36:25-52. Norpoth, Helmut and Thom Yantek. 1983. “Macroeconomic Conditions and Fluctuations of Presidential Popularity: The Question of Lagged Effects.” American Journal of Political Science (November) : 785-807. North Atlantic Treaty Organization. “Consensus decision-making at NATO”. http://www.nato.int/cps/ar/natolive/topics_49178.htm. (September 30, 2013). Oneal, John and Anna Lillian Bryan. 1995. “The Rally Round the Flag Effect in U.S. Foreign Policy Crisis, 1950-1985.” Political Behavior (December) : 379-401. Ostrom, Charles W. Jr., and Dennis M. Simon. 1987. P”romise and performance: A dynamic model of presidential popularity”. American Political Science Review 79: 334-58. Schubert, James N., Patrick A. Stewart and Margaret Ann Curran. 2002. “A Defining Presidential Moment: 9/11 and the Rally Effect.” Political Psychology. 23: 559-83 Stueck, William. 2010. The Korean War in World History. Lexington.: The University Press of Kentucky. http://ehis.ebscohost.com/eds/detail?vid=2&sid=a0bb7ea9-103f-4457-bd90- 37 abe45ee1a640%40sessionmgr112&hid=105&bdata=JnNpdGU9ZWRzLWxpdmU%3d#d b=nlebk&AN=413895 (July 19, 2013) Tago, Atsushi. 2005. “Determinants of Multilateralism in US Use of Force: State of Economy, Election Cycle, and Divided Government.” Journal of Peace Research. 42 (5): 585-604. Thomspon, Alexander. 2006. “Coercion through IOs: The Security Council and the logic of information transmission.” International Organization 60 (1): 1-34. United Nations Security Council. “Security Council.” http://www.un.org/en/sc/ (September 30, 2013). Witkopf, Eugene R., and Mark J. DeHaven. 1987. “Soviet behavior, presidential popularity, and the penetration of open political systems”. In New directions in the study of foreign policy, edited by Charles F. Hermann, Charles W. Kegley Jr., and James N Rosenau. Boston: Unwin Hyman. 2012. “How Does Gallup Polling Work?” Gallup Public Opinion Polling. 2010. “How Are Polls Conducted?” Gallup Polling Data.