Disclosure Piece Attitudes Toward Online Social Connection and Self-Disclosure as Predictors of Facebook Communication and Relational Closeness The recent widespread adoption of social network sites influences communication behaviour in a variety of contexts. Though users appropriate these sites for varied purposes, the maintenance of networked interpersonal relationships is their central attraction and function. The recent emergence of social network sites (SNSs), or “web-based” services that allow individuals to (1) construct a public or semi-public profile within a bounded system, (2) articulate a list of other users with whom they share a connection, and (3) view and traverse their list of connections and those made by others within the system” Conceptualize self-disclosure as “any message about the self that a person communicates to another.” - several features of Facebook that foster self-disclosure: “users post personal information such as pictures, hobbies, and messages to communicate with fellow students and instructors as well as friends and family.” Wright and his colleagues further validate the importance of self-disclosure behaviour via Facebook, finding that breadth and depth of self-disclosure is associated with increased interdependence and predictability. Thus, as Facebook’s own slogan claims, the site is indeed a location where users share information about the self with a proscribed set of others. Communication researchers have long recognized the role of self-disclosure in healthy relational development. Evidence from other studies of online communication suggest that generalized attraction to OSD may be associated with negative psychological and relational outcomes. Online communication scholars have long considered the antecedents and outcomes of identity formation and selfpresentation enacted via online disclosure with several studies reporting that communicators often self-disclose more online than they do when face to face. Lack of social competence may account for heightened self-disclosure online because those with poor social skills may prefer the greater control over communication behaviour that online contexts afford. Disentangling the Effects of Depression Symptoms and Adult Attachment on Emotional Disclosure Sharing information about one’s emotional experiences, referred to as emotional disclosure, is associated with a number of positive outcomes. Emotional disclosure is related to increased immune system functioning, decreased pain and medication use, and fewer doctor visits (Pennebaker & Seagal, 1999). Disclosing an emotional event also leads to decreased levels of depression, anxiety, distress, and anger (Frattaroli, 2006); a decrease in the emotional intensity of the event (Zech & Rime, 2005); and a reduction in intrusive thoughts about the event. In an opposite manner, the active concealment of distressing information is associated with psychological distress and physical symptoms such as headaches and backaches (Kelly & Yip, 2006; Larson & Chastain, 1990). Emotional disclosure appears to be part of the process of emotion regulation (Kahn & Garrison, 2009; Kahn, Hucke, Bradley, Glinski, & Malak, in press). In particular, Kahn and colleagues’ findings indicate that emotional disclosure is closely related to the emotion regulation strategy of suppression. According to Gross and John (2003), suppression is a common strategy individuals use to decrease negative affect following an emotional episode. Kahn et al. proposed that the high negative correlation between emotional disclosure and suppression may signify that, like suppression, the goal of emotional disclosure is to decrease negative emotions. Individuals who are high in attachment anxiety tend to have a strong desire to be overly close to others and fear abandonment. Individuals who are high in attachment avoidance tend to have a difficult time trusting others in relationships and are often overly self-reliant. Attachment theory suggests that the primary means by which individuals attempt to regulate their emotions is to seek out attachment figures (Bowlby, 1988). Individuals who are high in attachment avoidance or anxiety, however, have learned that they cannot always rely on their primary caregivers in this way. Specifically, avoidantly attached individuals generally have had caregivers who were unavailable when they were distressed, whereas anxiously attached individuals have generally experienced inconsistent care giving during times of distress. Due to these early experiences, these individuals tend to engage in two types of secondary attachment strategies in an attempt to regulate their emotions when they become distressed. Individuals who are high in attachment avoidance tend to engage in deactivating strategies. The primary aim of these strategies is to eliminate the experience of emotional distress. Repeatedly, these individuals have experienced the unavailability of their attachment figures and have learned that they cannot depend on relationship partners when they are distressed. As a result, these individuals tend to rely on themselves, and they tend not to go to others for support when they become emotionally distressed. Moreover, these individuals tend to suppress experiences, thoughts, and emotions that are emotionally distressing. By doing their best to avoid ully experiencing emotional distress, avoidantly attached individuals are attempting to reduce the likelihood that they will experience an emotionally distressing eperience that is too much for them to handle alone. Conversely, individuals who are high in attachment anxiety tend to engage in hyperactivating strategies. The primary aim of these strategies is to gain proximity to their attachment partners, as these individuals have come to expect that people they have relationships with will inconsistently be responsive to their needs. Specifically, they tend to demand the attention of others, experience exaggerated subjective negative emotional reactions to objectively minor threats, and ruminate over negative emotions. Furthermore, anxiously attached individuals tend to believe they are not able to regulate their own emotions and depend heavily on others in this area. Clearly, both avoidantly and anxiously attached individuals use maladaptive affect regulation strategies. Moreover, the secondary strategies are evident in the ways these individuals approach the use of emotional disclosure. Overall, research suggests that avoidantly attached individuals engage in low levels of emotional disclosure (Anders & Tucker, 2000; …) Let’s talk about us: Attachment, relationship-focused disclosure and relationship quality Very little attention has been paid to the impact of attachment insecurity in nonthreatening situations, despite the fact that behaviours enacted during these more routine relationship interactions such also influence relationship quality. For example, there is good evidence that intimates’ disclosure during everyday discussion plays an important role in relationship maintenance. When people disclose personal information to their partners during their daily interactions, and partners are responsive to these disclosures, this enhances trust, intimacy, and felt acceptance. Attachment avoidance: Resisting intimacy and disclosure Attachment avoidance is characterized by discomfort with intimacy, defensive selfreliance, and a strong motivation to maintain independence. Accordingly, people high in attachment avoidance employ numerous tactics to maintain psychological distance from their partners. Greater attachment avoidance is associated with a more disinterested and cold communication style during observed discussions with their partners and lower self-reported disclosure and disclosure intimacy. Importance of disclosure: disclosure to one’s partner communicates trust and regard and partner responsiveness to disclosure fosters understanding, care, and validation. Thus, self-disclosure maintains relationships by increasing closeness and signaling that partners are valued. Accordingly, research has consistently shown that disclosure and receipt of disclosure increases liking and intimacy and is associated with higher concurrent relationship. The restriction of disclosure by people high in attachment avoidance should inhibit intimacy, and be the one reason why attachment avoidance is associated with poorer relationship outcomes. Indeed, self-reported disclosure has been found to mediate the association between greater attachment avoidance. Attachment anxiety: Desiring intimacy and disclosure Individuals who are high in attachment anxiety are highly invested in their relationships, desire strong connections with their partner, and are preoccupied with maintaining and enhancing intimacy. They are chronically in intimacy-seeking mode, continually trying to achieve acceptance and emotional closeness with their partners. Ironically, this hypervigilance tends to produce destructive reactions within threatening situations. For example, because highly anxious individuals are concerned about acceptance, they become more distressed and hurt during them to behave with greater hostility and punishing behaviours. In the absence of threat, people high in anxiety should be especially motivated to engage in intimacy-generating disclosures, including greater relationship-focused disclosure, and do some more than those low in anxiety. Studies using a categorical approach to assess attachment have tended to find a general pattern of anxiously attached individuals disclosing a) more than avoidantly attached individuals but b) approximately the same as securely attached individuals although with a greater proclivity toward disclosing indiscriminately and excessively to others. Handbook of Attachment A core issue in attachment theory (Bowlby, 1969/1982) is the involvement of the attachment system in regulating negative emotions provoked by the appraisal of threats and dangers. Bowlby noted that reactions to such provocations include not only the classically emphasized “fight or flight” response, but also seeking proximity to a “stronger and wiser,” supportive, and protective attachment figure. Main called this the primary attachment strategy. In adulthood people can seek proximity to and support from an attachment figure either by requesting actual support from a real, physically present relationship partner, or by calling upon mental images, prototypes, schemas, or specific memories of interactions with real or imagined attachment figures. Two stages by which threat appraisals lead to activation of the primary attachment strategy. In the first stage, threat appraisals trigger preconscious activation of the system, which brings about an automatic increase in the mental accessibility or attachment-related representations in an associative memory network. In the second stage, this preconscious activation, if sufficiently robust, results in conscious thoughts about seeking proximity to attachment figures, behavioural intentions to seek proximity and support, and actual seeking of proximity and support. In the case of relatively secure adults, threat appraisals arouse thoughts and feelings related to positive interactions with attachment figures. In the case of relatively insecure adults, however, threat appraisals can bring to mind – either consciously or unconsciously – negative, painful, dispiriting attachment-related experiences. Insecure people have learned through many painful experiences with unavailable or unresponsive attachment figures that the primary attachment strategy (proximity seeking) often fails to accomplish its emotion regulation goal, making it necessary to consider alternative secondary strategies: hyperactivation or deactivation if the attachment system. Secondary (insecure) attachment strategies distort and damage emotion regulation, and thereby contribute to psychological and social problems. They include psychological defenses against the frustration and pain caused by attachment figures’ unavailability. In order to sustain these strategies, a person has to build otherwise distorted or constraining working models and adopt nonoptimal affect regulation strategies, which are likely to interfere with subsequent development and hamper attempts to create rewarding close relationships. Avoidant people attempt to block or inhibit emotional states associated with threatrelated thoughts because these thoughts can activate unwanted attachment-related needs, memories, and behaviours. Like secure people, avoidant ones attempt to down-regulate threat-related emotions. However, whereas secure people’s regulatory attempts usually promote communication, compromise and relationship maintenance, avoidant people’s efforts are aimed mainly at keeping the attachment system deactivated, regardless of the deleterious effects this can have on a relationship. Attachment anxious people are guide by an unfulfilled wish to cause attachment figures to pay more attention to them and provide more reliable protection. One way to attain this goal is to keep the attachment system chronically activated (state of hyperactivation) and intensify bids for attention until a satisfying sense of attachment security is attained. Chronically attachment-anxious tend to exaggerate the presence and seriousness of threats and to remain vigilant regarding the possible unavailability of attachment figures. They also tend to overemphasize their sense of helplessness and vulnerability, because signs of weakness and vulnerability can sometimes elicit other people’s attention and care. Attachment Styles and Patterns of Self-Disclosure Self-disclosure refers to the process by which persons let themselves be known to others. Derlega and Grzelak (1979) define self-disclosure as including “any information exchange that refers to the self, including personal states, dispositions, events in the past, and plans for the future. Research on self-disclosure has found that the ability to reveal one’s feelings and thoughts to another is a basic skill for developing close relationships. Self-disclosure has been found to facilitate the development of caring and mutual understandings. Lack of self-disclosure has often been related to dissatisfaction with one’s social network and feelings of loneliness. Individual variations in self-disclosure can be variously manifested in the amount, intimacy level, and content of disclosed information and in the target of the selfdisclosure. Another basic dimension of self-disclosure is its flexibility which reflects the ability to adequately attend to situational cues and adapt to one’s disclosing behavior accordingly. The individual who is able to modulate his or her disclosures across a wider range of social situations in response to situational and interpersonal demands will function interpersonally more adequately than the less flexible indivudal who has not learned the discriminant cues that signal whether disclosure is appropriate or inappropriate.